Title: SYNTAX
1SYNTAX
2- Syntax is the component of grammar that deals
with sentence structure. - Before we can talk about syntax, we need to
discuss the nature of linguistic competence and
the nature of grammar
3The nature of grammar
- Linguistic competence
- Theoretically permits infinite production of
novel utterances, including sentences. -
- How can our brains handle a system capable of
infinite production? - Answer The elements of the system are not
infinite. They are finite in number.
4Grammar
- A system that permits infinite creativity with a
relatively limited number of elements, including
speech sounds and the rules for combining them
into words and sentences. - The rules for making sentences are called the
syntax of the language. - What are these rules like and how are they stored
as part of competence?
5The Rules of Syntax
- Does knowing the rules of syntax involve storing
the syntactic structures of all possible
sentences in our heads as a set of templates with
slots to fill? -
- No, it does not.
-
- Here are for reasons why not
6Reason 1
- 1. Rules of syntax allow sentences to be
infinitely long. (The only limits are imposed by
performance.)
7Reason 2
- 2. All possible sentences have not yet been
uttered.
8Reason 3
- 3. Our storage capacity is finite.
9Reason 4
- 4. Sentence interpretation is structure
dependent. - That is, it relies on more than a simple linear
(one word after another) organization. - (This will be explained further.)
10The Nature of Syntax
- The following slides present a number of
illustrations of the nature of syntax and facts
about language that a linguistic theory of syntax
must account for.
11Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
- 1. How words in a sentence are assigned specific
roles with respect to one another. - For example, in English active sentences
- The first noun is the subject or doer of the
action or agent. - The second noun is the object or receiver (or
theme or patient) of the action.
12Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
- Thus different interpretations for
-
- The dog chased the cat.
- subject verb object
-
- The cat chased the dog.
- subject verb object
13Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
- Word order varies across languages.
- In German, its subject, object, verb
- Das Kind wird die Schwester lehren.
- The child will the sister teach.
- subject object verb
-
14Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
- For some languages, like Welsh, the preferred
order is verb, subject, object. - For example
- darllenais i y llyfr
- read I the book
- verb subject object
15Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
- Other languages may rely more on some form of
morphological marking than on word ordering to
assign roles to words in sentences. - However, these languages still have a most common
or canonical word order. - The other permissible word orders tend to
slightly alter the focus of the sentence. - See Japanese example on the next slide.
16Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
- Canonical word order in Japanese
- Yumiko scolded the child
- Yumiko-ga sono kodomo-o sikat-ta
- yumiko-NOM that child-ACC scold-past
- subject object verb
- Alternative order (focus on object)
- Sono kodomo-o Yumiko-ga sikat-ta
- that child-ACC yumiko-NOM scold-past
- object subject verb
17Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
- The most typical word order in a language is
called its canonical word order. - 95 of the worlds languages have one of the
following canonical word orders - SVO Canadians like hockey. (English)
- SOV Canadians hockey like. (German)
- VSO Like Canadians hockey. (Welsh)
- VOS OVS OSV rare or nonexistent
18Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
- Sentences with different surface structures and
word orders can have the same interpretation of
whos doing what to whom, even in a language with
strict word order and no specific marking of
subjects and objects. -
- Examine the following sentence pairs from
English
19Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
- scrambled order
- The boy gave the toy to the girl.
- direct indirect
- object object
- The boy gave the girl the toy.
- indirect direct
- object object
20Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
- active
- The student solved the problem.
- (agent/subject) (theme/object)
- passive
- The problem was solved by the student.
- (theme/object) (agent/subject)
21Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
- declarative
- Everyone is happy.
- (verb)
- yes/no question
- Is everyone happy?
- (verb)
22Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
- declarative
- You have been there.
- auxiliary
- yes/no question
- Have you been there?
- auxiliary
23Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 3
- 3.Sentences contain discontinuous elements which
are understood as part of the same structural
constituent, as in - perfect (auxiliary have suffix -en)
- I have eaten.
- progressive (auxiliary be suffix -ing)
- I am eating.
24Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 3
- Wh question
- What are they looking at?
- Object
- Declarative
- They are looking at the dog.
- object
- NB Although what has moved to the front of the
question sentence, it plays the same syntactic
role as the dog does in the declarative. Both
are part of the predicate.
25Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
- 4. It is possible for sentences to be embedded
inside other sentences. - The property of grammar which permits such
embedding is called recursiveness. -
26Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
- Recursiveness also allows sentence constituents
(parts) to occur inside other constituents. See
the string of adjectives for the subject noun
dog in the following example -
- The big gray shaggy friendly dog brought me the
newspaper. -
27Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
- In the following examples, note how a sentence
can grow as more strings of adjectives,
prepositional phrases and embedded sentences are
added - The dog was sleeping.
- subject
- The big dog was sleeping.
- subject dog one adjective
-
28Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
- The big ugly guard dog at the factory was
sleeping. - Subject dog string of adjectives
prepositional phrase
29Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
- The big ugly guard dog at the factory which
produces pillows that some people are allergic to
was sleeping on the road that leads to the quarry
where they found the hobo who had been strangled
with a shoelace that was later shown to have been
stolen from the woman on Main Street whose house
was broken into last week. -
- subject dog string of adjectives
prepositional phrases embedded sentences
30Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 5
- 5. Sentences with the same apparent structural
elements and word orders can have different
meanings or interpretations. - John is easy to please
- John is eager to please
- Subject verb adjective infinitive
-
- On the surface, the structures of sentences 1
and 2 appear to be the same.
31Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 5
- John is easy to please
- John is eager to please
- In sentence 1, John is unspoken/understood
object of to please
subject ? (somebody - In sentence 2, John is unspoken/understood,)
- subject of to please
object ? (somebody)
32Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 6
- 6. A single sentence may have more than one
meaning or interpretation. - This is called ambiguity.
- Structural ambiguity
- The two meanings hinge on different underlying
relationships between the words in the sentence. - Example
- Visiting professors can be interesting.
33Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 6
- The structural ambiguity of the sentence rests
on the ambiguous structure of the phrase visiting
professors. - Visiting professors can be interesting.
- modifier noun
- Meaning Professors who have come here
temporarily from other universities. - or
- Visiting professors can be interesting.
- gerund noun
- Meaning For someone to visit professors.
34Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 6
- Lexical ambiguity
- Alternative interpretations of a sentence rest
on different meanings of homophonous lexical
items. - Example
- She ate her cottage cheese with relish.
-
- relish condiment or enthusiasm.
- The two words sound identical, but have
different meanings.
35Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 7
- Not all elements of a sentence appear on the
surface, yet listeners can correctly interpret
the sentence. - For example
- Joe likes pizza and Jack does too.
- missing element likes pizza
- Both like pizza, but likes pizza appears only
once.
36Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 7
- Pronouns sometimes stand in for missing
elements - Mary bought some ice cream and __ ate it.
- Mary bought some ice cream and she ate it.
- If Mary and she stand for the same
individual, they are co-referenced. - It is also possible that she is co-referenced
with someone else.
37Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 8
- 8.There are quite strict rules for forming
sentences which speakers must adhere to. - Syntactically ill-formed sentences are
ungrammatical. - Eats the food the child.
-
- Mary refused to allow that the children go to
the concert. - ?Mary refused to allow that type of language.
-
38Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 9
- 9. Syntactic and semantic well-formedness are
independent of one another. - The following sentences are syntactically
well-formed but semantically anomalous or
nonsensical - Colourless green ideas sleep furiously.
- A verb crumpled the milk.
39Transformational/Generative Approach
- To account for these and other facts, most
linguists adopt a transformational generative
(TG) approach to describing the syntactic
component of grammar. - A TG approach emphasizes the search for Universal
Grammar (UG).
40Transformational/Generative Approach
- UG presupposes that languages operate under a
shared set of categories, operations and
principles. - Although languages obviously differ from one
another, their are common principles governing
the way they form sentences. - These properties of UG are called language
universals.
41Transformational/Generative Approach
- By using the same descriptive approach for all
languages, linguists hope to identify language
universals and gain a more complete understanding
of UG. - At the syntactic level, there are clearly two
subcomponents found in all languages - 1. a lexicon
- 2. a computational system
42Transformational/Generative Approach
- The lexicon or mental dictionary lists the words
(and morphemes) in a language. - The lexicon also includes information about each
entry regarding - -pronunciation
- -meaning
- -form (root, affix, bound, free, etc.)
- -syntactic category information? (noun, verb,
etc.)
43Transformational/Generative Approach
- The computational system includes the operations
that allow words to be combined into syntactic
structures. - The computational system has two major
components - Merge
- Move
44Transformational/Generative Approach
- Merge allows the creation of phrases (parts of
sentences) and the combining of the these phrases
into sentences. - Move allows certain elements to be transported to
a new position within a sentence. - An example of a moved element is the word what
moving out of object position into sentence
initial position in - What are they looking at?
45Transformational/Generative Approach
- We will examine the elements of syntax as
follows - 1. The lexicon and the notion of syntactic
categories - 2. The creation of phrases (phrase structure)
- 3. The merge operation to form sentences
- 4. Types of phrasal clausal complements
- 5. Move
46Transformational/Generative Approach
- Transformational Generative theory has undergone
many reformulations over the past 50 years. - We are operating with a recent version.
- Thus you may encounter terms elsewhere that are
roughly equivalent to the elements of syntax that
we are examining. - We will discuss these terms at a later date.
47The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
- There is some controversy in current linguistic
theory regarding whether the syntactic category
(noun, verb, etc.) of a word is stored or
syntactically determined. There are essentially
two positions. - 1. This information is stored in the lexicon.
- 2. The syntactic category is only determined when
the word enters into a syntactic structure. - We will not resolve the issue in this class. We
will assume position 1, although the reasoning
behind position 2 may be discussed.
48The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
- All languages group their words into syntactic
categories. - We find remarkably similar syntactic categories
across languages - Your textbook provides example of the commonly
found categories in Table 5.1.1 on page 133.
49(No Transcript)
50The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
- The Lexical/Nonlexical distinction is roughly
equivalent to the distinction we used in
morphology - OpenClass/Content or Lexical Words
- versus
- ClosedClass/Function Words
- or Non-lexical
- The chart is reproduced in the next slide
51Non-lexical/ functional
Lexical
Non-lexical/functional
(Closed Class)
52The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
- The classification of words into syntactic
categories can rely on - the type of meaning they express
- what inflectional affixes they take
- the types of structures they fit into
(distribution) - Classification relies on a combination of these
approaches. -
53Classifying Syntactic Categories
- We will apply these 3 types of classification in
the following slides. - This will hopefully allow you to gain a better
understanding of Syntactic Categories (aka parts
of speech)
54Classifying Syntactic CategoriesMeaning
- The meanings of Lexical (Open Class/Content)
words are generally much easier to define that
those of Non-lexical (Closed Class/Function)
words. - Consider the words
- the (Non-lexical/Closed Class - determiner)
and - brush (Lexcial/Open Class - noun)
55the versus brush
- Its easier to explain the function of the, or
where it occurs (before nouns), than what it
actually means. - Its easier to describe the concept brush than
the concept the.
56Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
- Nouns name entities objects (book)
- people (Mary)
- Verbs designate actions (eat)
- sensations (feel)
- states (be, seem)
- Adjectives designate properties and attributes
of what nouns name (heavy book) - Adverbs Denote properties and attributes of
what verbs designate (eat quickly) - Adverbs tell us how, why, where, when.
57Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
- Meaning is not always a clear-cut way of deciding
the part of speech of word. - Some lexical/content words are more difficult to
define. - For example, the concepts truth or honesty do
not name entities.
58Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
- Items from two different categories can have
similar meanings. - For example, the verb hate and the noun
revulsion have very similar meanings - I hate carrots.
- I feel revulsion toward carrots.
59Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
- Some words, like brush, can be a noun or a
verb. - I bought a new brush (noun) for the dog.
- I brush (verb) the dog every day.
60Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- So we still need to go beyond simple meaning to
define a lexical (content) words syntactic
category. - We can also look at what inflections a form
takes, as inflections usually attach to a
particular part of speech. - Note the references to parts of speech in the
following list of English inflections.
61The inflectional affixes of English
62Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Thus we can tell if ship is a noun or a verb
by seeing whether it takes affixes that normally
attach to a verb or a noun. - ship pl. s as in
- I like to sail on ships.
- ship poss. s as in
- The ships hull is painted red.
- So far, it looks as if ship is a noun.
63Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- To be certain, lets see if adjectival inflections
can be attached to ship. - Comparative er as in
- That one is shipper than the first one.
- Superlative est as in
- That is shippest one of all.
- So ship is not an adjective.
64Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- What about verbal inflections?
- Past tense ed as in
- They shipped the package last week.
- Progressive ing as in
- They are shipping the package by air.
- So, it looks as if ship is a verb as well as a
noun.
65Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- In fact, many words can belong to more than one
lexical category. - For example
- brush (noun, verb)
- comb (noun, verb)
- near (preposition, verb, adjective)
- They got bored near the end.
- They neared the finish line.
- The are nearer to the end than us.
66Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- We have seen that neither the meaning nor the
affix test tells us reliably which lexical
category a word belongs to. - Another more reliable way of determining lexical
category is by looking at a words distribution.
67Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- By distribution, we mean the type of elements
that a word can co-occur with. - The most useful type of elements for
distributional analysis are what functional
categories a word can be used with, although we
can also look at what lexical categories it can
be used with. -
68Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- For example
- nouns occur with determiners such as the
- the dog
- verbs occur with auxiliaries such as will
- will go
- adjectives occur with degree words such as
very - very hot
69Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Verbs do not occur with determiners
- the go
- Nouns do not occur with auxiliaries such as
will - will lamp
- Neither nouns nor verbs occur with degree words
such as very - very lamp very go
70Homework
- Study Guide Page 104
- OA Page 170-171, Ex. 2
- Think about how you could have used
distribution, meaning and inflection tests in
order to identify the syntactic categories (parts
of speech) of the underlined words and words in
the sentences of Ex. 2. - Refer to Tables 5.1, 5.2 and 5.3 in OA for
help.
71Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Distributional analysis helps us to see the
difference between verbs and auxiliaries. - A verb can co-occur with an auxiliary, but a verb
cannot co-occur with another verb. - We may eat.
- We have eaten.
- We are eating.
- We play eat.
72Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Sentences normally have only one verb.
- So a sentence with more than one verb is really
more than one sentence. - Such as two conjoined sentences as in
- Theyre moving and grooving.
- From Theyre moving. Theyre grooving.
73Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Or, two verbs might be a clue that there is a
main sentence containing an embedded or
subordinate sentence or infinitival complement as
in - We are going to play.
- go is the main verb and play is the verb in
the infinitival complement to play.
74Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Further distributional analysis can show us that
there are actually three types of auxiliary in
English - Modals (will, would, shall, should, can, could,
must, may, might) - Perfect (have en)
- Progressive (be-ing)
75Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Lets look at modals first.
- Modals can co-occur with verbs
- He will eat. He may eat.
- Modals cannot co-occur with modals
- He may will eat.
76Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- You cannot replace a verb with a modal as in
- We are maying.
- Nor can you replace a modal with a verb as in
- We wish be eating.
77Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Modals can co-occur with perfect have.
- He may have eaten.
- Modals can co-occur with progressive be.
- He may be eating.
- Notice that when modals co-occur with any of
these elements, the modal always comes first. -
78Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Lets try a substitution test to determine whether
the following are modals or verbs - Try substituting each of the following words for
will in the following sentence - can, play, could, shall, see
- He will eat.
- Which ones are OK?
79Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Now try substituting the same words for the verb
eat in - He will eat.
- can, play, could, shall, see
- Which ones are OK?
- So our substitution test tells us that
- can, could shall are modals
- play and see are verbs
80Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- Modals always occur before perfect and
progressive (when they are actually present)
and before a verb. - Verbs occur after all possible elements in the
auxiliary.
81Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- How do we know that perfect have -en is not a
modal? - Try the substitution test on
- He will have eaten.
- Can you substitute a modal for have?
- He will may eaten?
- Can you reverse the order of will and have?
- He have will eaten?
82Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- How do we know that progressive be -ing is not
a modal? - Try the substitution test on
- He will have eaten.
- He be having eaten.
-
83Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
- How do we know that progressive be ing and
perfect have en are separate categories? - Try reversing them in
- He may have been eating.
- He may be having eaten.
84Still not convinced about modals?
- Lets apply the meaning test.
- Modals express the speakers attitude or
intentions with respect to the sentence. Modals
can express possibility, probability, necessity,
intention, etc., as in - I will go.
- I may go.
- I must go.
85Still not convinced about modals?
- Lets apply the inflection test.
- Unlike verbs, modals do not take the present
tense s inflection in the 3rd person singular
verbs. - He eats at nine.
- He wills eat at nine.
- He mays eat at nine.
86Homework
- We can apply the distribution and meaning tests
to identify perfect and progressive as distinct
functional categories. - The meanings of sentences with perfect and
progressive are affected in interesting ways. - Think about the meanings of the sentences with
perfect and progressive on the following slide. - Be prepared to discuss them in your DGD.
-
87Perfect and Progressive
- John has studied karate, but he is more
interested in kickboxing. - John had studied karate for ten years before he
got his black belt. -
- I lived in Florida for ten years. Then I moved
to Texas. - I have lived in Florida for ten years, but I
still miss Canada. - I had lived in Florida for ten years before I
saw my first alligator. - I cant talk while I am driving.
- I didnt use my cell while I was driving.
- I am driving to Montreal tomorrow.
-
- Think about the concept of tense in English and
what it expresses -
- I am exercising right now. (present progressive
- ongoing) - I exercise every day. (simple present -
habitual) - I exercised every day last week. (simple past -
completed) - I was exercising when you called me last week.
(past progressive - ongoing in past)
88Perfect
- Perfect have-en expresses the duration of an
event as in I have lived in Florida for ten
years. In the past tense it can also express the
duration of an event with respect to a certain
point in the past, as in I had lived in Florida
for ten years when they discovered I was an
illegal alien. Perfect have-en can also be used
to refer to an event that took place over some
indefinite period in the past, even when the
sentence is actually inflected for the present,
as in I have studied karate. - Perfect have-en is rarely used to indicate a
completed act. Instead English uses the simple
past tense as in I lived in Florida for ten
years. Compare that to the sense of duration in
I have lived in Florida for 10 years. Note also
that the simple present in English cannot express
duration as in I know John for ten years.
However, the simple present in English does
express habitual actions as in I eat breakfast
at seven.
89Progressive
- Progressive be-ing refers to events in progress
in either the present or the past. In the
present, the event is understood as happening
while the sentence is being uttered. In the
past, the event is understood as happening while
another event in the discourse was happening.
Progressive can also communicate anticipation of
an event which will happen as in I am going
shopping later. There is a narrative discourse
style in English that uses the present
progressive and the simple present to relate
events that occurred in the story, as in Im
walking down the street last night and this guy
jumps out from behind a parked car and starts
yelling at me (etc.)
90A word about auxiliaries
- English has four different functional categories
that can be described as auxiliaries - tense, modal, perfect, progressive
- If modal, perfect and progressive are all in a
sentence, they must occur in that specific order
for the sentence to be grammatical. - We will discuss tense placement later.
91From Word Categories to Phrases
- Sentences are made up of sub-parts called
phrases. - Every language has a set of rules for forming
these phrases. - They are called Phrase Structure Rules
- The Phrase Structure Rules are a set of rules
that allow us to map out the structures of
phrases in a language. - We diagram these structures as inverted trees.
92From Word Categories to Phrases
- The inverted trees reflect the hierarchical
arrangement of phrases. - Sentences consist of a series of phrases also
joined together in a hierarchical manner.
93Merge X (X-bar)
- Merge is the part of the syntax in which of
words are fitted together into phrases and in
which phrases are joined together to form
sentences. - Merge combines words into phrases using the X
schema that we will be discussing shortly. - The Phrase Structure Rules determining these
phrases used to be fairly elaborate, but in the
current formulation of the theory have been
essentially streamlined to the X schema.
94Merge Subcategorization
- Phrases are also the product of an interaction
between the X schema and the subcategorization
properties of words. - Subcategorization refers to the type of
complement structures that certain words must or
can appear with. - to hate (verb) requires a direct object
- I hate. I hate carrots.
95I hate carrots.
96Merge D-structure
- Merge results in what used to be called the Deep
(D) Structure of a sentence -
- The D-structure of a sentence very closely
resembles the canonical word order in the
language. - D structure is not, however, the final form of
the sentence.
97Move
- Not all sentences have the canonical word order
in the language. - Thus another component of syntax, called Move,
moves elements to where they belong in the
surface structure or S-structure of the sentence. - Compare
- John is eating an apple. (canonical)
- Is John eating an apple? (non-canonical)
98Why D- and Surface Structure?
- We just saw that not all sentences follow
canonical word order. - But why not allow Merge to create these
structures in the first place? - Why do we need Move?
- I will provide some reasons in the next few
slides and point out others as we explore the
syntax more thoroughly.
99Why D- and Surface Structure?
- By adhering to strict canonical word order,
D-structure gives us important information about
the semantic roles of the elements in a
sentences, especially the main nouns with respect
to the verb. - Thus, for English, the first Noun Phrase
constituent in the tree represents the subject of
the sentence. - The subject of the sentence has a special
relationship to the verb, as it is the doer of
the action or agent.
100Why D- and Surface Structure?
- The first Noun Phrase immediately following a
transitive verb is its direct object or theme. - In semantics, these relationships of nouns with
respect to verbs and their subjects and objects
are called thematic roles. - The students read the book.
- agent theme
101Why D- and Surface Structure?
- The thematic roles of the constituents of the
sentence would not be so apparent at D-structure
if non-canonically ordered trees were permitted. - The move component of the syntax can operate on
D-structure trees to create these non-canonical
orders as required.
102Why D- and Surface Structure?
- At D-structure it is clear when two sentences
have the same thematic relationships between
their words. - Recall the sentences we looked at a few classes
ago
103Why D- and Surface Structure?
- declarative
- Everyone is happy.
- (verb)
- yes/no question
- Is everyone happy?
- (verb)
104Why D- and Surface Structure?
- Wh question
- What are they looking at?
- object
- theme
- Declarative
- They are looking at what.
- object
- theme
105Why D- and Surface Structure?
- scrambled order
- The boy gave the toy to the girl.
- direct indirect object
- theme goal
- The boy gave the girl the toy.
- indirect direct
- goal theme
106Why D- and Surface Structure?
- By having the alternative word orders created by
Move, there is no need for extra phrase structure
rules that would create essentially the same
structure in two different places in the tree. - So we dont need a rule in Merge permitting a
verb to occur at the beginning of a tree as well
as after the subject - Is everyone happy? Everyone is happy.
- We also dont risk confusion about whether the
role of the nouns everyone and what in the
question sentences.
107A schematic of syntax
- Merge
- Deep Structure
- Move
- Syntactic Surface Structure
108Is that all there is to the grammar?
- In the previous slide we saw that Merge creates
D-structure which is acted upon by Move which in
turn creates Syntactic Surface Structure. - Is that it for the grammar? No.
- The syntactic surface structure does not
represent the final spoken form of the utterance. - The rules of phonology and semantic
interpretation (logical form) must apply before
the sentence is fully derived.
109From theory to practice
- Merge is the product of the X schema.
- This basically sets out the structure of a
typical, generic phrase as - XP
- (Specifier) X
- X (complement)
110From theory to practice
- The abbreviations in the generic X phrase
structure tree Xpart of speech of the head of
the phrase - Pphrase (e.g. NP noun phrase)
- Specifierphrase boundary marker, makes
meaning of head more precise - Complementphrases which provide information
about the meaning of the head. The type of
complement a head can take is part of the
information stored with it in the mental
lexicon. - NB Parentheses ( ) mean that an element is
optional in some phrases and with some heads.
111Types of X - Heads
- Heads can be Nouns, Verbs, Adjectives,
Prepositions - At the sentence level, heads are called
- I Inflection C Complementizer
- We will discuss sentence heads later.
112Specifiers (abbreviations in bold)
- The following can act as Specifiers
- Determiners (for nouns N)
- e.g. the, a, this, these, those, no,
- Adverbs (for verbs V)
- e.g. never, perhaps, often, always
- Degree word (for Adjectives A, Prepositions P)
- e.g. very, quite, more, almost
113Complements
- Phrases that add information about entities and
locations implied by the head and for which the
head is subcategorized. - In the following example, the NP carrots is the
complement of the verb hate. - to hate requires a direct object complement
- I hate. I hate carrots.
114Other Complement Examples
- prepositional phrases as in
- I often eat at that restaurant.
- I never approved of that purchase.
- I was certain of his loyalty.
- The destruction of the city angered me.
- 1 and 2 show verb complements,
- 3 shows an adjective complement.
- 4 shows a noun complement.
115Practice drawing phrase structures
- Keep in mind that all phrases follow the x
schema. - XP
- (Specifier) X
- X (complement)
116(No Transcript)
117(No Transcript)
118He often reads tons of books
119He often reads tons of books
120He often reads tons of books
121How to recognize a phrase
- Look for typical heads, such as
- noun, verb, adjective, preposition
- Work from right to left, since English branches
right and your lowest phrases in the tree will
likely be found to the right. - Consider possible structures of phrase and see
how X applies
122Complements
- N B See OA Charts 5.5 (p143), 5.6, 5.7, 5.8
(p144) for types of complements
123Verb Complements
124Noun, adjective, preposition complements
125Phrase Tests
- You can also try the phrase tests of
- substitution
- movement
- coordination
126Substitution Test
- nouns can be replaced by they it
- The boys bought a firecracker.
- verbs can be replaced by do so
- The children can play.
- prepositional phrase can be replaced by there
- He went to the bank.
127Movement Test
- prepositional phrases can often be moved without
compromising the grammaticality of the sentence - The children sang in the chapel.
- In the chapel the children sang.
128Coordination Test
- a phrase can be conjoined with another phrase of
the same type using and - They mowed the lawn.
- They mowed the lawn and raked the leaves.
129Sally saw the car on the hill.
- - hill is a noun, so it must be part of an NP
- the is determiner, so it must be the specifier
of the NP the hill
130Sally saw the car on the hill.
131Sally saw the car on the hill.
- on is a preposition, which is the head of a PP
- PPs have NPs as their complement, so the hill
is the complement of the PP with of.
132Sally saw the car on the hill.
133Sally saw the car on the hill.
- car is a noun, so it must be the head of an NP
- the is a determiner, so it must be the
specifier of the NP with car - on the hill is a PP which acts as a complement
to car
134Sally saw the car on the hill.
135Sally saw the car on the hill.
- -saw is a verb, which acts as the head of a VP
- -saw is subcategorized for an NP complement
- -the NP complement of saw is the car
136Sally saw the car on the hill.
137Sally saw the car on the hill.
- Sally is a noun, which is the head of an NP
138Sally saw the car on the hill.
139But how does it all fit together?
- Using X, the theory treats the subject NP as the
specifier of a new phrase called IP - The theory treats VP of the sentence as the
complement of this phrase. - The head of the phrase is the tense inflection of
the sentence I. - Remember that English has only two tenses
- past (pst) present (-pst)
140Sally saw the car on the hill.
141Try The dog bit the cat.
142The dog bit the cat.
143IP
- IP allows us to deal with the tense and modal
parts of the auxiliary of the sentence. - The remaining parts of the auxiliary (perfect
have and progressive be) - are handled a little differently, as we will see.
144Modals in IP
- The theory treats models has the lexical part of
I of the IP. - This is because models have inherent tense.
- We never add a tense inflection to make them past
or present. - He will eat. (-pst, NB no s in 3rd pers.
sg.) - He would eat if he had money (-pst)
- He would eat whenever anyone fed him. (pst)
145Modals in IP
- You judge the tense of the modal based on the
rest of the sentence or discourse. - If there is a modal, the verb is not marked for
tense. - The dog will bite the cat.
- The dog bites the cat.
146Modals in IP
- Note that if there is no Modal, the tense in the
PS tree matches the inflection on the verb. - This may seem bizarre, but syntactic theory has
had a long-standing conflict of over whether and
how to handle inflectional morphology. - Compare
147(No Transcript)
148(No Transcript)
149Modals in IP
- Any modal could take the place of will in The
dog will bite the cat. - The dog will bite the cat.
- The dog would bite the cat.
- The dog may bite the cat.
- The dog can bite the cat.
- The dog could bite the cat.
- The dog should bite the cat.
- The dog must bite the cat.
- The dog might bite the cat.
- The dog shall bite the cat.
150Perfect and Progressive
- If the sentence contains the auxiliaries perfect
or progressive or both, these are treated as
verbs. - This will give us successive VPs as follows
1511
2
152Modal, Perfect and Progressive
- In The dog has bitten the cat, note that tense
is on perfect have, not on the verb. - Tense with perfect have is relative to the
other events in the discourse. - Compare a sentence with modal and perfect.
- The dog will have bitten the cat.
- Here tense is expressed through modal will,
rather than on have.
153(No Transcript)
154(No Transcript)
155(No Transcript)
156Modal-Perfect-Progressive
- In older versions of the theory, the affixes
associated with perfect have (-en) and
progressive be (-ing) were shown together with
them as unattached inflections in the PS. - Movement took place to attach them where they
belonged. - Nowadays, syntacticians do not want morphological
operations within syntax. - Thus they are glossed over in the PS trees.
157Whole Sentences as Complements
- What if our complement is a whole sentence
embedded in the bigger sentence. - We use the term clause for sentence.
- Hence embedded clause.
- A clause must have a verb.
158Embedded Clauses
- I know that Mary has left.
- Compare it to
- I know the answer.
- The structures are both complements of the
verb, but one is a clause (aka sentence). - The embedded clause acts like an NP.
159Embedded Clauses
- Embedded clauses that replace NPs are marked by
the words that or whether or if. - These are called complementizers.
- They form the head of a CP (complementizer
phrase) that goes into the complement position of
the VP.
160(No Transcript)
161(No Transcript)