Title: Stimulus Control and Language
1Stimulus Control and Language
2Why is Understanding Stimulus Control so
Important in Teaching Children with Autism?
- Because the aim of virtually all instruction is
to get specific responses to occur reliably under
particular antecedent stimulus conditions, all
instructional techniques involve manipulations of
antecedent stimuli, along with manipulations of
consequent stimuli (Green, 2001, p. 74)
3Why is Understanding Stimulus Control so
Important in Teaching Children with Autism?
- Spradlin and Brady (1999) conceptualized the
impairments in language and social skills in
children with autism - as limitations in the development of stimulus
control
4Social Skill Deficiencies
- Appear very early in life
- as young as several months old
- By 1-year old many showed
- Less eye contact
- Imitate less
- Less likely to point out objects to others
- Failed to engage in reciprocal games such as
pat-a-cake and peek-a-boo - Less symbolic or make beileve play
- Less likely to recognize social emotional
responses in other people
5Communication Skill Deficits
- Less likely to
- demonstrate multiple nonverbal communicative
behavior such as joint attention - Response to names verbal directions
- More likely to request than label
6Spradlin and Brady (1999)
- Proper stimulus control may not develop in
children with autism because social stimuli do
not function as reinforcers - It has been suggested that the patterns of the
human face and physical contact serve as primary
reinforcers for typically developing infants - Some children with autism avoid such contact
- Do faces and hugging not serve as reinforcers
from infancy? - What impact would this have on the development of
other reinforcers? Social interactions?
Language? - Proper stimulus control may not develop because
children with autism require more consistent
relationships between antecedents, responses, and
consequences
7Classical Conditioning Limitations
- Infants developed conditioned responses by
pairing various social stimuli with stimuli that
elicited the rooting response. - E.g., mothers voice or even footstep
- Perhaps children with autism fail to develop such
conditioned responses - The natural world is inconsistent in its pairing
of social stimulus with unconditioned stimuli - Perhaps children with autism require more
consistent pairings than other infants for
conditioning to occur or may be more likely to be
conditioned to nonsocial aspects of their
physical environment.
8Limitations in Learning Simple Discrimination
- Perhaps children with autism require that a
higher percentage of responses in the presence of
a stimulus be followed by the same consequence,
if they are to come under the control of that
stimulus. - Might explain why children with autism prefer
playing with objects than people - Contingencies of physical world may be more
reliable that the contingencies of the social
world. - E.g., child gestures to parent may or may not be
picked up. Child reaches out for object, always
results in obtaining object
9Limitations in Learning Simple Discrimination
- Overselectivity
- Lovaas found that children with autism often
learn to respond to some of the stimulus but not
to all of the stimulus of a complex stimulus - Sunberg Partington (Cond Disc)
- Truck in the presence of a truck and the spoken
word truck - Typically developing child may respond truck
in the presence of just a truck after a few
pairings, a child with autism may learn to only
response truck in the presence of the spoken
word
10Limitations in Learning Simple Conditional
Discrimination
- Most conventional stimulus control is conditional
- In the presence of discriminative stimulus a
specific response will be reinforced only when
conditional stimuli are present. - Typically developing children master this easily
and their behavior is reinforced for a high
percentage of response they make in the presence
of such discriminative stimuli - Children with autism fail to make that
conditional discrimination and may ultimately
extinguish all responses to the discriminative
stimuli - Social behavior saying come play with me in
the presence of Mom who is not otherwise engaged.
Child with autism my try to initiate play all at
the wrong times and eventually stop trying. Or
initiate greetings all at the wrong times.
11Social World
Social contingencies are often inconsistent
ReinforcerBeing held by Mom
SD Mom
Response Reach up
But mom may not always hold you when you reach
up And the way mom looks, smells, sounds, feels,
and holds you may be different
12Non-social World
- Contingencies in the nonsocial physical world
may be more consistent and reliable - So may be more easily learned by children with
autism - Might explain why children with autism prefer
playing with objects over people
Reinforcer Feel object in hand
Response Reach out
SD Object
Getting to hold the object is consistent and the
object is consistently the same
13Limitations in Stimulus Class Acquisition
- Written spoken picture
- Word ? word ? of ?
actual dog - DOG DOG a dog
- Stimuli are linked by shared function not
necessarily physical properties - Children with autism are limited in their
development of stimulus classes consisting of
members without defining physical properties - Leads to substantial limitations in language
development - Can imitate speech but that speech might have no
relation to the objects and events that are
typically related to that speech - Or read but not understand what he is reading
14Limitations in the Recombination of Minimal
Stimulus-Response Units
- Much of our behavior involves responding
appropriately to relatively novel situations - Generalized imitation
- Little similarity between what a child sees and
the specific stimulus produced by the imitator's
own behavior - Perhaps the development of generalized imitation
occurs as the development of instruction
following behavior - Multiple exemplar training results in learning a
number of minimal stimulus response units when
than may be recombined within the imitator is
presented a novel combination of responses for
imitation - Children with autism tend to imitate objects
easier than people - Leads to substantial social skill deficit
15Limitations in the Recombination of Minimal
Stimulus-Response Units
- Much of our behavior involves responding
appropriately to relatively novel situations - Following a verbal instruction
- By 2, most children can follow 3-word directions
- Made on the basis of conditional discriminations
- Children who can recombine units will be more
effective because they have the appropriate
word-object and word-action equivalences. - Recombinations of conditional discriminations and
equivalences classes may be limited in children
with autism - Child can complete complex chains of behavior if
the chains were invariant but would fail to
perform a series of responses on the basis of a
novel instruction.
16Use of Operant Teaching Methods
- Of course we can teach children with autism by
establishing stimulus control by structuring
consistent relations between specified stimuli
behavior and consequences
17Procedures to Overcome Basic Reinforcers Problems
- Classical conditioning and building motivational
systems
18Basic Discrimination Training Procedures
- Establishing attending skills and eye contact
through prompting and reinforcement - What type of prompts??
19Establishing New Forms of Conditional Stimulus
Control
- Imitation
- Direction following
20Establishing New Forms of Conditional Stimulus
Control
- Identity matching (single mode)
- Visual visual
- Auditory auditory
- Arbitrary matching (multimodal)
- Visual auditory
- Auditory visual
21Teaching Recombination of Stimulus-Response Units
- Teach direction following to promote
recombination's - Give me, go get, put on
- Plane, paper, coat
- Test for generalization of any give me, go get,
and put on instruction
22Concept Formation
- Complex stimulus control that results in
- Generalization within a class of stimuli and
- Discrimination between classes of stimuli
23Stimulus Class (Cooper et al., 2007)
- AKA Concept
- Set of stimuli that occasion a common response
These would probably be in the same stimulus
class for most people
24Types of Stimulus Classes (Fields Reeve, 2000)
- Perceptual Class
- Stimuli in the set share some physical
characteristics - Examples dogs, flowers, children, chairs, cars,
etc. - Relational Class
- Stimuli in the set characterize some abstract
relationship - Example examples of bigger than,
same/different - Equivalence Class
- Stimuli do NOT share any physical characteristics
(Stimuli go together just because society says
so) - Example numeral 1 written one spoken WUN
- Fields, L., Reeve, K. F. (2000). Synthesizing
equivalence classes and natural categories from
perceptual and relational classes. In J. C.
Leslie, D. Blackman (Eds.). Experimental and
applied analysis of human behavior (pp. 59-84).
Reno, NV Context Press.
25Equivalence Classes Definition
- a finite group of physically disparate stimuli
(no perceptual similarity) - stimuli become related solely as a function of
teaching (Fields, Adams, Buffington, Yang,
Verhave, 1996 Fields, Reeve, Adams, Brown,
Verhave, 1991 Sidman Tailby, 1982 Sidman,
1994) - Emergence of accurate responding to untrained and
nonreinforced stimulus-stimulus relations - Following the reinforcement of responses to some
stimulus-stimulus relations - An equivalence class must contain at least three
stimuli (but often has many more)
26Example Stimuli in a 3-Member Equivalence Class
- Written word
- Spoken word
- A picture
DOG
DOG
27Another Example
CAT
- Written word
- Spoken word
- A picture
CAT
28Stimulus Equivalence
- Train
- Emergent Relations
- Symmetry B ? A and C?B
- Transitivity A ? C
- Equivalence C ? A
DOG
dog
B
C
A
29Teaching Testing Summary
- We TAUGHT 2 conditional relations
- A ? B
- B ? C
- We TESTED for 4 EMERGENT (DERIVED) relations
- B ? A symmetry
- C ? B symmetry
- A ? C transitivity
- C ? A equivalence
- (Another set of tests for REFLEXIVITY (IDENTITY)
is often omitted if the learner already has this
skill in her repertoire A ? A B ? B
C ? C )
30Equivalence Class Training Testing Procedures
- Usually taught and tested with arbitrary
match-to-sample (MTS) - Symbolic notation is used to outline
training/testing procedures - A, B, C, N represent each of the disparate
stimuli that will make up the class - 1, 2, 3, etc. notate the number of classes to be
established - Thus, A1 first stimulus in class 1 B3 second
stimulus in class 3 etc.
31Training Testing Procedures
- To establish equivalence classes, at least two
potential classes must be trained concurrently - training establishes both substitutability of all
stimuli within a particular equivalence class in
addition to discrimination between classes
32Training Testing Procedures
- To establish equivalence classes with three
members, at least two stimulus-stimulus relations
must be trained for each potential class - Lets consider our DOG and CAT potential
equivalence classes - First train the AB relation (given stimulus A
select stimulus B) - the word DOG (A1) is presented as a sample
- The positive comparison (Co) would be the spoken
word dog (B1) (Selection would result in positive
feedback or reinforcement) - the negative comparison (Co-) would be the spoken
word cat (B2) (Selection would result in
corrective feedback or no reinforcement)
33Training AB relation
DOG
A1
DOG
CAT
B1
B2
34Training Testing Procedures
- To train the AB relation
- word CAT (A2) is presented as a sample
- positive comparison (Co) would be the spoken
word cat (B2) (Selection would result in positive
feedback or reinforcement) - negative comparison (Co-) would be the spoken
word dog (B2) (Selection would result in
corrective feedback or extinction)
35Training AB relation
CAT
A2
DOG
CAT
B1
B2
36Training Testing Procedures
- Once responding is 100 correct, we can conclude
AB conditional relation has been learned - At this point we can either continue training
more conditional discriminations or we can do our
first test for an EMERGENT (DERIVED) RELATION (a
conditional discrimination that emerges with no
direct training history) - If the learner knows that A goes with B, can
they demonstrate the reverse? (B goes with A) - This emergent relation shows SYMMETRY
37TESTING BA symmetry relation
CAT
B2
CAT
DOG
A1
A2
38TESTING BA symmetry relation
DOG
B1
CAT
DOG
A1
A2
39Training Testing Procedures
- the BA conditional relation is called symmetry
because the relation is a mirror image or
reversal of the one directly trained (i.e., AB
then BA) - At this point, we can continue training another
conditional discrimination - Lets train the BC conditional relation
40Training BC relation
DOG
B1
C2
C1
41Training BC relation
CAT
B2
C2
C1
42Training Testing Procedures
- Once the BC conditional relation is learned, we
can do our 2nd test for an EMERGENT (DERIVED)
RELATION - If the learner knows that B goes with C, can
they demonstrate the reverse? (C goes with B) - This emergent relation would show a SECOND
SYMMETRY relation
43TESTING CB symmetry relation
C1
DOG
CAT
B1
B2
44TESTING CB symmetry relation
C2
DOG
CAT
B1
B2
45Training Testing Procedures
- If CB symmetry TEST responding is 100 correct,
we can continue testing for another EMERGENT
(DERIVED) RELATION - If the learner knows that A goes with B, and B
goes with C, can they demonstrate that A goes
with C? - This emergent relation would show a TRANSITIVE
relation
46TESTING AC transitive relation
DOG
A1
C2
C1
47TESTING AC transitive relation
CAT
A2
C2
C1
48Training Testing Procedures
- If the AC transitive TEST responding is 100
correct, we have one last test for another
EMERGENT (DERIVED) RELATION - If the learner knows that A goes with B, and B
goes with C, can they demonstrate that C goes
with A? - This emergent relation would shows a combination
of symmetry and transitivity it is called an
EQUIVALENCE relation
49TESTING CA equivalence relation
C1
CAT
DOG
A2
A1
50TESTING CA equivalence relation
C2
CAT
DOG
A2
A1
51Teaching Testing Summary
- We TAUGHT 2 conditional relations
- A ? B
- B ? C
- We TESTED for 4 EMERGENT (DERIVED) relations
- B ? A symmetry
- C ? B symmetry
- A ? C transitivity
- C ? A equivalence
- (Another set of tests for REFLEXIVITY (IDENTITY)
is often omitted if the learner already has this
skill in her repertoire A ? A B ? B
C ? C )
52Teaching Stimulus Classes
- We need to do a better job at this
- Teach
- picture ? object ? spoken word
- Test spoken word sets the occasion for the
picture to ensure an equivalence class has been
established
53Children with Autism Have Difficulty Learning
Concepts (Reeve et al., 2007)
- May be due to failure to respond to a wide range
of stimuli - If a child only learns dog in the presence of 2
dogs, then he might not learn the accurate
concept dog why? - May be due to failure to respond to multiple
components of stimuli - If a child only attends to four legs as a feature
of dog, then he will not learn the accurate
concept dog why?
54Overselectivity
- Children with autism often respond to some parts,
but not all parts, of a complex stimulus - Lovaas, Schreibman, Koegel, and Rehm (1971)
- When parts of the stimulus were then presented
alone - Typically developing children responded to the
complex stimulus and single stimuli similarly - Children with autism responded primarily to only
one of the stimuli (it differed across children
which one)
Reinforcer candy
Response Press lever
55Limitations in Equivalence Class Acquisition
- How would this affect language development?
- Child may be able to imitate speech, but it might
have no relation to the objects and events
typically related to it - Relationship between dog and dog, but no
relationship between dog and - Or read but not understand what he is reading
- Relationship between DOG and dog, but no
relationship between DOG and
56Jackson, Williams, and Biesbrouck (2006)
- In general, studies show that people with basic
language abilities demonstrate equivalence
relations and those that dont do not - It is not known if
- Language is necessary for the formation of
equivalence classes OR - The formation of equivalence classes plays a role
in the development of language
57So, How Should We Teach?
- There are different ways to present stimuli, but
research suggests that - Simultaneous Discrimination arrangement produces
faster discrimination and generalization - Simultaneous Discrimination Arrangement
- SD and S? are presented together at same time
- Successive Discrimination Arrangement
- SD and S? are presented separately
58Green (2001)
- Present the comparisons in an array on a board
(Velcro!) - Begin each trial with having the learner make an
observing response (look at teacher or point to
sample) - Teach the learner to point rather than put with
same - Have at least 3 comparisons on every trial
- Present the samples unsystematically. A good
rule of thumb is dont present the same sample
more than 2x in a row. - Limit the auditory stimulus to the target word
(i.e., dont say touch, point to)
59(No Transcript)
60Green (2001)
- Present each sample equally often.
- All comparisons should be in the field during
every trial. - Each comparison should be the correct answer for
only 1 sample. - Each comparison should be the incorrect answer
equally often. - The number of comparisons in the field should
equal the number of samples presented in a block
of trials. - Within a session of trials, a different sample
should be presented on each trial, but the same
comparisons should appear on every trial. - Each comparison should be the correct response
for only one sample, and should be the incorrect
response equally often. - The position of the correct comparison should
vary from trial to trial. - Between trials, rearrange the comparison stimuli
out of sight - Use errorless teaching methods (most-to-least
prompting and prompt fading)
61Green (2001)
62References
- Chavez-Brown, M., Scott, J., Ross, D. (2005).
Antecedent selection Comparing simplified and
typical verbal antecedents for children with
autism. Journal of Behavioral Education, 14,
153-165. - Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., Heward, W. L.
(2007). Applied behavior analysis. Upper Saddle
River, NJ Pearson Prentice Hall. - Fields, L., Reeve, K. F. (2000). Synthesizing
equivalence classes and natural categories from
perceptual and relational classes. In J. C.
Leslie, D. Blackman (Eds.). Experimental and
applied analysis of human behavior (pp. 59-84).
Reno, NV Context Press. - Green, G. (2001). Behavior analytic instruction
for learners with autism Advances in stimulus
control technology. Focus on Autism and Other
Developmental Disabilities, 16, 72-85. - Laraway, S. Snycerski, S., Michael, J., Poling,
A. (2003). Motivating operations and terms to
describe them Some further refinements.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36,
407-414. - Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R.,
Rehm, R. (1971). Selective responding by
autistic children to multiple sensory input.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 211-222. - Reeve, K. F., Reeve, S. A., Brown, J. L.
(2007, August). Concept formation in children
with autism What do we know about it? Paper
presented at 3rd annual conference of the New
Jersey Association for Behavior Analysis, Rutgers
University. - Spradlin, J. E., Brady, N. C. (1999). Early
childhood autism and stimulus control. In P. M.
Ghezzi, W. L. Williams, J. E. Carr (Eds.),
Autism Behavior analytic perspectives (pp.
49-65). Reno, NV Context Press.