MPhil Seminar: Evaluating OT - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 40
About This Presentation
Title:

MPhil Seminar: Evaluating OT

Description:

Focus on comparison of leading theories (RBP : OT) What each predicts to be possible ... IL effects will result from either hidden UG rankings in L1, or from ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:235
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 41
Provided by: bert61
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: MPhil Seminar: Evaluating OT


1
MPhil Seminar Evaluating OT
  • L1/L2 Acquisition and learnability
  • 8-3-2007

2
Overview
  • Focus on comparison of leading theories (RBP
    OT)
  • What each predicts to be possible and impossible
  • How these predictions compare to the data
  • General acquisition effects
  • SLA effects

3
Scientific study of acquisition research
questions
theory
Top-down what does each theory predict to be
possible and impossible?
Bottom-up what are the central acquisition
phenomena that any theory must account for?
data
4
Evaluating theories
Is a theory that makes fewer predictions better?
Venn diagram of outputs for grammar 1 and grammar
2
  • Grammar/Theory 1
  • Lexicon a
  • Rules
  • a optionally ? b
  • Grammar/Theory 2
  • Lexicon a
  • Rules
  • a optionally ? b
  • b optionally ? c

a b c
grammar 1
grammar 2
Loci of predictive difference
box range of conceivable outputs for any theory
5
Some basic learning issues
  • All existing OT learning models (except some
    flawed nascent work by Tesar) require being
    handed correct URs, SRs, and CON.
  • How can an OT grammar be learned, given the large
    number of constraint rankings that can produce
    the limited data set to which a language learner
    is exposed? Does UG provide a default ranking of
    constraints?
  • Prince and Smolensky 203 since constraints cant
    be inferred from surface data (because theyre
    violable), they must be part of UG
  • Sherrard 1997 and McMahon 2000 in OT 10 (untied)
    constraints produce 36 million possible grammars,
    posing a learning problem, whereas rules simply
    state observable generalisations.
  • McMahon and Calabrese the first step in learning
    the grammar is observing alternations/generalizati
    ons. DP stops there, but OT requires an extra
    step of going from there to a constraint ranking.
    Why is this necessary?

6
Typical OT presentation of the learning task
  • http//www.ling.rochester.edu/people/cross/acquisi
    tion-of-ot.pdf
  • Prince and Smolensky 1993 All constraints are
    universal, occurring in all languages.
  • Languages only vary with respect to their
    lexicons (input forms) and the rankings of the
    constraints.
  • Implication If constraints are innate, you don't
    have to learn them.
  • Constraints that have to be learned anyway
  • morpheme constraints
  • Leftmost(um) Place the morpheme um- in the
    leftmost position possible. (PS 1993).
  • clitic alignment constraints, à la Anderson 1994.
  • parameterisable constraints
  • RhythmicTypeIamb/Trochee Feet are
    iambic/trochaic. (PS 1993)
  • conjoined constraints?
  • Any two simple constraints can be conjoined to
    form a complex constraint. Are all possible
    conjunctions of simple constraints universal? Can
    you conjoin a previously conjoined constraint?

7
Hale and Reiss 1998
  • Fact child language production differs from
    comprehension
  • Children understand khæt but produce ta
  • Smolensky 1996 Optimality Theory (OT) can
    generate this
  • Hale and Reiss no it cant, and it shouldnt
  • (1) The nature of child phonology
  • a. The Strong Identity Hypothesis, which holds
    that child phonology is governed by the same
    principles as adult phonology H/R
  • b. The view that child phonology is
    fundamentally distinct from adult phonologythat
    it licenses processes unattested in adult
    language, that it depends on a series of
    developmental stages, and so on S
  • (2) The nature of the evidence
  • a. Deviations from target formsin childrens as
    well as adults grammarsare to be attributed to
    performance effects, including nonlinguistic
    cognitive and motor processing. H/R
  • b. Many deviations from target forms are the
    result of child phonology (i.e., the childs
    phonological competence)grammatical effects for
    which the target language provides no evidence.
    S

8
OT learning algorithms
  • M F
  • GLA
  • CDA

9
Smolenskys scheme
  • initial state of grammar M F
  • production OT grammar selects most harmonic
    output for a given input
  • comprehension OT grammar selects most harmonic
    input for an observed output
  • correction algorithm when the comprehension
    evaluation is less harmonic (i.e. there are more
    asterisks) than the production evaluation,
    relevant constraints are demoted to make the
    comprehension parse more harmonic than the
    production parse

10
Smolenskys scheme
11
Problems with S identified by H/R
  • cant account for surface neutralization, e.g.
    German /rad/, /rat/ ? rat
  • Asudeh 20017 OT grammars seem to be
    non-trivially non-reversible, whether they use
    interpretive parsing or not. This is in conflict
    with the reversibility prized by other
    constraint-based architectures and is tantamount
    to having separate grammars for production and
    comprehension, which is conceptually
    undesirable.
  • it isnt possible to have a steady-state grammar
    of Smolenskys type, because the correction
    algorithm will always kick in immediately and
    rerank the constraints
  • cant account for Captain Hazelwoods drunken
    speech (misarticulation of liquids, final
    devoicing, deaffrication), which in OT requires
    reranking of constraints (see next slide on
    performance)
  • S has severe problems with chain shifts and
    opacity effects.
  • S cant account for higher accuracy in direct
    imitation
  • i. improved performance under concentration
  • S predicts simply better pronunciation of s for
  • ii. parroting
  • S would have to require instantaneous ranking of
    Cree constraints!

12
competence vs. performance
  • Johnson, Pisoni, and Bernacki (1990) report on
    the intoxicated speech of the captain of the
    Exxon Valdez around the time of the accident at
    Prince William Sound, Alaska. (669)
  • Observed effects
  • misarticulation of /r/ and /l/
  • final devoicing
  • Deaffrication
  • Performance problem, not different grammar
  • Cf. mimicking Cree sentences doesnt mean that
    one has acquired Cree grammar (671)
  • attempts at producing adult phen pen collected
    from a 15-month-old child in a 30-minute period
    (Ferguson 1986)
  • mã?, v?, dedn, hIn, mbõ, pHIn,
    tHn?tHn?tHn?, bah, dhau?, buã
  • Kids who cant speak yet but can accurately
    identify subject and object of sentence,
    distinguish phonemes, etc. (Hirsh-Pasek and
    Golinkoff 1991)

13
Hale and Reisss C-P model
14
Some problems with OT acquisition theories
identified by Menn 2000
  • mushy and non-phonemicized early outputs
  • rules that are no longer constraint-based
  • U-shaped developmental curves
  • the need to differentiate input in the sense of
    the adult form the child is trying to
    approximate from input in the sense of
    abstract strings of concatenated morphemes to be
    parsed

15
The Gradual Learning Algorithm
  • Boersma and Hayes 2001
  • The GLA is cast within a stochastic version of
    OT that places constraints on a numeric scale
    (Boersma 1998).
  • Each time the grammar is used to evaluate
    Input-Output pairings, the ranking values are
    perturbed slightly (by evaluation noise) before
    converting the values to a ranking order. This
    produces variation between the rankings of
    constraints whose values are close to one
    another.
  • In the version proposed by Boersma and Hayes
    (2001), the values of all constraints preferring
    the winner are raised, and the values of those
    preferring the loser are lowered, all by an equal
    amount termed the plasticity.
  • The result is that the model is much closer to a
    standard stochastic grammar than would at first
    appear phonological templates which are
    abundantly instantiated in the training set end
    up being highly favored by the grammar, and those
    which are poorly instantiated end up disfavored.
    Thus, the rankings of constraints closely track
    the frequency values that would be assigned to
    the same constraints in some stochastic
    grammars. (Pierrehumbert 2003)

16
Problems GLA
  • non-convergence (Pater 2005)see later slide
  • Pierrehumbert 2003225-6
  • requires constraints like k?s to get
    morphophonemic rules like velar softening
  • in fact, one needs to postulate a universal set
    of constraints pairing each phoneme with each
    other phoneme (i.e. k?s, k?t, k?p)
  • the price of folding unnatural
    morphophonological correspondences into the
    phonological grammar is splitting the
    correspondences into unrelated constraint pairs,
    which are ranked separately.
  • In the GLA model, all constraints are at the
    same level and all constraint rankings are
    trained on the same data set. Thus, the
    connection drawn above between the granularity of
    constraints and the size of the effective
    training set does not appear to be available.
  • Phonetics, in common with many other physical
    processes, provides examples of skewed e.g.
    gamma distributions relating to physical
    nonlinearities and saturationsIn contrast,
    distributions arising from repeated independent
    decisions (as in coin-flipping, or a forced
    choice experiment) tend to be Gaussian. Since the
    assumption of Gaussian distributions is critical
    to the mathematical tractability of GLA, the
    existence of non-Gaussian distributions appears
    to be problematic.
  • The GLA model also does not distinguish effects
    relating to type frequency from effects relating
    to surface, or token, frequency.
  • It also provides no way to downweight the
    grammatical impact of extremely frequent words,
    as Bybee (2001) and Bailey and Hahn (2001) show
    to be necessary.

17
The Constraint Demotion Algorithm
  • Tesar and Smolensky 1993 version, as reported by
    Crosswhite and Keer

18
Problems CDA
  • learning of unnatural patterns
  • cant deal with variation (Tesar and Smolensky
    1998, Boersma and Hayes 2001)
  • see later slide

19
Learning unnatural patterns
  • CDA predicts that natural patterns will be easier
    to learn than unnatural ones, because they
    require fewer departures from UG ranking.
  • Support from Paters 2005 study?
  • On the other hand, if learning involves
    formulating rules and their difficulty is
    computed on the basis of their formal structure,
    then unnatural patterns should be equally
    learnable.
  • Supported in studies by Pycha et al. 2003,
    Morrison 2005, and Seidl Buckley (forthcoming)

20
Variation is a problem for constraint demotion
algorithms
  • inconsistent data, such as variation in the
    ambient language, causes RCD to choke
  • McCarthy 2002204-5
  • Cf. Hayes 2000
  • Intra-individual variability in L2 production
  • Tropf 1987174multiple renditions of German
    nicht not in a one-hour session with a Spanish
    speaker learning German nIS 31, nI 25, nISt 4,
    nIs 4, nIk 4, nEt 2, nIC 2, nI?t 1, nIZ 1
  • Similar findings in L1 acq
  • Hayes alternative (strictness bands) predicts
    that only adjacent constraints can be involved in
    optionality
  • Problem Pierrehumberts hovacity data

21
Non-convergence for GLA in complex credit
scenarios (Pater 2005)
  • The GLA will succeed on the learning problem
    presented in the last section if it is run in
    demotion only mode, in which it only decreases
    the values of loser preferring constraints, and
    does not change the value of winner preferring
    constraints. However, as Boersma (1997, 1998)
    shows, this version of the GLA fails to converge
    in cases involving variation.

22
Variation in CDA
  • from Pater 2005

23
Second Language Acquisition
24
OT on SLA
  • We assume that second language acquisition
    involves creation of a new grammar, using the
    same resources as first language acquisition
    (though other cognitive strategies may be used as
    well). One major difference, however, is that the
    initial state of second language acquisition is
    the final state of first language acquisition
    (Pater and Tessier 2005)

25
Predictions of canonical OT
  • M/F-based learning
  • No opacity, derived environment, or avoidance
    effects that dont appear in L1 or L2
  • wouldnt make sense to spontaneously invoke
    constraint conjunction, Null Parse, sympathy
    constraints, etc.
  • Consistency
  • Consistent cross-linguistic treatment of a given
    phenomenon, e.g. resolution of theta
  • same constraints as characterize natural
    languages, so D/_ will always be dealt with via
    devoicing, etc.
  • No levels
  • no level-based effects, since there are no levels
  • unnatural processes will not be imported into L2,
    because they are morphologically conditioned
    (according to Lombardi, Steriade, etc.)
  • Markedness
  • IL effects will result from either hidden UG
    rankings in L1, or from intermediate degrees of
    constraint demotion/promotion NOT from reversion
    to UG rankings when already superseded by L1
    rankings
  • markedness-based changes will conform to
    universal markedness hierarchy
  • neutralizations will be in direction of unmarked
    member of opposition
  • Natural/unmarked patterns will be easier to learn

26
Some central SLP phenomena
  • Contra M/F-based learning
  • Nonderived environment blocking
  • Opacity
  • Avoidance
  • Contra Consistency
  • Optionality and variation
  • Convention vs. automaticity
  • Final devoicing
  • Contra No levels
  • Level-based interference
  • Unnatural interference
  • Contra Markedness
  • Cases where IL phenomenon ? NL, TL
  • Unnatural patterns not harder to learn

27
Nonderived Environment Blocking
  • Eckman and Iverson 1995 et seqq.
  • Suppression of s-palatalization in Korean acq of
    English
  • Suppression of spirantization in Spanish acq of
    English
  • Kiparsky and Menn 198747derived environment
    effect in acq of Greek
  • Polish devoicing and raising with loanwords
  • snop (not snup) but pagoda ? pagut, toga ? tuk
  • Standard OT treatment of NDEB constraint
    conjunction (Lubowicz 1999)
  • Smolensky only postulate CC as warranted by PLD
  • DEC is problem for
  • OT claim that grammars only differ in constraint
    ranking
  • OTs rejection of generalizations/rulesDEC in
    SLA is clearly a generalization kicking in, not a
    constraint conjunction spontaneously appearing

28
Opacity in SLP
  • Counterfeeding chain shift substitution
  • Cho and Lee 2001, Idsardi 2002 on opacity in
    Korean acq of English
  • sin ? sjin thin ? sin
  • same is found in L1 (Dinnsen, OConnor, and
    Gierut 2001)
  • opaque substitution contrast maintenance
    ordering, not sympathy, turbidity, targeted
    constraints, etc.
  • Smolensky only postulate constraint conjunction
    as warranted by PLD
  • Idsardi 2002 this spontaneous chain shiftdoes
    not reflect properties of their original L1
    grammar, the target L2 grammar, or of Universal
    Grammaronly by employing persistent rules can we
    correctly create the conditions for chain-shift
    persistence of constraints and constraint
    rankings into the L2 does not correctly induce
    the chain-shifting behavior
  • Counterbleeding repairs
  • Weinberger 1987412Mandarin learners of English
    who apply final epenthesis before final C-cluster
    simplification, e.g. ? aen?
  • Counterfeeding and counterbleeding in toy L2 acq
  • already discussed earlier in course

29
Avoidance
  • speakers sometimes avoid complex L2
    configurations even if their L1 has them
  • Laufer and Eliasson 37, Jordens 1977, Kellerman
    1977, 1978, 1986
  • Celce-Murcia 1977
  • child learning English and French simultaneously
    avoided words containing fricatives in one
    language by using the word from the other
    language, e.g. couteau for knife
  • Well-documented in L1 phonological acquisition
    and disorders also
  • cleft palate speech, lisp
  • Standard OT treatment of avoidance Null Parse
  • Wrongly predicts phonologically empty output,
    rather than contentful output that crashes
  • see Orgun and Sprouse 1999, Nevins and Vaux 2004
    for further discussion and exemplification

30
Ambiguity and animal wug tests
Gallistel, C. 2003. Conditioning from an
information processing perspective. Behavioural
Processes 61.31234 1-13.
31
Consistency
  • Lombardi 2003 repair of L2 ?, ð predictable
    from structure of L1 system
  • /s, z/ Japanese, France French, German
  • /t, d/ Russian, Quebecois, Hungarian, Sinhalese
  • Cf. Ritchie 1968, Nemser 1971, Hancin-Bhatt 1994,
    Weinberger 1997)
  • Actual facts intra-lingual/individual variation
    in L1 and L2 acq
  • multiple L1 substitutes for unfamiliar/difficult
    L2 sound (Hammarberg 1990)
  • Polish replacement of ?, ð by s, z t, d or
    f, v (Gussmann 198431)
  • Japanese, German, and Turkish speakers vary in
    what they hear ?, ð as (Hancin-Bhatt 1994)
  • Calabrese uses f/v, whereas other Italians use
    t/d (Flege, Munro, and MacKay 1996) (cf. Cockney
    vs. New York Archibald 1998102)
  • Unschooled French speakers use t (Berger 1951)
    beginners use f, intermediate learners use t
    (Wenk 1979) Quebecois use t, France uses s
    (Archibald 1998102)
  • Korean variation between tense s' and tense
    t' (think ssink ttink) (Oh 2002)
  • Austrian learners of English vary between f s
    dental s lenisized ? (Wieden 1997232)
  • Cf. L1
  • English L1 acq ? ? f in stages IV-VII ð ? d
    or v in stages V-VII (Wenk 1979, Grunwell 1982)
  • ? ? f s, e.g. Susies think ? sink fink
    (Vihman and Greenlee 1987)
  • Conclusion ambiguity resolved by arbitrary
    choice (convention)

32
Dealing with coda voice
  • Overview
  • IL final devoicing as TETU?
  • L1 vs. L2 strategies for dealing with coda
    voice
  • Sources and mechanisms of devoicing

33
Devoicing as TETU?
  • From Uffmann 2004
  • 2 guiding principles
  • Initial state L1 ranking
  • L2 learners may also assume default M F
  • Ranking for lgs that dont allow codas
  • Coda/voi, Coda Faith
  • Ranking for lgs that allow contrastive voicing in
    codas
  • Faith Coda/voi, Coda
  • Demotion of Coda below Faith based on TL
    evidence
  • Coda/voi Faith Coda
  • NB requires ignoring evidence for voiced codas
  • To get Hungarian-English phenomenon (IL devoicing
    despite both NL and TL having voice contrast in
    codas), Uffmann proposes L2 learners assume
    default M F until they receive evidence to the
    contrary
  • Problem contravenes OT assumption that L2
    learners start with L1 ranking (Pater 2005, etc.)

34
L1 vs. L2 strategies for coda voice
  • L1 claimed to only use devoicing
  • The too many solutions problem (Lombardi 1995,
    Kager 1999, Steriade 2001, McCarthy 2002)
  • Lombardi MaxOns, Lar, MaxVoi
  • Steriade P-map
  • McCarthy 2002 targeted constraints can get
    deletion repair, and therefore shouldnt be part
    of OT
  • Kiparsky 2004 blocking also used (Konni, Meccan
    Arabic)
  • L2 epenthesis and deletion attested
  • Deletion
  • Chinese Anderson 1983, Xu 2004
  • Epenthesis
  • Brazilian Portuguese Major 1987 Korean Kang
    2003, Iverson and Lee 2004 Vietnamese Nguyen
    and Ingram 2004 Chinese Eckman 1981, Xu 2004
  • Also found in L1 acq (Major 2001)

35
More problems with OT analysis of final devoicing
  • L1 prediction doesnt fall out nicely from
    constraint inventory requires conspiracy of
    several constraints
  • Coda/voi-less system (Lombardi 2000, Beckman
    2004) misses key articulatory motivation by
    avoiding (cf. Smith et al. 2005)
  • Lombardis scheme should hold for NC as well, but
    doesnt (Myers 2002)
  • Lombardi cant get word-initial neutralization,
    like in English and Lac Simon
  • L1 prediction falsified by L2 data
  • Not clear why devoicing should be easier to learn
    than contrast
  • L2 devoicing may not be emergent UG effect
  • English has devoicing (Haggard 1978, Ladefoged
    1982, Pierrehumbert and Talkin 1992, Smith 1997)
  • May be articulatory phonetic problem
  • L2 speakersmay not have developed adequate
    voicing control abilities (Smith et al. 2005)
  • English-Hungarian case requires reversion to UG
    ranking, not hidden rankings

36
Levels
  • Only low-level automatic L1 rules can cause
    interference (Linell 1979, Rubach 1980, 1984,
    Broselow 1984, Singh and Ford 1987, Eckman and
    Iverson 1995)
  • Morphologically-conditioned processes do not
    cause interference
  • Problem 1 incoherent in monostratal model
  • Problem 2 interference from processes treated as
    morphologically-conditioned in OT (McCarthy 1997,
    Steriade 2001, Picard 2002, Lombardi 2003)
  • r-insertion in RP pronunciation of L2 French,
    German, Spanish (Wells 1982)
  • Jétais déjàr ici, ich bin jar fertig,
    vivar España
  • n-insertion in Korean (H. Kim 1999)
  • lukenñu?selph look at yourself

37
Cases where IL phenomenon ? NL, TL
  • Cases where hidden rankings arent involved
  • Idsardis 2002 spontaneous chain shift
  • Hungarian-English final devoicing (?)
  • Japanese antepenultimate mora accentuation (?)
  • Hungarian gemination after stressed vowel (?)
  • Cases where novel marked configurations are
    produced, violating markedness hierarchy
  • Russian c
  • Odd neutralizations in L1 acq
  • Child with dental click for all coronal
    continuants (Bedore et al. 1994)
  • Child with ingressives for all postvocalic stops
    (Gierut and Champion 2000)
  • 48 subject, Jason, produces pfw to represent
    nearly all word-initial liquid clusters, as well
    as initial labial fricatives (Edwards 1996153)
  • Amahls word-initial engma and voiceless
    sonorants (Smith 19734)
  • child with s in onset, x in coda (Gierut 1993)
  • L2 Yuchuns production of ü for i in English
    UG, phonology, etc.
  • NB she doesnt just do it after j

38
Conclusions
  • Classic OT predictions disconfirmed by
    acquisition data
  • contra M/F-based learning NDEB, opacity,
    avoidance
  • contra Consistency variation, convention
  • contra No levels Level-based interference,
    Unnatural interference
  • contra Markedness Cases where IL phenomenon ?
    NL, TL Unnatural patterns not harder to learn
  • Universal markedness/UG plays a role in SLA
  • final devoicing, cluster simplification
  • Not well captured in OT SLA requires reference
    to UG system overridden by L1 and to phenomena
    not in L1/L2/UG-CON
  • Conversely, language allows a broader range of
    possibilities than is countenanced in OT
  • strategies to deal with devoicing, unnatural
    rules
  • Acquisition is generalization-driven and
    potentially variable
  • supported by DEC effects, conventionalized
    segmental substitutions, which V to delete in
    hiatus, deneutralization
  • Cf. Kiparsky and Menns 1977 model, which
    involves active hypothesis formation and testing
    on the part of the child, and Fey and Gandour
    1979, vs. Stampes and OTs, which are closer to
    behaviorist stimulus-response
  • For variation, cf. Macken and Fergusons flexible
    learning model that allows for variation,
    contrary to earlier deterministic models that had
    predictable L1?L2 transfer

39
References
  • Bedore, L., J. Leonard, and J. Gandour. 1994. The
    substitution of a click for sibilants A case
    study. Clinical Linguistics Phonetics
    8283-293.
  • Broselow, Ellen, S.-I. Chen, C. Wang. 1998. The
    emergence of the unmarked in second language
    phonology.  Studies in Second Language
    Acquisition, 20, 261-280.
  • Broselow, Ellen. 2004. Unmarked structures and
    emergent rankings in second language phonology.
    In Lleó, C., Vogel, I. (Eds.), On the
    acquisition of second language phonology Special
    issue. International Journal of Bilingualism
    8.151-65.
  • Cebrian, J. 2002. Phonetic Similarity,
    Syllabification and Phonotactic Constraints in
    the Acquisition of a Second Language Contrast.
    Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto.
  • Celce-Murcia, M. 1977. Phonological factors in
    vocabulary acquisition a case study of a
    two-year-old English-French bilingual. Working
    Papers in Bilingualism 1327-41.
  • Cho, Mi-Hui and Shinsook Lee. 2001. Phonological
    transparency and opacity in the sound
    substitution of interlanguages. Studies in
    Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology. 7.2. 449-468
  • Connelly, Mark (1994) Phonological Markedness
    and Second Language Error Interpretation.
    Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.
  • Davidson, Lisa. 2002.  The effects of hidden
    rankings on the acquisition of consonant
    clusters. In A. James J. Leather (Eds.), New
    Sounds 2000 (pp. 87-96).  Klagenfurt, Austria 
    University of Klagenfurt.
  • Dell, G. S., Reed, K. D., Adams D. R. and Meyer,
    A.S. 2000. Speech errors, phonotactic
    constraints, and implicit learning A study of
    the role of experience in language production.
    Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning,
    Memory, and Cognition, 26 1355-1367.
  • Dinnsen, Daniel, Kathleen OConnor, and Judith
    Gierut. 2001. The puzzle-puddle-pickle problem
    and the Duke-of-York gambit in acquisition.
    Journal of Linguistics 37.
  • Dziubalska-Kolaczyk, Katarzyna. 1988. How do
    Poles perform English tips of the slung? In
    Papers and studies in contrastive linguistics
    22179-86.
  • Eckman, Fred Iverson, G. K. (1994),
    Pronunciation difficulties in ESL coda
    consonants in English interlanguage In M. Yavas
    (Ed) First and second language phonology.
    (pp.251-265). San Diego Singular Publishing
    Group.
  • Eckman, Fred Iverson, G. K.  (1997).  Structure
    preservation in interlanguage phonology.  In S.
    J. Hannahs, M. Scholten (Eds.), Focus on
    phonological acquisition (pp. 183-207). 
    Amsterdam John Benjamins.
  • Eckman, Fred, A. Elreyes, Greg Iverson. 2003.
    Some Principles of Second Language Phonology.
    Second Language Research 19169-208.
  • Eckman, Fred. (1987b) The reduction of word-final
    consonants in interlanguage. In. A. James L.
    Leather (Eds.) Sound patterns in second language
    acquisition (pp. 143-162). Dordrecht Foris.
  • Gierut, Judith and Annette Hust Champion. 2000.
    Ingressive substitutions typical or atypical
    phonological pattern?
  • Grijzenhout, Janet and Bertus van Rooy. 200x.
    Second language phonology acquisition through
    gradual constraint demotion.
  • Grunwell, P. 1982. Clinical phonology. London
    Croom Helm.
  • Gussmann, Edmund. 1984. Contrastive analysis,
    substantive evidence and the abstractness issue.
    In Theoretical issues in contrastive phonology,
    S. Elliason (ed), 27-36. Heidelberg Julius Groos
    Verlag.

40
References
  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1977. On the Acquisition of
    Phonology (with L. Menn). In J. Macnamara (ed.),
    Language Learning and Thought, Academic Press,
    1977. Reprinted in Georgette Ioup and S. W.
    Weinberger (eds.), Interlanguage Phonology the
    Acquisition of a Second Language Sound System,
    Cambridge, Mass. Newbury House, 1987.
  • Kiparsky, Paul. 2004. Universals constrain
    change, change results in typological
    generalizations.
  • Lombardi, Linda. 2001. Why Place and Voice are
    Different Constraint-Specific Alternations in
    Optimality Theory. In Lombardi, Linda, ed. (2001)
    Segmental Phonology in Optimality Theory
    Constraints and Representations. Cambridge
    Cambridge University Press.
  • Lombardi, Linda. 2003. Second language data and
    constraints on manner Explaining substitutions
    for the English interdentals. Second Language
    Research 19225-250.
  • Lubowicz, Anna. 1999. Derived environment effects
    in OT. The Proceedings of the Seventeenth West
    Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, ed. by
    Kimary Shahin, Susan Blake and Eun Sook Kim,
    451-65. Stanford, CA Center Study Language
    Information. Rutgers Optimality Archive 239
  • Major, Roy. 1987. A model for interlanguage
    phonology. In G. Ioup S. Weinberger.(Eds.)
    Interlanguage Phonology (pp 101 - 124). Rowley,
    MA Newbury House Publishers.
  • Morrison, G. S. (2005, May). Phonetic naturalness
    and phonological learnability. Paper presented at
    The 13th Manchester Phonology Meeting (mfm 13),
    Manchester, UK.
  • Nguyen, Thu and John Ingram. 2004. A corpus-based
    analysis of transfer effects and connected speech
    processes in Vietnamese English. Proceedings of
    the 10th Australian International Conference on
    Speech Science Technology, Macquarie
    University.
  • Pater, Joe and Anne-Michelle Tessier. 2005.
    Phonotactics and alternations Testing the
    connection with artificial language learning. In
    Kathryn Flack and Shigeto Kawahara (eds.),
    University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in
    Linguistics 311-16.
  • Pierrehumbert 2003, in Bod, Rens, Jennifer Hay
    and Stefanie Jannedy, eds. 2003. Probabilistic
    Linguistics. Cambridge, MA MIT Press.
  • Pycha, Anne, Pawel Nowak, Eurie Shin, and Ryan
    Shosted. 2003. Phonological Rule-Learning and Its
    Implications for a Theory of Vowel Harmony. WCCFL
    22 Proceedings, ed. M. Tsujimura and G. Garding,
    pp. 101-114. Somerville, MA Cascadilla Press.
  • Rubach, J. 1984. Rule typology and phonological
    interference. In Theoretical issues in
    contrastive phonology, S. Elliason (ed), 37-50.
    Heidelberg, Julius Groos Verlag.
  • Seidl, A. and E. Buckley. To appear. On the
    learning of arbitrary phonological rules. In
    Language Learning and Development.
  • Singh, Rajendra and Alan Ford. Interphonology and
    phonological theory. In Sound patterns in second
    language acquisition, James and Leather, eds.,
    163-172. Dordrecht Foris.
  • Smith, Bruce, Darcie DeMille Amy Roberts, Ann
    Bradlow Tessa Bent. 2005. Devoicing in
    word-final voiced stop targets produced by native
    and nonnative speakers of English.
    http//www.sfu.ca/spchlab/A54.pdf
  • Smith, Caroline. 1997. The devoicing of /z/ in
    American English effects of local and prosodic
    context. Journal of Phonetics 25.4471-500.
  • Stark, J. 1974. Aphasiological evidence for the
    abstract analysis of the German velar nasal.
    Wiener linguistische Gazette 721-37.
  • Tarone, E. 1976. Some influences on interlanguage
    phonology. Working Papers in Bilingualism
    887-111.
  • Tarone, E.  1988.  Variation in interlanguage. 
    London Edward Arnold.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com