Title: Review of graphic representation of uncertainty
1Review of graphic representation of uncertainty
Suraje Dessai Tyndall Centre for Climate Change
Research, UK and School of Environmental
Sciences, University of East Anglia,
Norwich Expert meeting Uncertainty
Communication 10 December 2004, Utrecht
2Jeroen asked me to give this presentation, but
- Disclaimer Im not an expert on graphics In
fact I usually ask a colleague to do my graphs!
You probably wont believe in this presentation
anymore. - I started my research by asking some colleagues
if they new of any research in the area of
graphic representation of uncertainty their
answers
3Pseudo expert elicitation of literature
- Jeroen Look at Tufte, E.R. (1983) The visual
display of quantitative information. Graphics
Press - John Handmer, RMIT University has done some work
in the context of communicating uncertainty in
short-term weather forecasts, but has lacked
funding to to look explicitly at the graphs is
planning some focus groups on this though - Tony Patt/Torsten Grothmann, PIK Look at Morgan
and Herion (1999) and recent IPCC workshop report
(2004) - Richard Moss, CCSP After an examination of the
literature Moss and Schneider (2000) decided
Tukey box plots was a good way to represent
uncertainty in graphs. - Tony Leiserowitz, Decision Research he asked his
physiologist friends but nothing
4There is surprisingly little research done on the
evaluation of the effectiveness of different
graphical ways of communicating uncertainty!
- One exception is Ibrekk and Morgan (1987) so
lets look at what they did. - They used nine different graphs to display
uncertainty and evaluated their ability to
communicate to nontechnical people.
5The nine graphical representation were
- Traditional point estimate with an error bar
that spans a 95 confidence interval - Bar chart (discretized version of the density
function) - Pie chart (discretized version of the density
function) - Conventional probability density function (PDF)
- Probability density function of half its regular
height together with its mirror imagine - Horizontal bars of constant width that had been
shaded to display probability density using dots - Horizontal bars of constant width that had been
shaded to display probability density using
vertical lines - Tukey box modified to indicate the mean with a
solid point - Conventional cumulative distribution function
(CDF), the integral of the PDF.
6Respondents were asked to
Context Subjects were told to suppose that they
were going to a mountain cabin next Friday and
were concerned about snow
- Estimate the forecasters best estimate
- Estimate the chances that there will be more than
2 inches of snow - Estimate the chances that there will be between 2
and 12 inches of snow - Rate how sure they felt about their answer
- Tell us whether they had ever before seen
uncertain information communicated with this sort
of pictures
7- This was done twice
- without having received any explanation of how to
use or interpret the various displays. - With a series of nontechnical explanations of the
meaning and use of each of the displays in the
context of water depth in a flood. - At the end of both sections respondents were
asked which of the pictures would you prefer to
have the newspaper use?
8- On best estimate, point estimate (1) and Tukey
box (8), which explicitly marked the location of
the mean, had the best results. For the other
graphs most people were influenced by the
location of the modes. Subjects were most sure
about their responses for displays 1 and 2 and
least sure about 5 and 9 (CDF). Explanations had
a weak effect. CDFs alone are likely to mislead
significantly - For over threshold only pie chart and CDF
produce correct responses, although performance
is degraded for pie chart after explanation - For between thresholds only pie chart before
explanation and CDF after explanation - The best performance for a 95 confidence
interval was display 1, followed by CDF (9) - Before explanation subjects preferred bar and pie
charts, while after explanation their preferred
pie charts and CDFs
9Some insights from this study
- The performance of a display depends upon the
information that a subject is trying to extract
displays that explicitly contain the information
that people need show the best performance - In making judgements about the location of the
best estimate, people show a tendency to select
the mode rather than the mean unless the mean is
explicitly marked. - Used alone the CDF is not a reliable way to
communicate the mean. - The authors conclude that a CDF plotted directly
above a PDF with the same horizontal scale and
with the location of the mean clearly indicated
on both curves is a good approach
10The literature seems to imply that graphics are a
better way to communicate uncertainty than text
- The COMET Outreach Program undertook a study
examining different methods of communicating
hurricane risks and uncertainties to the general
public. A large majority of the sample (202)
preferred a graphical approach to information
dissemination in contrast to the text-based
approaches in a comparison of a text-based
win advisory versus a graphical advisory, 74.75
of the sample preferred the graphical advisory as
opposed to 11.88 who preferred the text-based
product. - But preference is not understanding Over 62 of
the sample answered correctly, from a map of the
probabilities that there was a 20 chance of the
centre of Hurricane Georges striking within 75
miles of Mobile, Alabama within 72 hours. This
result was in striking contrast to a correct
answer of 24.75 on a similar question using a
copy of a text-based warning issued by the NWS
for the probability of Georges striking Panama
City, Florida. - The authors concluded that there is strong
evidence that the public is better able to
interpret graphical products than textual
products.
11Of course graphic representation of uncertainty
depends on context
- So for example, if one uses the Kandlikar et al.
(2004) scale Ambiguous direction (sign) of change
could be represented graphically like this
12Ambiguous direction (sign) of change
13Ranges upper and lower bounds or as the 5th and
9rth percentiles based on objective analysis or
expert judgment
14(No Transcript)
15Probability distribution determined for a range
of changes in the variable either objectively or
through use of a formal decision analytic survey
or protocol
16Cumulative probabilities
17Which are the most promising experiments to do in
the policy lab to test different graphical
representations?
- Considering there is hardly any empirical work in
this area and the importance of effective risk
communication we could do almost anything! - Here are some suggestions
- Compare text-based versus graphic-based
communication of uncertainty (to discover the
added value of graphics, etc.) - Compare different graphical ways to display
uncertainty (things have moved since 1987,
especially in terms of computer graphics perhaps
a good way to do this would be to ask people
first how they would like graphs to display
uncertainty then test a mix of public and expert
displays in laymen to see which work best) - It seems that most of us work in areas of deep
uncertainty so we resort to scenarios very often
however, these seem to be the most difficult to
communicate uncertainty about as show by this
example
18Whats the best estimate of global temperature
change in 2100?
- Based on this IPCC figure most people are likely
to say something around 3ºC. However, because
scenarios were used to construct these figures
and because the scenarios have no probabilities
associated with it the correct answer would be
the range 1.4-5.8ºC. Of course some text could
make this clear, but this illustrates the
difficulty of communicating uncertainty using
graphics when there is deep uncertainty.
Does the IPCC not know how to communicate
uncertainties ?!?
19References
- Centre for Risk Community Safety (2003)
Communicating uncertainty in short-term weather
forecasts. Report to the Bureau of Meteorology - COMET (2003) http//www.comet.ucar.edu
- Ibrekk, H. and M.G. Morgan (1987) Graphical
communication of uncertain quantities to
nontechnical people. Risk Analysis, 7 (4),
519-529 - Kandlikar, M., J.S. Risbey and S. Dessai (2004)
Representing and communicating deep uncertainty
in climate change assessment. Geosciences (in
press). - Morgan, M.G. and M. Henrion (1990) Uncertainty a
guide to dealing with uncertainty in quantitative
risk and policy analysis. Cambridge University
Press - Murphy, A., S. Lichtensten, B. Fischhoff and R.
Winkler (1980) Misinterpretations of
precipitation probability forecasts. Bulletin of
the American Meteorological Society, 61, 695-701