Review of graphic representation of uncertainty - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 19
About This Presentation
Title:

Review of graphic representation of uncertainty

Description:

Bar chart (discretized version of the density function) ... issued by the NWS for the probability of Georges striking Panama City, Florida. ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:54
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 20
Provided by: copern1
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Review of graphic representation of uncertainty


1
Review of graphic representation of uncertainty
Suraje Dessai Tyndall Centre for Climate Change
Research, UK and School of Environmental
Sciences, University of East Anglia,
Norwich Expert meeting Uncertainty
Communication 10 December 2004, Utrecht
2
Jeroen asked me to give this presentation, but
  • Disclaimer Im not an expert on graphics In
    fact I usually ask a colleague to do my graphs!
    You probably wont believe in this presentation
    anymore.
  • I started my research by asking some colleagues
    if they new of any research in the area of
    graphic representation of uncertainty their
    answers

3
Pseudo expert elicitation of literature
  • Jeroen Look at Tufte, E.R. (1983) The visual
    display of quantitative information. Graphics
    Press
  • John Handmer, RMIT University has done some work
    in the context of communicating uncertainty in
    short-term weather forecasts, but has lacked
    funding to to look explicitly at the graphs is
    planning some focus groups on this though
  • Tony Patt/Torsten Grothmann, PIK Look at Morgan
    and Herion (1999) and recent IPCC workshop report
    (2004)
  • Richard Moss, CCSP After an examination of the
    literature Moss and Schneider (2000) decided
    Tukey box plots was a good way to represent
    uncertainty in graphs.
  • Tony Leiserowitz, Decision Research he asked his
    physiologist friends but nothing

4
There is surprisingly little research done on the
evaluation of the effectiveness of different
graphical ways of communicating uncertainty!
  • One exception is Ibrekk and Morgan (1987) so
    lets look at what they did.
  • They used nine different graphs to display
    uncertainty and evaluated their ability to
    communicate to nontechnical people.

5
The nine graphical representation were
  • Traditional point estimate with an error bar
    that spans a 95 confidence interval
  • Bar chart (discretized version of the density
    function)
  • Pie chart (discretized version of the density
    function)
  • Conventional probability density function (PDF)
  • Probability density function of half its regular
    height together with its mirror imagine
  • Horizontal bars of constant width that had been
    shaded to display probability density using dots
  • Horizontal bars of constant width that had been
    shaded to display probability density using
    vertical lines
  • Tukey box modified to indicate the mean with a
    solid point
  • Conventional cumulative distribution function
    (CDF), the integral of the PDF.

6
Respondents were asked to
Context Subjects were told to suppose that they
were going to a mountain cabin next Friday and
were concerned about snow
  • Estimate the forecasters best estimate
  • Estimate the chances that there will be more than
    2 inches of snow
  • Estimate the chances that there will be between 2
    and 12 inches of snow
  • Rate how sure they felt about their answer
  • Tell us whether they had ever before seen
    uncertain information communicated with this sort
    of pictures

7
  • This was done twice
  • without having received any explanation of how to
    use or interpret the various displays.
  • With a series of nontechnical explanations of the
    meaning and use of each of the displays in the
    context of water depth in a flood.
  • At the end of both sections respondents were
    asked which of the pictures would you prefer to
    have the newspaper use?

8
  • On best estimate, point estimate (1) and Tukey
    box (8), which explicitly marked the location of
    the mean, had the best results. For the other
    graphs most people were influenced by the
    location of the modes. Subjects were most sure
    about their responses for displays 1 and 2 and
    least sure about 5 and 9 (CDF). Explanations had
    a weak effect. CDFs alone are likely to mislead
    significantly
  • For over threshold only pie chart and CDF
    produce correct responses, although performance
    is degraded for pie chart after explanation
  • For between thresholds only pie chart before
    explanation and CDF after explanation
  • The best performance for a 95 confidence
    interval was display 1, followed by CDF (9)
  • Before explanation subjects preferred bar and pie
    charts, while after explanation their preferred
    pie charts and CDFs

9
Some insights from this study
  • The performance of a display depends upon the
    information that a subject is trying to extract
    displays that explicitly contain the information
    that people need show the best performance
  • In making judgements about the location of the
    best estimate, people show a tendency to select
    the mode rather than the mean unless the mean is
    explicitly marked.
  • Used alone the CDF is not a reliable way to
    communicate the mean.
  • The authors conclude that a CDF plotted directly
    above a PDF with the same horizontal scale and
    with the location of the mean clearly indicated
    on both curves is a good approach

10
The literature seems to imply that graphics are a
better way to communicate uncertainty than text
  • The COMET Outreach Program undertook a study
    examining different methods of communicating
    hurricane risks and uncertainties to the general
    public. A large majority of the sample (202)
    preferred a graphical approach to information
    dissemination in contrast to the text-based
    approaches in a comparison of a text-based
    win advisory versus a graphical advisory, 74.75
    of the sample preferred the graphical advisory as
    opposed to 11.88 who preferred the text-based
    product.
  • But preference is not understanding Over 62 of
    the sample answered correctly, from a map of the
    probabilities that there was a 20 chance of the
    centre of Hurricane Georges striking within 75
    miles of Mobile, Alabama within 72 hours. This
    result was in striking contrast to a correct
    answer of 24.75 on a similar question using a
    copy of a text-based warning issued by the NWS
    for the probability of Georges striking Panama
    City, Florida.
  • The authors concluded that there is strong
    evidence that the public is better able to
    interpret graphical products than textual
    products.

11
Of course graphic representation of uncertainty
depends on context
  • So for example, if one uses the Kandlikar et al.
    (2004) scale Ambiguous direction (sign) of change
    could be represented graphically like this

12
Ambiguous direction (sign) of change
13
Ranges upper and lower bounds or as the 5th and
9rth percentiles based on objective analysis or
expert judgment
14
(No Transcript)
15
Probability distribution determined for a range
of changes in the variable either objectively or
through use of a formal decision analytic survey
or protocol
16
Cumulative probabilities
17
Which are the most promising experiments to do in
the policy lab to test different graphical
representations?
  • Considering there is hardly any empirical work in
    this area and the importance of effective risk
    communication we could do almost anything!
  • Here are some suggestions
  • Compare text-based versus graphic-based
    communication of uncertainty (to discover the
    added value of graphics, etc.)
  • Compare different graphical ways to display
    uncertainty (things have moved since 1987,
    especially in terms of computer graphics perhaps
    a good way to do this would be to ask people
    first how they would like graphs to display
    uncertainty then test a mix of public and expert
    displays in laymen to see which work best)
  • It seems that most of us work in areas of deep
    uncertainty so we resort to scenarios very often
    however, these seem to be the most difficult to
    communicate uncertainty about as show by this
    example

18
Whats the best estimate of global temperature
change in 2100?
  • Based on this IPCC figure most people are likely
    to say something around 3ºC. However, because
    scenarios were used to construct these figures
    and because the scenarios have no probabilities
    associated with it the correct answer would be
    the range 1.4-5.8ºC. Of course some text could
    make this clear, but this illustrates the
    difficulty of communicating uncertainty using
    graphics when there is deep uncertainty.

Does the IPCC not know how to communicate
uncertainties ?!?
19
References
  • Centre for Risk Community Safety (2003)
    Communicating uncertainty in short-term weather
    forecasts. Report to the Bureau of Meteorology
  • COMET (2003) http//www.comet.ucar.edu
  • Ibrekk, H. and M.G. Morgan (1987) Graphical
    communication of uncertain quantities to
    nontechnical people. Risk Analysis, 7 (4),
    519-529
  • Kandlikar, M., J.S. Risbey and S. Dessai (2004)
    Representing and communicating deep uncertainty
    in climate change assessment. Geosciences (in
    press).
  • Morgan, M.G. and M. Henrion (1990) Uncertainty a
    guide to dealing with uncertainty in quantitative
    risk and policy analysis. Cambridge University
    Press
  • Murphy, A., S. Lichtensten, B. Fischhoff and R.
    Winkler (1980) Misinterpretations of
    precipitation probability forecasts. Bulletin of
    the American Meteorological Society, 61, 695-701
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com