Title: MPhil Seminar: Evaluating OT
1MPhil Seminar Evaluating OT
- L1/L2 Acquisition and learnability
- 8-3-2007
2Overview
- Focus on comparison of leading theories (RBP
OT) - What each predicts to be possible and impossible
- How these predictions compare to the data
- General acquisition effects
- SLA effects
3Scientific study of acquisition research
questions
theory
Top-down what does each theory predict to be
possible and impossible?
Bottom-up what are the central acquisition
phenomena that any theory must account for?
data
4Evaluating theories
Is a theory that makes fewer predictions better?
Venn diagram of outputs for grammar 1 and grammar
2
- Grammar/Theory 1
- Lexicon a
- Rules
- a optionally ? b
- Grammar/Theory 2
- Lexicon a
- Rules
- a optionally ? b
- b optionally ? c
a b c
grammar 1
grammar 2
Loci of predictive difference
box range of conceivable outputs for any theory
5Some basic learning issues
- All existing OT learning models (except some
flawed nascent work by Tesar) require being
handed correct URs, SRs, and CON. - How can an OT grammar be learned, given the large
number of constraint rankings that can produce
the limited data set to which a language learner
is exposed? Does UG provide a default ranking of
constraints? - Prince and Smolensky 203 since constraints cant
be inferred from surface data (because theyre
violable), they must be part of UG - Sherrard 1997 and McMahon 2000 in OT 10 (untied)
constraints produce 36 million possible grammars,
posing a learning problem, whereas rules simply
state observable generalisations. - McMahon and Calabrese the first step in learning
the grammar is observing alternations/generalizati
ons. DP stops there, but OT requires an extra
step of going from there to a constraint ranking.
Why is this necessary?
6Typical OT presentation of the learning task
- http//www.ling.rochester.edu/people/cross/acquisi
tion-of-ot.pdf - Prince and Smolensky 1993 All constraints are
universal, occurring in all languages. - Languages only vary with respect to their
lexicons (input forms) and the rankings of the
constraints. - Implication If constraints are innate, you don't
have to learn them. - Constraints that have to be learned anyway
- morpheme constraints
- Leftmost(um) Place the morpheme um- in the
leftmost position possible. (PS 1993). - clitic alignment constraints, à la Anderson 1994.
- parameterisable constraints
- RhythmicTypeIamb/Trochee Feet are
iambic/trochaic. (PS 1993) - conjoined constraints?
- Any two simple constraints can be conjoined to
form a complex constraint. Are all possible
conjunctions of simple constraints universal? Can
you conjoin a previously conjoined constraint?
7Hale and Reiss 1998
- Fact child language production differs from
comprehension - Children understand khæt but produce ta
- Smolensky 1996 Optimality Theory (OT) can
generate this - Hale and Reiss no it cant, and it shouldnt
- (1) The nature of child phonology
- a. The Strong Identity Hypothesis, which holds
that child phonology is governed by the same
principles as adult phonology H/R - b. The view that child phonology is
fundamentally distinct from adult phonologythat
it licenses processes unattested in adult
language, that it depends on a series of
developmental stages, and so on S - (2) The nature of the evidence
- a. Deviations from target formsin childrens as
well as adults grammarsare to be attributed to
performance effects, including nonlinguistic
cognitive and motor processing. H/R - b. Many deviations from target forms are the
result of child phonology (i.e., the childs
phonological competence)grammatical effects for
which the target language provides no evidence.
S
8OT learning algorithms
9Smolenskys scheme
- initial state of grammar M F
- production OT grammar selects most harmonic
output for a given input - comprehension OT grammar selects most harmonic
input for an observed output - correction algorithm when the comprehension
evaluation is less harmonic (i.e. there are more
asterisks) than the production evaluation,
relevant constraints are demoted to make the
comprehension parse more harmonic than the
production parse
10Smolenskys scheme
11Problems with S identified by H/R
- cant account for surface neutralization, e.g.
German /rad/, /rat/ ? rat - Asudeh 20017 OT grammars seem to be
non-trivially non-reversible, whether they use
interpretive parsing or not. This is in conflict
with the reversibility prized by other
constraint-based architectures and is tantamount
to having separate grammars for production and
comprehension, which is conceptually
undesirable. - it isnt possible to have a steady-state grammar
of Smolenskys type, because the correction
algorithm will always kick in immediately and
rerank the constraints - cant account for Captain Hazelwoods drunken
speech (misarticulation of liquids, final
devoicing, deaffrication), which in OT requires
reranking of constraints (see next slide on
performance) - S has severe problems with chain shifts and
opacity effects. - S cant account for higher accuracy in direct
imitation - i. improved performance under concentration
- S predicts simply better pronunciation of s for
- ii. parroting
- S would have to require instantaneous ranking of
Cree constraints!
12competence vs. performance
- Johnson, Pisoni, and Bernacki (1990) report on
the intoxicated speech of the captain of the
Exxon Valdez around the time of the accident at
Prince William Sound, Alaska. (669) - Observed effects
- misarticulation of /r/ and /l/
- final devoicing
- Deaffrication
- Performance problem, not different grammar
- Cf. mimicking Cree sentences doesnt mean that
one has acquired Cree grammar (671) - attempts at producing adult phen pen collected
from a 15-month-old child in a 30-minute period
(Ferguson 1986) - mã?, v?, dedn, hIn, mbõ, pHIn,
tHn?tHn?tHn?, bah, dhau?, buã - Kids who cant speak yet but can accurately
identify subject and object of sentence,
distinguish phonemes, etc. (Hirsh-Pasek and
Golinkoff 1991)
13Hale and Reisss C-P model
14Some problems with OT acquisition theories
identified by Menn 2000
- mushy and non-phonemicized early outputs
- rules that are no longer constraint-based
- U-shaped developmental curves
- the need to differentiate input in the sense of
the adult form the child is trying to
approximate from input in the sense of
abstract strings of concatenated morphemes to be
parsed
15The Gradual Learning Algorithm
- Boersma and Hayes 2001
- The GLA is cast within a stochastic version of
OT that places constraints on a numeric scale
(Boersma 1998). - Each time the grammar is used to evaluate
Input-Output pairings, the ranking values are
perturbed slightly (by evaluation noise) before
converting the values to a ranking order. This
produces variation between the rankings of
constraints whose values are close to one
another. - In the version proposed by Boersma and Hayes
(2001), the values of all constraints preferring
the winner are raised, and the values of those
preferring the loser are lowered, all by an equal
amount termed the plasticity. - The result is that the model is much closer to a
standard stochastic grammar than would at first
appear phonological templates which are
abundantly instantiated in the training set end
up being highly favored by the grammar, and those
which are poorly instantiated end up disfavored.
Thus, the rankings of constraints closely track
the frequency values that would be assigned to
the same constraints in some stochastic
grammars. (Pierrehumbert 2003)
16Problems GLA
- non-convergence (Pater 2005)see later slide
- Pierrehumbert 2003225-6
- requires constraints like k?s to get
morphophonemic rules like velar softening - in fact, one needs to postulate a universal set
of constraints pairing each phoneme with each
other phoneme (i.e. k?s, k?t, k?p) - the price of folding unnatural
morphophonological correspondences into the
phonological grammar is splitting the
correspondences into unrelated constraint pairs,
which are ranked separately. - In the GLA model, all constraints are at the
same level and all constraint rankings are
trained on the same data set. Thus, the
connection drawn above between the granularity of
constraints and the size of the effective
training set does not appear to be available. - Phonetics, in common with many other physical
processes, provides examples of skewed e.g.
gamma distributions relating to physical
nonlinearities and saturationsIn contrast,
distributions arising from repeated independent
decisions (as in coin-flipping, or a forced
choice experiment) tend to be Gaussian. Since the
assumption of Gaussian distributions is critical
to the mathematical tractability of GLA, the
existence of non-Gaussian distributions appears
to be problematic. - The GLA model also does not distinguish effects
relating to type frequency from effects relating
to surface, or token, frequency. - It also provides no way to downweight the
grammatical impact of extremely frequent words,
as Bybee (2001) and Bailey and Hahn (2001) show
to be necessary.
17The Constraint Demotion Algorithm
- Tesar and Smolensky 1993 version, as reported by
Crosswhite and Keer
18Problems CDA
- learning of unnatural patterns
- cant deal with variation (Tesar and Smolensky
1998, Boersma and Hayes 2001) - see later slide
19Learning unnatural patterns
- CDA predicts that natural patterns will be easier
to learn than unnatural ones, because they
require fewer departures from UG ranking. - Support from Paters 2005 study?
- On the other hand, if learning involves
formulating rules and their difficulty is
computed on the basis of their formal structure,
then unnatural patterns should be equally
learnable. - Supported in studies by Pycha et al. 2003,
Morrison 2005, and Seidl Buckley (forthcoming)
20Variation is a problem for constraint demotion
algorithms
- inconsistent data, such as variation in the
ambient language, causes RCD to choke - McCarthy 2002204-5
- Cf. Hayes 2000
- Intra-individual variability in L2 production
- Tropf 1987174multiple renditions of German
nicht not in a one-hour session with a Spanish
speaker learning German nIS 31, nI 25, nISt 4,
nIs 4, nIk 4, nEt 2, nIC 2, nI?t 1, nIZ 1 - Similar findings in L1 acq
- Hayes alternative (strictness bands) predicts
that only adjacent constraints can be involved in
optionality - Problem Pierrehumberts hovacity data
21Non-convergence for GLA in complex credit
scenarios (Pater 2005)
- The GLA will succeed on the learning problem
presented in the last section if it is run in
demotion only mode, in which it only decreases
the values of loser preferring constraints, and
does not change the value of winner preferring
constraints. However, as Boersma (1997, 1998)
shows, this version of the GLA fails to converge
in cases involving variation.
22Variation in CDA
23Second Language Acquisition
24OT on SLA
- We assume that second language acquisition
involves creation of a new grammar, using the
same resources as first language acquisition
(though other cognitive strategies may be used as
well). One major difference, however, is that the
initial state of second language acquisition is
the final state of first language acquisition
(Pater and Tessier 2005)
25Predictions of canonical OT
- M/F-based learning
- No opacity, derived environment, or avoidance
effects that dont appear in L1 or L2 - wouldnt make sense to spontaneously invoke
constraint conjunction, Null Parse, sympathy
constraints, etc. - Consistency
- Consistent cross-linguistic treatment of a given
phenomenon, e.g. resolution of theta - same constraints as characterize natural
languages, so D/_ will always be dealt with via
devoicing, etc. - No levels
- no level-based effects, since there are no levels
- unnatural processes will not be imported into L2,
because they are morphologically conditioned
(according to Lombardi, Steriade, etc.) - Markedness
- IL effects will result from either hidden UG
rankings in L1, or from intermediate degrees of
constraint demotion/promotion NOT from reversion
to UG rankings when already superseded by L1
rankings - markedness-based changes will conform to
universal markedness hierarchy - neutralizations will be in direction of unmarked
member of opposition - Natural/unmarked patterns will be easier to learn
26Some central SLP phenomena
- Contra M/F-based learning
- Nonderived environment blocking
- Opacity
- Avoidance
- Contra Consistency
- Optionality and variation
- Convention vs. automaticity
- Final devoicing
- Contra No levels
- Level-based interference
- Unnatural interference
- Contra Markedness
- Cases where IL phenomenon ? NL, TL
- Unnatural patterns not harder to learn
27Nonderived Environment Blocking
- Eckman and Iverson 1995 et seqq.
- Suppression of s-palatalization in Korean acq of
English - Suppression of spirantization in Spanish acq of
English - Kiparsky and Menn 198747derived environment
effect in acq of Greek - Polish devoicing and raising with loanwords
- snop (not snup) but pagoda ? pagut, toga ? tuk
- Standard OT treatment of NDEB constraint
conjunction (Lubowicz 1999) - Smolensky only postulate CC as warranted by PLD
- DEC is problem for
- OT claim that grammars only differ in constraint
ranking - OTs rejection of generalizations/rulesDEC in
SLA is clearly a generalization kicking in, not a
constraint conjunction spontaneously appearing
28Opacity in SLP
- Counterfeeding chain shift substitution
- Cho and Lee 2001, Idsardi 2002 on opacity in
Korean acq of English - sin ? sjin thin ? sin
- same is found in L1 (Dinnsen, OConnor, and
Gierut 2001) - opaque substitution contrast maintenance
ordering, not sympathy, turbidity, targeted
constraints, etc. - Smolensky only postulate constraint conjunction
as warranted by PLD - Idsardi 2002 this spontaneous chain shiftdoes
not reflect properties of their original L1
grammar, the target L2 grammar, or of Universal
Grammaronly by employing persistent rules can we
correctly create the conditions for chain-shift
persistence of constraints and constraint
rankings into the L2 does not correctly induce
the chain-shifting behavior - Counterbleeding repairs
- Weinberger 1987412Mandarin learners of English
who apply final epenthesis before final C-cluster
simplification, e.g. ? aen? - Counterfeeding and counterbleeding in toy L2 acq
- already discussed earlier in course
29Avoidance
- speakers sometimes avoid complex L2
configurations even if their L1 has them - Laufer and Eliasson 37, Jordens 1977, Kellerman
1977, 1978, 1986 - Celce-Murcia 1977
- child learning English and French simultaneously
avoided words containing fricatives in one
language by using the word from the other
language, e.g. couteau for knife - Well-documented in L1 phonological acquisition
and disorders also - cleft palate speech, lisp
- Standard OT treatment of avoidance Null Parse
- Wrongly predicts phonologically empty output,
rather than contentful output that crashes - see Orgun and Sprouse 1999, Nevins and Vaux 2004
for further discussion and exemplification
30Ambiguity and animal wug tests
Gallistel, C. 2003. Conditioning from an
information processing perspective. Behavioural
Processes 61.31234 1-13.
31Consistency
- Lombardi 2003 repair of L2 ?, ð predictable
from structure of L1 system - /s, z/ Japanese, France French, German
- /t, d/ Russian, Quebecois, Hungarian, Sinhalese
- Cf. Ritchie 1968, Nemser 1971, Hancin-Bhatt 1994,
Weinberger 1997) - Actual facts intra-lingual/individual variation
in L1 and L2 acq - multiple L1 substitutes for unfamiliar/difficult
L2 sound (Hammarberg 1990) - Polish replacement of ?, ð by s, z t, d or
f, v (Gussmann 198431) - Japanese, German, and Turkish speakers vary in
what they hear ?, ð as (Hancin-Bhatt 1994) - Calabrese uses f/v, whereas other Italians use
t/d (Flege, Munro, and MacKay 1996) (cf. Cockney
vs. New York Archibald 1998102) - Unschooled French speakers use t (Berger 1951)
beginners use f, intermediate learners use t
(Wenk 1979) Quebecois use t, France uses s
(Archibald 1998102) - Korean variation between tense s' and tense
t' (think ssink ttink) (Oh 2002) - Austrian learners of English vary between f s
dental s lenisized ? (Wieden 1997232) - Cf. L1
- English L1 acq ? ? f in stages IV-VII ð ? d
or v in stages V-VII (Wenk 1979, Grunwell 1982) - ? ? f s, e.g. Susies think ? sink fink
(Vihman and Greenlee 1987) - Conclusion ambiguity resolved by arbitrary
choice (convention)
32Dealing with coda voice
- Overview
- IL final devoicing as TETU?
- L1 vs. L2 strategies for dealing with coda
voice - Sources and mechanisms of devoicing
33Devoicing as TETU?
- From Uffmann 2004
- 2 guiding principles
- Initial state L1 ranking
- L2 learners may also assume default M F
- Ranking for lgs that dont allow codas
- Coda/voi, Coda Faith
- Ranking for lgs that allow contrastive voicing in
codas - Faith Coda/voi, Coda
- Demotion of Coda below Faith based on TL
evidence - Coda/voi Faith Coda
- NB requires ignoring evidence for voiced codas
- To get Hungarian-English phenomenon (IL devoicing
despite both NL and TL having voice contrast in
codas), Uffmann proposes L2 learners assume
default M F until they receive evidence to the
contrary - Problem contravenes OT assumption that L2
learners start with L1 ranking (Pater 2005, etc.)
34L1 vs. L2 strategies for coda voice
- L1 claimed to only use devoicing
- The too many solutions problem (Lombardi 1995,
Kager 1999, Steriade 2001, McCarthy 2002) - Lombardi MaxOns, Lar, MaxVoi
- Steriade P-map
- McCarthy 2002 targeted constraints can get
deletion repair, and therefore shouldnt be part
of OT - Kiparsky 2004 blocking also used (Konni, Meccan
Arabic) - L2 epenthesis and deletion attested
- Deletion
- Chinese Anderson 1983, Xu 2004
- Epenthesis
- Brazilian Portuguese Major 1987 Korean Kang
2003, Iverson and Lee 2004 Vietnamese Nguyen
and Ingram 2004 Chinese Eckman 1981, Xu 2004 - Also found in L1 acq (Major 2001)
35More problems with OT analysis of final devoicing
- L1 prediction doesnt fall out nicely from
constraint inventory requires conspiracy of
several constraints - Coda/voi-less system (Lombardi 2000, Beckman
2004) misses key articulatory motivation by
avoiding (cf. Smith et al. 2005) - Lombardis scheme should hold for NC as well, but
doesnt (Myers 2002) - Lombardi cant get word-initial neutralization,
like in English and Lac Simon - L1 prediction falsified by L2 data
- Not clear why devoicing should be easier to learn
than contrast - L2 devoicing may not be emergent UG effect
- English has devoicing (Haggard 1978, Ladefoged
1982, Pierrehumbert and Talkin 1992, Smith 1997) - May be articulatory phonetic problem
- L2 speakersmay not have developed adequate
voicing control abilities (Smith et al. 2005) - English-Hungarian case requires reversion to UG
ranking, not hidden rankings
36Levels
- Only low-level automatic L1 rules can cause
interference (Linell 1979, Rubach 1980, 1984,
Broselow 1984, Singh and Ford 1987, Eckman and
Iverson 1995) - Morphologically-conditioned processes do not
cause interference - Problem 1 incoherent in monostratal model
- Problem 2 interference from processes treated as
morphologically-conditioned in OT (McCarthy 1997,
Steriade 2001, Picard 2002, Lombardi 2003) - r-insertion in RP pronunciation of L2 French,
German, Spanish (Wells 1982) - Jétais déjàr ici, ich bin jar fertig,
vivar España - n-insertion in Korean (H. Kim 1999)
- lukenñu?selph look at yourself
37Cases where IL phenomenon ? NL, TL
- Cases where hidden rankings arent involved
- Idsardis 2002 spontaneous chain shift
- Hungarian-English final devoicing (?)
- Japanese antepenultimate mora accentuation (?)
- Hungarian gemination after stressed vowel (?)
- Cases where novel marked configurations are
produced, violating markedness hierarchy - Russian c
- Odd neutralizations in L1 acq
- Child with dental click for all coronal
continuants (Bedore et al. 1994) - Child with ingressives for all postvocalic stops
(Gierut and Champion 2000) - 48 subject, Jason, produces pfw to represent
nearly all word-initial liquid clusters, as well
as initial labial fricatives (Edwards 1996153) - Amahls word-initial engma and voiceless
sonorants (Smith 19734) - child with s in onset, x in coda (Gierut 1993)
- L2 Yuchuns production of ü for i in English
UG, phonology, etc. - NB she doesnt just do it after j
38Conclusions
- Classic OT predictions disconfirmed by
acquisition data - contra M/F-based learning NDEB, opacity,
avoidance - contra Consistency variation, convention
- contra No levels Level-based interference,
Unnatural interference - contra Markedness Cases where IL phenomenon ?
NL, TL Unnatural patterns not harder to learn - Universal markedness/UG plays a role in SLA
- final devoicing, cluster simplification
- Not well captured in OT SLA requires reference
to UG system overridden by L1 and to phenomena
not in L1/L2/UG-CON - Conversely, language allows a broader range of
possibilities than is countenanced in OT - strategies to deal with devoicing, unnatural
rules - Acquisition is generalization-driven and
potentially variable - supported by DEC effects, conventionalized
segmental substitutions, which V to delete in
hiatus, deneutralization - Cf. Kiparsky and Menns 1977 model, which
involves active hypothesis formation and testing
on the part of the child, and Fey and Gandour
1979, vs. Stampes and OTs, which are closer to
behaviorist stimulus-response - For variation, cf. Macken and Fergusons flexible
learning model that allows for variation,
contrary to earlier deterministic models that had
predictable L1?L2 transfer
39References
- Bedore, L., J. Leonard, and J. Gandour. 1994. The
substitution of a click for sibilants A case
study. Clinical Linguistics Phonetics
8283-293. - Broselow, Ellen, S.-I. Chen, C. Wang. 1998. The
emergence of the unmarked in second language
phonology. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 20, 261-280. - Broselow, Ellen. 2004. Unmarked structures and
emergent rankings in second language phonology.
In Lleó, C., Vogel, I. (Eds.), On the
acquisition of second language phonology Special
issue. International Journal of Bilingualism
8.151-65. - Cebrian, J. 2002. Phonetic Similarity,
Syllabification and Phonotactic Constraints in
the Acquisition of a Second Language Contrast.
Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto. - Celce-Murcia, M. 1977. Phonological factors in
vocabulary acquisition a case study of a
two-year-old English-French bilingual. Working
Papers in Bilingualism 1327-41. - Cho, Mi-Hui and Shinsook Lee. 2001. Phonological
transparency and opacity in the sound
substitution of interlanguages. Studies in
Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology. 7.2. 449-468 - Connelly, Mark (1994) Phonological Markedness
and Second Language Error Interpretation.
Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University. - Davidson, Lisa. 2002. The effects of hidden
rankings on the acquisition of consonant
clusters. In A. James J. Leather (Eds.), New
Sounds 2000 (pp. 87-96). Klagenfurt, Austria
University of Klagenfurt. - Dell, G. S., Reed, K. D., Adams D. R. and Meyer,
A.S. 2000. Speech errors, phonotactic
constraints, and implicit learning A study of
the role of experience in language production.
Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 26 1355-1367. - Dinnsen, Daniel, Kathleen OConnor, and Judith
Gierut. 2001. The puzzle-puddle-pickle problem
and the Duke-of-York gambit in acquisition.
Journal of Linguistics 37. - Dziubalska-Kolaczyk, Katarzyna. 1988. How do
Poles perform English tips of the slung? In
Papers and studies in contrastive linguistics
22179-86. - Eckman, Fred Iverson, G. K. (1994),
Pronunciation difficulties in ESL coda
consonants in English interlanguage In M. Yavas
(Ed) First and second language phonology.
(pp.251-265). San Diego Singular Publishing
Group. - Eckman, Fred Iverson, G. K. (1997). Structure
preservation in interlanguage phonology. In S.
J. Hannahs, M. Scholten (Eds.), Focus on
phonological acquisition (pp. 183-207).
Amsterdam John Benjamins. - Eckman, Fred, A. Elreyes, Greg Iverson. 2003.
Some Principles of Second Language Phonology.
Second Language Research 19169-208. - Eckman, Fred. (1987b) The reduction of word-final
consonants in interlanguage. In. A. James L.
Leather (Eds.) Sound patterns in second language
acquisition (pp. 143-162). Dordrecht Foris. - Gierut, Judith and Annette Hust Champion. 2000.
Ingressive substitutions typical or atypical
phonological pattern? - Grijzenhout, Janet and Bertus van Rooy. 200x.
Second language phonology acquisition through
gradual constraint demotion. - Grunwell, P. 1982. Clinical phonology. London
Croom Helm. - Gussmann, Edmund. 1984. Contrastive analysis,
substantive evidence and the abstractness issue.
In Theoretical issues in contrastive phonology,
S. Elliason (ed), 27-36. Heidelberg Julius Groos
Verlag.
40References
- Kiparsky, Paul. 1977. On the Acquisition of
Phonology (with L. Menn). In J. Macnamara (ed.),
Language Learning and Thought, Academic Press,
1977. Reprinted in Georgette Ioup and S. W.
Weinberger (eds.), Interlanguage Phonology the
Acquisition of a Second Language Sound System,
Cambridge, Mass. Newbury House, 1987. - Kiparsky, Paul. 2004. Universals constrain
change, change results in typological
generalizations. - Lombardi, Linda. 2001. Why Place and Voice are
Different Constraint-Specific Alternations in
Optimality Theory. In Lombardi, Linda, ed. (2001)
Segmental Phonology in Optimality Theory
Constraints and Representations. Cambridge
Cambridge University Press. - Lombardi, Linda. 2003. Second language data and
constraints on manner Explaining substitutions
for the English interdentals. Second Language
Research 19225-250. - Lubowicz, Anna. 1999. Derived environment effects
in OT. The Proceedings of the Seventeenth West
Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, ed. by
Kimary Shahin, Susan Blake and Eun Sook Kim,
451-65. Stanford, CA Center Study Language
Information. Rutgers Optimality Archive 239 - Major, Roy. 1987. A model for interlanguage
phonology. In G. Ioup S. Weinberger.(Eds.)
Interlanguage Phonology (pp 101 - 124). Rowley,
MA Newbury House Publishers. - Morrison, G. S. (2005, May). Phonetic naturalness
and phonological learnability. Paper presented at
The 13th Manchester Phonology Meeting (mfm 13),
Manchester, UK. - Nguyen, Thu and John Ingram. 2004. A corpus-based
analysis of transfer effects and connected speech
processes in Vietnamese English. Proceedings of
the 10th Australian International Conference on
Speech Science Technology, Macquarie
University. - Pater, Joe and Anne-Michelle Tessier. 2005.
Phonotactics and alternations Testing the
connection with artificial language learning. In
Kathryn Flack and Shigeto Kawahara (eds.),
University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in
Linguistics 311-16. - Pierrehumbert 2003, in Bod, Rens, Jennifer Hay
and Stefanie Jannedy, eds. 2003. Probabilistic
Linguistics. Cambridge, MA MIT Press. - Pycha, Anne, Pawel Nowak, Eurie Shin, and Ryan
Shosted. 2003. Phonological Rule-Learning and Its
Implications for a Theory of Vowel Harmony. WCCFL
22 Proceedings, ed. M. Tsujimura and G. Garding,
pp. 101-114. Somerville, MA Cascadilla Press. - Rubach, J. 1984. Rule typology and phonological
interference. In Theoretical issues in
contrastive phonology, S. Elliason (ed), 37-50.
Heidelberg, Julius Groos Verlag. - Seidl, A. and E. Buckley. To appear. On the
learning of arbitrary phonological rules. In
Language Learning and Development. - Singh, Rajendra and Alan Ford. Interphonology and
phonological theory. In Sound patterns in second
language acquisition, James and Leather, eds.,
163-172. Dordrecht Foris. - Smith, Bruce, Darcie DeMille Amy Roberts, Ann
Bradlow Tessa Bent. 2005. Devoicing in
word-final voiced stop targets produced by native
and nonnative speakers of English.
http//www.sfu.ca/spchlab/A54.pdf - Smith, Caroline. 1997. The devoicing of /z/ in
American English effects of local and prosodic
context. Journal of Phonetics 25.4471-500. - Stark, J. 1974. Aphasiological evidence for the
abstract analysis of the German velar nasal.
Wiener linguistische Gazette 721-37. - Tarone, E. 1976. Some influences on interlanguage
phonology. Working Papers in Bilingualism
887-111. - Tarone, E. 1988. Variation in interlanguage.
London Edward Arnold.