Title: Yvonne Barnes-Holmes
1Understanding the Clinical Processes in ACT
- Yvonne Barnes-Holmes
- Dermot Barnes-Holmes
2Co-Authors
- Ian Stewart
- Louise McHugh
- Kelly Wilson
- Barbara Johnson
- Brandy Fink
- Andy Cochrane
- Anne Kehoe
- Hilary-Anne Healy
- Claire Keogh
- Jenny McMullen
- Carmen Luciano
- Francisco J. Molina Cobos
- Olga Gutiérrez
- Sonsoles Valdivia
- Marisa Páez
- Miguel Rodríguez
- Francisco Cabello
- Carmelo Visdómine
- José Ortega
- Francisco Montesinos
- Mónica Hernández
- Laura Sánchez
3Introduction
- There is no theory behind therapy, the former is
a coherent set of theoretical constructs that
hang together and make predictions, the latter is
a coherent set of techniques that make a
different set of predictions - Almost never in the history of psychology have
they come together in a manner that was both
theoretically consistent and technologically
effective - ACT is no different, but as the field develops,
there is growing reason to believe that there is
considerable overlap between Relational Frame
Theory (RFT) and ACT and that the former can make
sound predictions about why the latter works, and
to some extent about what the latter should look
like
4Overview
- The current talk will review some of the
predictions and empirical evidence that support
processes and techniques identified in ACT - For the sake of simplicity, and in order to be
consistent with the evidence, we will divide ACT
into the following - Acceptance vs. Avoidance
- Acceptance vs. Cognitive Control
- Values
- Defusion
5Acceptance vs. Avoidance
6Acceptance vs. Avoidance
- Our first place to start looking at ACT (Study 1)
was to analyse the distinction between acceptance
and avoidance if this was not clear-cut, then
the basic terminology might need to be
reconsidered - ACTs emphasis on the dichotomy between
acceptance and avoidance and the development of
the AAQ suggested that we might be able to
functionally differentiate individuals in terms
of their propensity towards acceptance or
avoidance - We took 15 undergraduates who were low in
acceptance (at least 1 SD below the mean on the
AAQ) and 14 high in acceptance (at least 1 SD
above the mean)
7Acceptance vs. Avoidance
- Participants were exposed to a simple automated
task that required them to match nonsense
syllables - During the task, however, matching on some trials
resulted in the presentation of a horrible
aversive image (e.g. mutilated bodies) for 6
seconds - Participants were required to rate each aversive
picture - But, primarily we wanted to determine how long it
took them to do the task when they had
discriminated which type of picture would come
next - Our prediction was that low accepters/high
avoiders would take longer to complete tasks,
which they had learned would be followed by an
aversive picture - This, for us, was a type of avoidance
8Median Reaction Times
- During the task, High Acceptance produced similar
reaction times whether they expected to see
either an aversive or a neutral image next, so
anticipation or avoidance was limited - But, Low Acceptance exhibited significantly
longer reaction times when they expected to see
an aversive image (p 0.015)
3
2.5
High
Low
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
A N A N
But could this be simply because the Low
Acceptance Group perceived the neutral pictures
to be more unpleasant and thus legitimtely more
avoidable than the High Acceptance group?
9Self-Report Ratings
- No, because High Acceptance rated the aversive
images as more unpleasant and more emotionally
intense than Low Acceptance - But yet, Low Acceptance were less willing to look
at either images than High Acceptance
Pleasant Unpleasant
Mild Intense
Willing Unwilling
10Discussion
- So, the outcomes were consistent with ACT
predictions regarding acceptance and avoidance
and their dichotomy - Individuals low in acceptance/high in avoidance
showed greater anticipatory avoidance of the
negative pictures than those high in
acceptance/low in avoidance - This avoidance was consistent with their own
ratings of willingness to look at the pictures - Furthermore, this avoidance occurred even though
these individuals rated the pictures as less
unpleasant and less intense than the other group - The high acceptance groups, therefore, showed
less avoidance and greater experiential
willingness in the face of adversity outcomes
that are consistent with ACT predictions
11ERPs and Avoidance
- Study 2 replicated Study 1, but incorporated
Event Related Potentials (ERPs) during the task
with - 6 High Acceptance
- 6 Low Acceptance
- 6 Mid-Range Acceptance
- Once again, we predicted that level of avoidance
would differentiate and we hoped it would be
detected by the ERPs
12Median Reaction Times
- Identical to Study 1, High and Mid Acceptance
produced similar reaction times for both aversive
and neutral images, showing no anticipation or
avoidance - But, Low Acceptance again emitted longer reaction
times when they expected to see an aversive,
rather than a neutral, image (p 0.0431)
3.5
3
2.5
2
Low
1.5
High
Mid
1
0.5
0
A N A N A N
13Self-Report Ratings
- Again, this was not because the pictures were
less unpleasant, because the High and Mid
Acceptance rated the aversive images as more
unpleasant and emotionally intense than Low
Acceptance - But, Low and Mid Acceptance were less willing to
look at the images
14ERPs Recordings
- As expected, the ERPs recordings discriminated
between the two types of pictures, with the
unpleasant pictures producing significantly more
positive wave forms than the neutral pictures for
all groups
- And an interesting finding emerged with regard to
the scalp locations . . .
15Low Acceptance
Area Dimensions (?V ms)
High Acceptance
16ERPs Recordings
- The fact that the Low Acceptance group showed
greater negative activation for left hemisphere
electrodes could suggest greater verbal activity
for this group, which might indicate the use of
verbal avoidance strategies (e.g. This is not
real, think of something else, etc.)
17Discussion
- So again, the avoidance groups could be
distinguished from one another on several
predictable counts -- Low Acceptance showed
greater anticipation of the aversive images than
the others and were less willing to look at them
-- and yet, they rated the pictures as less
unpleasant - Some willingness distinctions even emerged
between mid and high range accepters - The unwillingness and tolerance avoidance for Low
Acceptance was associated with greater negative
activation for left hemisphere electrodes,
suggesting the activation of verbal areas - Again, the former outcomes are consistent with
ACTs emphasis on acceptance, avoidance and
willingness and the ERPs data were consistent
with RFTs emphasis on verbal behaviour
18Acceptance vs. Cognitive Control
19 Acceptance
- Up until the mid-90s, CBT was still insistent
that explicit attempts to control cognitive
events directly would reduce their frequency and
impact, and thus be associated with positive
clinical outcomes - ACT has always offered a counter-approach because
of its contextualistic underpinnings that argues
that the only way to change verbal events is to
change the context in which they occur and
acceptance is the term we use to describe this
broader target - In this regard, though not intentionally, ACT is
more in line with Eastern traditions that
emphasise acceptance/mindfulness - But Eastern traditions are not sciences and thus
cannot be relied upon to provide scientific
argument or evidence
20 Acceptance
- Although in Eastern traditions and in ACT, we had
reason to believe that acceptance was an active
ingredient in positive clinical outcomes and
psychological well-being generally, there was
almost no empirical evidence to attest to this - Furthermore, positive empirical evidence for the
impact of acceptance would to some extent
undermine positivity for the main existing
alternative that was cognitive control which
functionally may be seen as the opposite of
acceptance - It should also be added that empirical evidence
for cognitive control as an active ingredient in
CBT is relatively scarce, in spite of its wide
usage
21Acceptance
- So, thus far, we had some comfort in the
terminology that suggested a dichotomy between
acceptance and avoidance - But, acceptance as a clinical tool was something
else - In our first empirical analysis of acceptance as
a mechanism of change, we set out with a very
simple aim -- to see if we could construct a
short, but potent, acceptance intervention that
would be functionally similar to what is
presented in therapy, but which might just work
in an experimental context - This was demonstration research of the simplest
kind
22Study 3
- During Study 3, normal participants were simply
presented with a computerised task in which they
were asked to match a lot of neutral pictures and
a small number of horrible aversive pictures
(e.g. mutilated bodies) - The former pictures simply represented an
experimental control, while the latter
represented our core effort to provide
participants with a clinical strategy they could
use to deal with unpleasant psychological/visual
content
23Avoiding Negative Images
- Because the matching was too simple to function
as a dependent variable, we targeted
participants willingness to look at the aversive
pictures by (1) giving them the option to avoid
the pictures altogether before the trial and
counting how many they looked at and (2)
observing how long they would endure them on
screen
24Acceptance or Control
- Participants were exposed to the baseline
matching task, the intervention, and then the
task again - Both interventions involved the presentation of a
vignette in which participants were asked to --
imagine that they had witnessed a horrific car
accident in which they had to rescue the badly
injured and bloodied victims from the car and to
imagine that they found the sight of blood
extremely aversive - They were then given a coping strategy/interventio
n to help them deal with the vignette (and to
influence their subsequent performances on the
negative pictures)
25Acceptance vs. Control
- Participants in Cognitive Control were instructed
to try to control their emotional reactions and
to avoid feelings of discomfort (e.g. by imaging
that the blood was just like tomato ketchup) - Participants in Acceptance were instructed to
fully embrace their feelings of discomfort (i.e.
to fully accept that trying to save the bloodied
and mutilated victims would be the most horrific
experience of their lives)
26Experimenter Influence
- Experimenter influence were also manipulated by
altering the instructions and the extent to which
the experimenter monitored the matching
performances - During the No Instruction/No Monitoring
conditions, participants were informed that it
did not matter whether they looked at the
negative pictures (i.e. no instruction) and the
experimenter sat approximately 30 feet away and
pretended to read a book (no monitoring) - During the Instruction/Monitoring conditions,
participants were told that it was very important
to look at the negative pictures (instruction)
and the experimenter walked around actively
monitoring performances (monitoring)
27Results
- The results of the study failed to differentiate
between the two groups on the number of aversives
observed - However, they did differ in their mean response
latencies while the aversives were on the screen
(i.e. aversive tolerance time)
28Mean Response Times Neutral Pictures
On the neutral pictures, there were no changes at
all between Baseline and Post-intervention, as
expected
29Mean Response Times Aversive Pictures
- But, on the aversive pictures, Acceptance and
Control differed significantly when combined with
Instruction/Monitoring (p 0.002) - Strategy and Experimenter Influence interacted
significantly
30Discussion
- The Acceptance strategy increased participants
tolerance time in the presence of the aversive
pictures (when combined with active experimenter
influence) - Control did not and decreased tolerance in both
cases - While both strategy outcomes appeared to be
influenced by the social context, further
analyses indicated that this primarily affected
the extent to which participants applied the
strategies, rather than affecting the strategies
directly (i.e. the strategies were applied more
when the experimenter attended) - This was our first empirical evidence that
acceptance could be delivered as a brief
therapeutic intervention in an experimental
context and was associated with positive outcomes - Cognitive control was in fact counter-productive
in terms of altering aversive tolerance when the
images were present
31Acceptance vs. Control with Pain
- In Study 4, we were concerned that the data so
far would not generalise to physical pain and the
psychological content associated with that
perhaps different outcomes would emerge relative
to coping with aversive visual imagery - So, we exposed participants to systematic
electric shocks - This was based on a previous study by Gutierrez,
Luciano, Rodriguez, and Fink who compared
acceptance and control as coping interventions
with electric shock with 40 undergraduates - They reported that Acceptance not only increased
shock tolerance, but also reduced participants
believability of their own subjective pain ratings
32Our Study
- Although the original study was entirely
consistent with our own findings thus far, there
was increasing concern within the community about
experimental precision but this was hard to
offset against external validity - So in Study 4, we tried to come up with a format
that was fully automated (hence experimentally
clean), but that would still allow the
interventions to be impactful - We did some refinement of the Acceptance and
Control exercises and metaphors to remove
possible confounds - And we began to look at values as an active
addition to acceptance
33Design
- 40 normal participants were assigned to four
conditions
Intervention Values Context Pre-Intervention Post-Intervention
Acceptance High
Acceptance Low
Control High
Control Low
34Delivery
- The entire procedure was automated through a
program containing a series of video clips - Participants progressed through the clips at
their own pace, individually and alone - Clips were rated first by independent observers,
for consistency, adherence and empathy and were
found to not differ in any capacity
35Delivery
36Acceptance High/Low Values
A Participants were provided with metaphors and
experiential exercises indicating that the best
way to deal with pain related thoughts and
feelings was to accept them in the context of
whatever action is being taken
HV Participants were asked to imagine that they
suffered from chronic pain and that the task
involving shock was one which they must do in
order to support their family LV Participants
were told that the aim of the experiment was to
contribute to research on the relation between
voltage level and perception of shock
37Control High/Low Values
C Participants were given metaphors and
experiential exercises designed to teach them
that the best way to deal with pain related
thoughts and feelings was to distract themselves
by imagining pleasant images
HV Participants were asked to imagine that they
suffer from chronic pain and that the task
involving shock was one which they must do in
order to support their family LV Participants
were told that the aim of the experiment was to
contribute to research on the relation between
voltage level and perception of shock
38Shock Tolerance Data
- The Acceptance participants significantly
increased their shock tolerance from pre- to
post-intervention - Control produced no change
39Self-Report Data
Low Pain
- There was an interesting effect for values
although there was no significant main effect,
High Values participants rated the pain as
greater across time, whereas Low Values rated it
as less
40Tolerating High Pain
- We wanted to check whether some of the effects
were driven by people who had different
perceptions of how much pain they were in -- so
we examined only those reporting great pain more
closely - 100 of participants in Acceptance who reported
greater experienced pain Post-Intervention showed
an increase in tolerance levels, compared to only
50 of the same sub-set of Control (significant
p 0.0455) - We also analysed the number of trials for which
participants continued in the Post-Intervention
task after reporting high levels of pain (gt 80)
and found that the median number of trials for
Acceptance was 4, compared to 2 for Control
(significant p 0.0069)
41Discussion
- So as an intervention, Acceptance worked better
than Control in the context of experimentally
physical pain in the form of electric shock - Changes in tolerance were particularly strong for
participants experiencing a lot of pain and using
Acceptance - The effects were the same as those reported by
other researchers even in a highly structured
automated experimental environment - While the Values manipulation did not have a
significant effect on shock tolerance, it did
affect self-reports of pain, in that participants
in High Values reported more pain subsequent to
the intervention (perhaps the values component
oriented them more towards their pain, but not in
an avoidant way)
42Study 5 Simple Rules
- One issue that had been emerging across
experiments was the possibility that participants
were not really engaging with the various
features of the interventions (i.e. the exercises
and metaphors), but that they were simply
generating or following simple rules - So, in Study 5, we compared the full Acceptance
and Control interventions used before, but added
two new interventions that simply comprised of an
Acceptance Rule and a Control Rule -- a brief and
simple rule for accepting or distracting - In this study, we also employed a Placebo
Condition
43Experimental Conditions
Acceptance Rule Acceptance Rule, Metaphor Exercise Control Rule Control Rule, Metaphor Exercise Placebo
44Tolerance Data
- Only Full Acceptance increased tolerance
significantly from Pre- to Post-Intervention, but
none of the other four - Distraction-Rule actually decreased tolerance
significantly
45More Pain More Shocks
- Again, we looked at those participants who
reported more pain and still took more shocks and
found that these were mostly in the Acceptance
Conditions
46Discussion
- So, the positive acceptance outcomes thus far
could not be explained in terms of simple rule
following the metaphors and exercises were
essential - When these were absent, the moderate improvement
in pain tolerance for an acceptance rule was
non-significant - Although Distraction effects are again negligible
- Distraction actually makes you worse when it
comes in the form of a simple rule
47Different Pain Same Outcome
- The next study (Study 6) was also concerned
acceptance, but attempted to broaden the
generality of the work by employing a new type of
pain induction, that might circumvent criticisms
that electric shock is not a good analogue of
clinical pain - So, three groups of participants were assigned
to - Acceptance
- Control
- Placebo
- And were exposed to the radiant heat pad in a
fully automated procedure
48Heat Apparatus
49Results
- At baseline, the groups did not differ on a
series of psychological measures - And the amount of heat tolerance was tightly
controlled
50Tolerance Data
- Both Acceptance and Control increased pain
tolerance, but only Acceptance was significant
51Discussion
- So, positive outcomes again for acceptance now
a total of six experiments - Acceptance is always significantly better than
Control, which had negligible effects - Outcomes so far have included tightly controlled
experimental environments, a range of populations
and numerous experimental methodologies and types
of pain - The data overall are highly consistent with ACTs
centrality for acceptance and its predictions on
avoidance - The ERPs data were consistent with both ACT and
RFT and added legitimacy to the outcomes and
methodologies
52One Query?
- But one thing troubled us and we had seen it in
research by other labs - In some studies, there had been positive (albeit
limited and never significant) outcomes for
Cognitive Control - So, in the radiant heat research, we began to
look more closely at our interventions and those
used in other studies - In the heat study, in particular, we noticed that
part of the Control intervention involved saying
a pain-related thought aloud before participants
tried to distract themselves from it
53Revisions
- So, we thought that it might just be possible
that this feature offered a type of defusion, or
at least cognitive distancing, that may have
attributed to the outcomes - And we set about modifying the Control
intervention so as to eliminate this potential
confound (Study 7) - Our new condition was called Control Revised
- And we were amazed at what we found . . .
54Tolerance Data
- The effects for Acceptance were exactly the same
- But, Control had no effect at all, and in fact
increased pain tolerance was decreasing
55Discussion
- So, even the small improvements that had been
previously recorded for Cognitive Control may not
have functioned in the way that was intended - Some of the experimental interventions had
spurious features that enabled aspects of
defusion to creep into the Control protocols - In our latter heat experiment in which this
feature was addressed directly, the effects for
Control could not be differentiated from Placebo
56Values
57Investigating Values
- But, of course, there is more to ACT than
acceptance and much of what we do in the therapy
depends upon the combination of active
ingredients rather than simply a series of
incoherent or unintegrated steps - However, as much as possible, we try to isolate
the components individually for experimental
purposes to get a better understanding of
outcomes and processes - So, we turned our attention next to Values
- But note, that where we had looked at values
before, the outcomes were mixed and it would be
very difficult to deliver values as a solitary
intervention
58Values
- We have done only one study (Study 8) to date
looking specifically at values - This study was conducted in Spain and attempted
primarily to assess the influence of a values
clarification exercise - Although two types of exposure to painful private
events were also compared (writing down versus
experiential exercise) across three conditions
Values Clarification Values Clarification Writing Values Clarification Experiential Exercise
59Values
- 10 participants were assessed on personal
barriers, valuable actions and areas of valued
living affected by problems and barriers
Subject 2
Values Clarification
- Values Clarification alone quickly and steadily
reduced barriers and improved reports of valued
living and effect enhanced across time
60Values Clarification Writing
- Values Clarification Writing alone showed a
similar outcome, but the decrease in barriers was
less - Values Clarification Exercise alone was similar
- Overall, the type of exposure to private events
did not matter greatly, and these even softened
the effects relative to Values Clarification
alone
Values Clarification Exercise
61Discussion
- So, some positive effects for values
clarification - No matter, how you do it, a simple values
clarification exercise helps to increase the
extent of actual valued living and decrease
barriers to same - There were some minor differences in terms of how
this can be done, but these were minimal - The data also identified what appeared to be a
functional relationship between decreases in
barriers and improvements in valued living - These are entirely consistent with ACT
predictions regarding how private events can
function as barriers and how these can be altered
with values
62Defusion
63Defusion
- But, no-one would think for a second that ACT
would be ACT without defusion - In fact, defusion, it seems is the gel that glues
the active ingredients together - In fact, acceptance is often difficult when
defusion is not in place - Also, for RFT the deliteralisation effects that
underpin defusion techniques are central to ACTs
outcomes, so in ways studying defusion is perhaps
the best test of the relationship between the
theory and the therapy
64Defusion
- When we started looking at defusion, we had only
one previous study by Masuda et al. (2004) to
work from - They attempted to assess the impact of word
repetition on believability and discomfort levels
associated with negative self-relevant words
(e.g. anxious, anxious, anxious etc.) - Their findings indicated that the use of a
defusion rationale produced greater reductions in
discomfort and believability about the words when
compared to a thought suppression rationale or a
distraction task
65Defusion
- In this study (Study 9) , we automated the
presentation of 20 positive and 20 negative
self-statements - This generated a total of 60 statements because
there were three exposures to each statement - After the appearance in screen of each statement,
participants were asked to provide ratings
regarding their reactions to the statements in
terms of - Comfort
- Believability
- Willingness
66Defusion
- We manipulated defusion in two ways
- (1) Defusion Instructions
- The 80 undergraduates were randomly assigned to
- Defusion Condition (pro-defusion instructions)
- Anti-Defusion Condition (anti-defusion
instructions) - Neutral Condition (neutral-defusion instructions)
67Defusion Instructions
- In the current experiment, we are interested in
the emotional impact of unusual self-statements.
The scientific literature in this area shows that
if you rephrase a self-statement like I am an
awful person into I am having the thought that
I am an awful person, then the emotional impact
of the statement is reduced - In other words, thinking or saying words like I
am having the thought that I am an awful person
is easier to deal with than simply thinking or
saying I am an awful person
68Defusion
- (2) Defusion in Visual Format
- We wanted to see the extent to which defusion
within the visual presentation of the
self-statements would give rise to
defusion-predictable outcomes - To manipulate this, we employed three types of
presentation format for each statement - Normal
- Defusion
- Abnormal
69Normal Negative Self-Statement
Deep down there is something wrong with me
70Defusion Negative Self-Statement
I am having the thought that deep down there is
something wrong with me
71Abnormal Negative Self-Statement
I have a wooden chair and deep down there is
something wrong with me
72Results Comfort
- The (pro) defusion instructions were correlated
with less discomfort than the other two types of
instruction - As was the defusion presentation format
73Results Willingness
- The (pro) defusion instructions were correlated
with more willingness than the other two types of
instruction - As was the defusion presentation format very
similar results to comfort ratings
74Results Believability
- Contrary to predictions, the (pro) defusion
instructions were correlated with more
believability than the other two types of
instruction - As was the defusion presentation format very
similar results to comfort and willingness ratings
75Discussion
- Although they looked impactful in the ratings,
the defusion instructions did not have a
significant influence - However, the Defused presentation format
significantly decreased discomfort, increased
willingness, but unexpectedly increased
believability - However, on closer inspection of the data and
other information gathered from participants it
may be the case that they were rating the
believability of whole statements I am having
the thought that . . rather than the content
itself this is not unlike defusion
76Discussion
- So, increases in willingness to having negative
self-referential content were consistent with
ACTs predictions regarding defusion - Believability ratings, upon closer inspection,
suggested that the defused format decreased
participants believability of the content
directly - Decreases in discomfort were not directly
predicted by ACT, but such outcomes are positive
although they would not be targeted directly
77Defusion Interventions
- In the previous study, we had assessed simple
impacts for defusion and found that it generated
positive and largely ACT consistent outcomes even
when defusion occurred within the visual
presentation of the content - But, if we employed defusion as an intervention,
as had been the case for Masuda et al., would we
find similar outcomes? - Study 10 attempted to address this question
78Study 10
- Participants generated a personalised negative
self-relevant thought that represented a summary
of several related personal statements - They were then given a written protocol that
contained an instruction followed by an exercise - The three protocols were
- Defusion
- Thought Control
- Placebo
79Experimental Conditions
Rationale Exercise
Defusion Defusion
Thought Control Thought Control
Defusion Thought Control
Thought Control Defusion
Defusion Placebo
Thought Control Placebo
Placebo Defusion
Placebo Thought Control
Placebo Placebo
80Method
- Once again, the emotional impact of the negative
self-referential statements was measured in terms
of - Discomfort
- Believability
- Willingness
81Results Comfort
Uncomfortable
100
80
60
40
20
Pre-Intervention
0
Post-Intervention
Comfortable
Condition
- All interventions with a defusion component
generated decreases in discomfort - But, the largest effects were DD and PD,
suggesting activity in the defusion exercise
82Results Comfort
- Interestingly, the only significant differences
pre- and post-intervention emerged for the
following conditions - Placebo-Defusion
- Defusion-Placebo
- Defusion-Defusion
- Thought Control-Thought Control
83Results Believability
Believable
Pre-Intervention
Post-Intervention
Unbelievable
Condition
- All effects were in the right direction of
decreasing believability - But, D-D and TC-D showed largest decreases in
believability
84Results Believability
- The only significant differences pre- and
post-intervention emerged for the following
conditions - Placebo-Defusion
- Placebo-Thought Control
- Defusion-Placebo
- Defusion-Defusion
- Defusion-Thought Control
- Thought Control-Defusion
- Thought Control-Thought Control
- So, a very mixed bag overall
85Results Willingness
- All effects were in the right direction of
decreasing unwillingess - But, D-TC was the only significant outcome
86Discussion
- Quite a mixed bag overall
- But, generally most positive effects in predicted
directions for packages containing defusion
features - Defusion exercise appeared to be somewhat more
effective than a simple rationale
87Concluding Comments
88Concluding Comments
- There are many more analogue studies completed
and underway than those reported here - The effects for ACT components across the board
are predominantly as predicted and compare
favourably with substantively weaker outcomes
generated by target comparisons - The range of issues generated by the studies
shows the complexity of the effects and the
difficulty in conducting high quality research in
this modality - As studies progress, the standard of experimental
rigour is exceptional
89Concluding Comments
- Automated interventions
- Balancing for gender
- Balancing for heat tolerance, acceptance etc.
- Pre-screening with relevant psychological
assessments - Including self-report measures
- Blind experimenter
- Use of different types of physical and
psychological stressors - Use of non-clinical populations
- Very substantive N in some cases
- Interventions are very closely matched,
topographically and functionally - Range of ACT components tested
90Concluding Comments
- We are now in a place where these types of
analyses can be done effectively and with high
levels of precision - The evidence is overwhelmingly positive . . .