Title: Perceptual Classes Concepts Categories
1Perceptual Classes(Concepts Categories)
- Ps625 Concept Formation
- Dr. Ken Reeve
- Caldwell College Grad ABA Programs
2Other Names of Perceptual Classes
- similarity-based categories (Wasserman
DeVolder, 1993) - open-ended categories (Herrnstein, 1990)
- generalization classes (Stemmer, 1980)
- feature classes (McIlvane, Dube, Green, Serna,
1993 Stromer, Mackay, 1997).
3Definition
- set of perceptually similar, but discriminable,
stimuli that controls a common response - Responding comes under the control of the entire
set of stimuli even though the organism was
directly trained to respond to a smaller subset
of the stimuli in the class
4Types of Perceptual Classes
- types of perceptual classes can be differentiated
by the stimulus domain, or set of limiting
properties, that characterize the stimuli in the
classes - Typically this involves looking at dimensions and
features
5Dimensional Perceptual Classes
- dimensional class is, is defined by a range of
discriminable values along some stimulus
dimension (Fields et al., 1997 Herrnstein, 1984
Zentall, Jackson-Smith, Jagielo, 1990) - stimulus dimension refers to some continuous
range of measurable values resulting from changes
in a particular stimulus property, feature, or
characteristic (Guttman Kalish, 1956 Jenkins
Harrison, 1960, 1962
6Dimensional Perceptual Classes
- Within a specific range along the dimension, each
stimulus occasions a particular response - This occurs even though the emission of the
response was reinforced only in the presence of
some small subset of the stimuli in the range - Other stimuli outside of this range along the
dimension do not occasion the response.
7Types of Dimensional Classes
- dimension along which such a class is defined can
be a simple physical one such as - length (Fields et al., 1997)
- Color (visible light wavelength) (Zentall,
Jackson-Smith, Jagielo, 1990) - sound frequency (Njegovan, Ito, Mewhort,
Weisman, 1995 Risley, 1964) - object rotation (Wasserman, Gagliardi, Cook,
Kirkpatrick-Steger, Astley, Biederman, 1996)
8Example of Dimensional Classes
- consider a tabletop containing long and short
sticks - child is taught to say short in the presence of
a 5 cm stick and long in the presence of a 15
cm stick (see next)
9Example of Dimensional Classes
- Say long
- Prompt if necessary
- Reinforce if child is correct
10Example of Dimensional Classes
- Say short
- Prompt if necessary
- Reinforce if child is correct
11Example of Dimensional Classes
- Continue to alternate between these two trials
until come defined criterion is met - Ex. Correct on 6 trials in a row
- Lean out reinforcement
12Example of Dimensional Classes
- we can then test (with no reinforcement provided)
the extent to which other sticks along the
dimension of length will occasion either of these
verbal responses - Results Maybe 3 or 4 of the different shorter
sticks will occasion the verbal response short
while another 4-5 of the different longer sticks
will occasion the verbal response of long
13Example of Dimensional Classes
- The frequency of responding short or long as
a function of changes along length can then be
plotted as a generalization gradient (Guttman
Kalish, 1956 Jenkins Harrison, 1960, 1962
Honig Urcuioli, 1981).
14Example of Dimensional Classes
- The gradient will show that all sticks within a
particular range occasion the verbal response
short every time they are presented while
another range along the dimension will occasion
the verbal response long. - Thus, these two ranges of stimuli may be
functioning as separate dimensional classes
15Example of Dimensional Classes
- some sticks that are intermediate in length may
occasion the response short sometimes and
long other times - These stimuli are not functioning as members of
either dimensional class
16Example of Dimensional Classes
- Thus, in a dimensional class, only stimuli within
a specific range of values along the dimension
occasion a particular response even though the
physical dimension itself is continuous (Fields
et al., 1997 Risley, 1964 Zentall et al., 1990)
17MoreDimensional Classes
- quantifiable measures of the stimuli (such as
length) are easily obtained - These measures can then be used to determine how
they correspond to the control of responding by
specific ranges of stimuli along the dimension
(Blough, 1990) - Thus, stimuli comprising dimensional classes
differ quantitatively from each other
18Another Type of Perceptual Class Fuzzy Classes
- In these classes, properties of the stimuli are
more difficult to objectively measure because the
stimuli differ qualitatively from one another
(Adams, Fields, Verhave, 1993 Blough, 1990
Herrnstein, 1990 Lea, 1984 Rosch Mervis,
1975 Von Fersen Lea, 1990 Wittgenstein, 1968) - stimuli in fuzzy classes contain different
combinations of discrete physical
characteristics, or features, that occur in
different combinations across the class members
(Cerella, 1979 Cook et al., 1990 Jitsumori,
1993, 1996 Herrnstein, 1984) - Thus, stimuli in fuzzy classes differ from one
another across a multiplicity of features and/or
along different dimensions
19Other Names of Fuzzy Classes
- Fuzzy categories have been referred to by a
variety of names including - basic level classes (Rosch, 1978 Rosch Mervis,
1975 Rosch, Mervis, Gray, Johnson,
Boyes-Bream, 1976) - natural classes (Herrnstein, 1990 Lea Ryan,
1984 Wasserman DeVolder, 1993 Wittgenstein,
1968) - ill-defined classes (Bourne, Dominowski,
Loftus, 1979 Homa Little, 1985 Omohundro,
1981 Neisser, 1967) - probabilistic categories (Medin Smith, 1984)
20Examples Fuzzy Classes
- researchers have investigated fuzzy categories
comprised of - leaves (Cerella, 1979)
- fish (Herrnstein de Villiers, 1980)
- people (Malott Siddall, 1972)
- cars, cats, and flowers (Wasserman, Kiedinger,
Bhatt, 1988) - locations on a college campus (Honig Stewart,
1988) - trees and water (Herrnstein, Loveland, Cable,
1976) - different musical pieces (Porter Neuringer,
1985)
21Fuzzy Classes Control of Responding
- responding to the members of a fuzzy category
comes under the control of some unspecified
combination of many of the potential features
found across the class members (Herrnstein, 1990
Jitsumori, 1993, 1996 Lea Ryan, 1984 Medin
Smith, 1984 Rosch Mervis, 1975 Smith Medin,
1981 Wasserman, Kiedinger, Bhatt, 1988) - That is, of the many possible stimulus features
that occur across class members, a class exemplar
must contain some non-specified number of these
features to evoke the same responding as other
class members - Thus, the phrase some of many characterizes the
stimulus domains of fuzzy classes because the
specific features are not easily identified or
quantified - In addition, no one characteristic is either
necessary nor sufficient to define membership in
a fuzzy class.
22Fuzzy Classes
- unlike stimuli that comprise dimensional classes,
stimuli in fuzzy classes cannot be arrayed along
simple dimensions such as length, temperature,
brightness, size, etc. - Stimuli in fuzzy classes, however, can be scaled
along psychometrically defined dimensions. - One example that illustrates the creation of such
a dimension involves the sorting of stimuli
according to their typicality in a particular
category (Bourne, Dominowski, Loftus, 1979
Cook et al., 1990 Lea Ryan, 1990 Rosch
Mervis, 1975 Smith, 1989). - More
23Fuzzy Classes Rosch Mervis 1975
- Human participants were presented with six
different sets of visual stimuli (car, truck,
airplane, chair, table, and lamp) with 15 members
in each. - Participants were asked to rate each stimulus in
the six sets on a 7-point scale corresponding to
the degree to which that stimulus was
representative (or typical) of the category
(thus, 7 was the most typical) - these best exemplars, or most representative
members of each class, were called prototypes
for each class by Rosch Mervis - Other stimuli could thus be ranked according to
how prototypical they were of each category and
then plotted along a dimension of prototypicality - Lets try to rate the followingMoregtgtgt
24Fuzzy Classes Rosch Mervis 1975
25Dimensional Fuzzy Classes
- Both dimensional classes and fuzzy categories
have been referred to as open-ended categories
(Herrnstein, 1990) because the number of
potential class members in both is theoretically
unlimited - With regard to dimensional classes, because the
members can be arrayed along some continuous
dimension there are an infinite number of
exemplars for each dimensional class - With regard to fuzzy classes, because there is
presumably no limit to the number of combinations
of some of many features that members of a
basic level category can contain, there is also a
potentially infinite number of exemplars
26Polymorphous Classes
- Unlike dimensional and fuzzy classes, a third
type of perceptual category, called a
polymorphous class, contains a closed or finite
number of potential class members (Herrnstein,
1990) - Polymorphous categories are frequently
investigated as artificial laboratory analogs
of basic level or fuzzy categories - Unlike the unspecified some of many features
that define fuzzy class membership, the features
found in members of polymorphous classes are
generally experimenter-defined and, thus, more
easily quantified
27Polymorphous Classes
- use of experimenter-defined polymorphous classes
came from difficulty identifying the some of
many stimulus features that come to control
behavior in naturally occurring fuzzy categories
such as trees, fish, etc. (Blough, 1990
Jitsumori, 1993, 1996 Lea Ryan, 1990. - Membership of stimuli in polymorphous classes is
defined by a mathematical rule in which at least
m of some n experimenter-defined features must be
present in a stimulus (Aydin Pearce, 1994
Fersen Lea, 1990 Jitsumori, 1993, 1996 Lea,
1984 - These m features are a subset of a finite number
of n features that the stimuli in polymorphous
classes can contain - Thus, polymorphous classes may contain a large
number of exemplars, but they are limited in size
by the number of possible n features each
exemplar may possess.
28Example of Polymorphous Class
- Diagnostic Criteria for Autistic Disorder
- A total of six (or more) items from (A), (B), and
(C), with at least two from (A), and one each
from (B) and (C) - A qualitative impairment in social interaction
- B qualitative impairments in communication
- C restricted repetitive and stereotyped
patterns of behavior
29Example of Polymorphous Class
- Stimulus must have at least 3 of 4 (m of n) of
the following features to be a member - Star shaped
- Black dot in interior
- Circle touching it
- Plus sign outside it
30(No Transcript)
31 32 33Required Functions of PC
- Regardless of stimulus domains used, specific
behavioral properties must be demonstrated in the
presence of the stimulus members to infer that
they are functioning as a class - each stimulus in the set occasions a particular
response stimuli in other sets do not occasion
that same response (Adams et al., 1993 Fields,
Adams, Buffington, Yang, Verhave, 1996
Wasserman DeVolder, 1993). - This emerges after training has occurred with
only a subset of the stimuli in the potential
class (Lea, 1984 Herrnstein, 1990).
34How is this conducted?
- first a response is trained to occur in the
presence of only one or some of the stimuli in
the set - Next, novel stimuli are presented under
extinction conditions in a transfer test to
determine the extent to which control of
responding by the training stimuli transfers to
the novel stimuli - When novel stimuli occasion responding to the
same extent as those stimuli used in training,
then the entire set of stimuli presented during
training and during the transfer test may be
functioning as a class (see next)
35Why may?
- Although class-consistent responding during the
transfer test is necessary to infer that
perceptual classes have been established, it is
not sufficient to do so - Such performances can be explained in two ways
- One explanation is that the stimuli in such sets,
while different physically, may not be
discriminable from each other (psychophysical
limits of the sensory system of the organism).
Thus, the different stimuli in the set may
occasion the same response simply because an
organism is unable to discriminate among the
stimuli within the set, or between the stimuli
used during training and transfer testing.
36Why may?
- Such performances can be explained in two ways
- The other explanation (the one we want to be
true!) is that the stimuli within the set, while
physically similar, are discriminable from each
other and still occasion the same response. - To demonstrate this, need an independent measure
of discriminability - Demonstrating this within-set discriminability
would support the conclusion that perceptual
classes had been established (because responding
would have transferred to discriminable, novel
stimuli). (this test will be explained in a
subsequent slide dealing with training/testing
procedures)
37Training Testing Procedures Simple
Discrimination
- During training, participants are exposed to at
least two different stimuli, each of which is a
member of a potential perceptual class - If only one stimulus is used for each potential
class during discrimination training, the
procedure is referred to as single exemplar
training (Bhatt Wright, 1992 Cerella, 1972). - Thus, if two potential classes were being
established, two stimuli would be used during
training if three potential classes were being
established, three stimuli would be used, etc.
38Training Testing Procedures Simple
Discrimination
- training stimuli can be presented either one at a
time (successive discrimination), or concurrently
(simultaneous discrimination). - Each stimulus in a set serves as a discriminative
stimulus (S) for a particular response. - This response may entail physically contacting
the stimulus or emitting some other behavior such
as a lever press, key peck, or verbal response. - response specific to each stimulus is reinforced
only when it occurs in presence of that
particular stimulus (emitting the response at any
other time is not followed by reinforcement).
39Risley (1964)
- Used single exemplar successive discrimination
procedure to train adults with mental retardation
to press a bar specific to a particular sound
frequency - 1900 cps sound right bar presses
- 600 cps sound left bar presses
- Once the participants were reliably responding
correctly, 16 different tones (range 160-12,000
cps) were presented under extinction conditions. - generalization test measured extent to which left
or right bar presses were maintained in the
presence of the multiple probe stimuli. - resulting generalization gradients showed that
dimension of sound frequency was divided by the
participants into two potential dimensional
classes.
40Training Testing Procedures Simple
Discrimination
- perceptual classes can also be established using
multiple stimuli from each potential class during
training (called multiple exemplar training
(Becker, 1971 Cook et al., 1990 Haring, Breen,
Laitenen, 1989 Homa Little, 1985) or
multiple stimulus discrimination training (Lea
Ryan, 1990). - For example, rather than training a
discrimination between only one picture of a dog
or cat, many pictures of cats and dogs could be
used.
41Herrnstein Loveland (1964)
- successive discrimination procedure with multiple
exemplar training was used to investigate the
demonstration of fuzzy categories with pigeons. - In training, presented over 1200 slides
containing natural settings such as trees,
meadows, towns, and the countryside. - 50 of slides contained a person or part of a
person (in other slides, no person was present).
More -
42Herrnstein Loveland (1964)
- Pigeons were trained to peck a key to gain access
to food when instances of person (S) were
presented on a slide viewing screen - When slides depicting no people (S-) were
presented, pecks were under extinction - Discrimination training continued until the
pigeons likelihood of pecking was systematically
higher in the presence of the person slide
exemplars than the non-person slide exemplars.
More
43Herrnstein Loveland (1964)
- Although the pigeons learned the discrimination
between the sets of stimuli, it cannot be
inferred that perceptual classes had emerged
birds may have simply learned many independent
discriminations in which specific slides were
correlated with reinforcement for responding
while others were not - To assess whether perceptual classes had been
established, need to present novel person and
non-person slides. - this transfer test showed that the birds
systematically responded to the novel person
slides to a greater degree than non-person slides
even though they had never been observed before.
44Bhatt, Wasserman, Reynolds, Knauss (1988)
- used a multiple exemplar successive
discrimination procedure to see if pigeons could
learn four fuzzy categories - pigeons were randomly exposed to 10 exemplars of
four categories consisting of cats, flowers,
people, and cars on a slide viewing screen - For a particular category, each exemplar served
as an S for pecks to a key located in one corner
of a rectangular array - discrimination training continued until the
pigeons reached an asymptote of responding
correctly on about 75 of trials (well above
chance!)
45Bhatt, Wasserman, Reynolds, Knauss (1988)
- Based on the performances during discrimination
training alone, two possible inferences can be
made the pigeons had simply learned many
unrelated discriminations in which particular
slides were correlated with reinforcement for
pecking at a particular location or - pecking had come under the control of the members
of a perceptual class. - Transfer tests were next used to determine which
of these explanations accounted for pigeon
performances.
46Bhatt, Wasserman, Reynolds, Knauss (1988)
- In testing, 10 new slides depicting novel
exemplars were presented for each of the four
stimulus sets - If learning many unrelated discriminations had
occurred, correct responding to the novel
exemplars would not be expected to exceed chance
responding of 25 correct. - transfer test, however, showed that the pigeons
responded correctly to the novel stimuli at
levels significantly greater than chance (but not
as high as the 75 correct during training) - this provides minimal requirements to infer that
fuzzy classes had been established.
47Lea Harrison (1978)
- studied emergence of polymorphous categories with
pigeons - used multiple exemplar successive discrimination
procedure - six potential features could be present in the
stimuli used in training and testing - features were
- 2 different shapes
- 2 brightness levels
- 2 colors
- Three features were used in training for the S
or positive set (green background, circular
figure, or black figure). Another three features
were used for the S- or negative set.
48Lea Harrison (1978)
- features were
- 2 different shapes (1 positive and 1 negative)
- 2 brightness levels (1 positive and 1 negative)
- 2 colors (1 positive and 1 negative)
- Each stimulus contained 3 features
- Positive stimuli (reinforced for pecking) had 2
positive features and 1 negative - Negative stimuli (extinction for pecking) had 2
negative features and 1 positive - Thus, polymorphous rule was 2 of 3 features
(using our m of n notation)
49Lea Harrison (1978)
- During training, pigeons learned discrimination
between the two sets - Because we cannot infer perceptual class has
emerged after training alone, stimuli containing
either all three positive features or all three
negative features were used during testing - Birds were successful during testing
50Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
- establishment of perceptual classes is not
restricted to simple discrimination training
procedures - also used conditional discrimination procedures,
such as matching to sample (Cumming Berryman,
1965) (although used less frequently than simple
discrimination) - Studies of arbitrary class formation (e.g.,
equivalence classes), however, have used
conditional discrimination almost exclusively!
51Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
- To establish two potential perceptual classes
using a matching to sample procedure,
participants are exposed to at least three
stimuli during each trial - first stimulus presented in a trial (called the
sample), is drawn from a potential perceptual
class (For two potential classes, at least two
different sample stimuli would be randomly and
successively presented).
52Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
sample
53Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
- Following presentation of the sample, some
observing response (Wyckoff, 1952) such as
touching the sample, must be emitted. Why? - Next, at least two additional stimuli, called
comparisons, are presented. - One is drawn from the same potential perceptual
class as the sample and is called the positive
comparison - other comparison comes from a different class
and is called the negative comparison
54Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
Sample 1
- comparison
comparison
55Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
Sample 2
- comparison
comparison
56Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
- Note that the behavioral functions of the
comparison stimuli change as a condition of what
the sample is - That is, the negative comparison and positive
comparison swap functions depending on the sample
57Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
- forming perceptual classes using a conditional
discrimination procedure requires that only some
stimuli in a potential class occasion selection
of other stimulus members as a result of direct
training - remaining stimuli must occasion selection of
other set members even though they had never been
paired together before in a conditional
discrimination - To assess this, a match to sample transfer test
containing multiple novel stimuli not used in
training must be presented to assess the
emergence of perceptual classes
58Training Testing Procedures Conditional
Discrimination
- As in simple discrimination training, conditional
discrimination training may involve either single
or multiple exemplar training - In single exemplar training, only one stimulus is
drawn from each potential class (such as in our
circle example). - This stimulus serves as both a sample and a
comparison during conditional discrimination
trials. - When multiple exemplars are used during
conditional discrimination training, many stimuli
drawn from the potential classes serve as samples
and comparisons
59Fields, Reeve, Adams, Brown, Verhave (1997)
- Used conditional discrimination training
procedure to establish two dimensional classes
with college students - Training was conducted with single exemplars
drawn from each of two potential dimensional
classes - Sample stimuli were short or long lines (1- or
25-units long) - two comparison stimuli were the same 1- and
25-unit lines.
60Fields, Reeve, Adams, Brown, Verhave (1997)
Sample 1
comparison
- comparison
61Fields, Reeve, Adams, Brown, Verhave (1997)
- Once training reached 100, students were
presented with samples of intermediate length
lines (varied from 2 to 24 units) - comparisons were identical to those used in
training (1 or 25 24 units). - transfer test determined extent to which
selection responding generalized to the test
stimuli - resulting generalization gradients showed that a
range of short test lines always occasioned the
selection of the short comparison line (likewise,
a range of long test lines always occasioned the
selection of the long comparison)
62Fields, Reeve, Adams, Brown, Verhave (1997)
Test sample
Test comparison
test comparison
63Discriminabilty Measures
- Most researchers investigating the demonstration
of perceptual classes rely on performances
observed during transfer testing to infer that
classes have emerged - While class-consistent performances during
transfer testing may be indicative of the
emergence of perceptual classes, an additional
requirement must be satisfied to make such an
inference - Specifically, some stimuli in the potential
perceptual class must be discriminable from the
other stimuli within the set - If within-set discriminability is demonstrated
following class-consistent performances during a
transfer test, such an extension of class
membership represents the demonstration of a
perceptual class (Cook et al., 1990 Fields et
al., 1997 Lea, 1984 Wasserman, Kiedinger,
Bhatt, 1988).
64Discriminabilty Measures
- Two general procedures have been used to provide
discriminability measures of stimuli that
constitute potential perceptual classes - In one study (Fields et al., 1997), a conditional
discrimination format was used - Recall that in their study a range of short test
lines always occasioned the selection of the
short line used in training. In addition, a range
of long test lines always occasioned the
selection of the long line - If participants could not discriminate the test
lines from those used in training, however,
transfer of responding to the test stimuli would
not be indicative of class formation (Cook et
al., 1990 Fields et al., 1997 Wasserman,
Kiedinger, Bhatt, 1988). - Rather, during testing, control of responding
would simply have been maintained by stimuli that
were not discriminable from the training stimuli.
- If the lines within a set were discriminable,
however, the emergence of dimensional classes can
be inferred.
65Discriminabilty Measures
- To assess this, Fields et al. (1997) reinforced
the selection of a neither response option for
all lines previously used in testing (lines 2-24
units long) - Below is a sample trial with an intermediate
length sample line. Only the selection of the
neither comparison is reinforced.
NEITHER
Line 1
Line 25
66Discriminabilty Measures
- In addition, reinforcement for selecting the
short line used in training (1 unit ling) was
provided only when the identical training line
appeared as a sample. Likewise, reinforcement for
selecting the long line (25 units long) was
provided only if the long training line appeared
as a sample. - In summary, the reinforced trials were
- Sample 1 unit gtgt comparison 1 unit
- Sample 25 units gtgt comparison 25 units
- Sample 2-24 units gtgt neither comparison
- Once such selections were occurring reliably, all
lines were presented as samples under extinction
conditions - generalization gradients indicated that
participants systematically selected the short
line in the presence of the short line, the
neither comparison in the presence of all the
other test lines (2-24), and the long line in the
presence of the long line. - These performances indicated that the students
could discriminate the test lines from the
training lines. - Thus, the inference that dimensional classes had
been established could be made because
selection-based responses transferred to stimuli
that were novel and discriminable from those used
during training.
67Discriminability Measures Pseudoconcepts
- comprised of stimuli that are arbitrary and
heterogeneous (they do not share any common
physical characteristics) (McIlvane, Dube, Green,
Serna, 1993 Stromer, Mackay, 1997) - Used in the following study to determine whether
members of different fuzzy classes were
discriminable from one another
68Wasserman, Kiedinger, Bhatt (1988)
- Used same 40 slides as those used by Bhatt et al.
(1988) in a previous study - Trained pseudoconcepts with pigeons
- Class 1 cat slides 1-10 (peck upper left key)
- Class 2 cat slides 11-20 (peck upper right key)
- Class 3 flower slides 1-10 (peck lower left
key) - Class 4 flower slides 11-20 (peck lower right
key) - Thus, we are artificially partitioning two fuzzy
classes into 4
69Wasserman, Kiedinger, Bhatt (1988)
- Why? If birds cannot form these pseudoconcepts,
then the members WITHIN each fuzzy class (cats or
flowers) are NOT discriminable from each other - Results showed that birds did confuse some
members with each fuzzy category but they were
correct more often than chance would predict
70Effects of Number of Exemplars
- Only a small number of experiments have
investigated how the number of training exemplars
influences the establishment of perceptual
classes - these studies focused exclusively on fuzzy class
formation
71Cook, Wright, Kendrick (1990)
- used pigeons to investigate the demonstration of
fuzzy classes consisting of slides of line
drawings of birds or mammals - Used either 5 or 35 exemplars of each during
training - During generalization test, only birds exposed to
35 exemplars correctly responded to class members - Birds exposed to 5 exemplars responded at chance
levels during testing
72Homa Little (1985)
- Participants were typical 10-year-old children
- effect of number of training exemplars on class
demonstration was assessed on a within-subject
basis - kids first presented with three different
experimenter-defined prototype stimuli that
consisted of computer-generated, 9-sided figures - kids were informed that each belonged to a
particular category (designated by letters A, B,
and C) - Next, successive discrimination training was
conducted with 18 new stimuli consisting of 3, 6,
and 9 exemplars of the three potential fuzzy
categories (exemplars were distortions of the
prototype of each class, generated by changing
the locations of the apices of each figure) - That is, one class used 3 exemplars, one used 6,
and one used 9
73Homa Little (1985)
74Homa Little (1985)
- During discrimination training, children received
feedback for all category selections - training continued until 100 correct
- Following training, transfer test determined
degree of generalization to novel exemplars of
the 3 potential classes - Transfer test used 15 new stimuli from each of
the 3 classes
75Homa Little (1985)
- Test results showed that when only three
exemplars were used during discrimination
training for a particular potential class,
class-consistent responding during the transfer
test was near chance levels (33) - This increased to about 50 for 6 exemplar
training and 75 for the 9 exemplar training
condition - Thus, increases in exemplars increased likelihood
of class formation - Similar findings have been found by others
(Becker, 1971 Bhatt Wright 1992 Engelmann,
Carnine, 1982 Homa Chambliss, 1975 Homa,
Cross, Cornell, Goldman, Swartz, 1973 Homa,
Sterling, Treple, 1981 Omohundro, 1981 Sands,
Delius, 1988)
76Cerella (1979)
- Found that fuzzy classes could be established
with only a single exemplar during training for
pigeons - successive discrimination training was used
- For group 1, 40 slides of different oak leaves
were positive exemplars (Ss for pecking).
Negative exemplars were 40 slides of leaves from
20 other species of trees. - For group 2, a single oak leaf was used as a
positive exemplar (S for pecking) with the same
40 negative exemplars used with group 1.
77Cerella (1979)
- Following discrimination training by pigeons in
both groups, a transfer test was conducted with
40 novel oak leaves plus the 40 negative
exemplars (non-oak) from training - results showed that the pigeons in both groups
correctly discriminated between instances of oak
and non-oak leaves (with no differences across
groups) - But, is it possible that the birds couldnt tell
the difference among the members in the oak
class?...
78Cerella (1979)
- Next, used a successive discrimination procedure
- pecks were now reinforced ONLY in the presence of
the oak leaf slide used in single exemplar
discrimination training - Pecks were NOT reinforced for 40 oak leaf slides
previously used during transfer testing - Despite over 100 training sessions, pigeons could
not discriminate between the single exemplar
slide and the novel oak leaf slides - Sowe really cannot infer that classes had formed
79Why is ME Training Superior for Fuzzy Classes?
- Remember in a fuzzy class, no one exemplar
contains all the necessary and sufficient
features that define membership - In addition, no one stimulus feature is either
necessary or sufficient for membership - Rather, membership in a potential fuzzy class
requires presence of a combination of some of
many features - Each feature occurs with different probabilities
across the class members - For control of responding to be exerted by these
combinations of features, the participant must,
by necessity, be exposed to more than one class
exemplar - As the number of exemplars used during
discrimination increases, there is a greater
probability that the participant will be exposed
to the some of many stimulus features that define
class membership
80Why is ME Training Superior for Fuzzy Classes?
- Thus, with each successive presentation of
different exemplars drawn from a particular
potential class, the some of many features that
define category membership become correlated with
reinforcement for responding - responding in the presence of the some of many
features that define a particular class is
reinforced with a greater frequency than
responding in the presence of irrelevant features - these differential reinforcement histories select
the combinations of features differentiating
positive and negative exemplars of a class
(Balsam, 1988). - Only the presentation of multiple exemplars of
the class during training makes it likely that
class-defining combinations of many features,
albeit ambiguously specified, will come to
control responding
81Why is ME Training Superior for Fuzzy Classes?
- When only a few or a single exemplar are used
during training, however, there is a decreased
probability that a sufficient some number of
class-defining features will be correlated with
reinforcement - presentation of a single exemplar provides an
equal likelihood that both relevant and
irrelevant features will be correlated with
reinforcement for responding - This would be evident in non-differential
responding to both positive and negative
instances of a class during a transfer test - Such performances indicate that responding during
training had simply come under the control of
individual stimuli rather than by the some of
many defining features defining the class
82