Title: Gestural communication in children and chimpanzees
1Gestural communication in children and
chimpanzees
2- Humans communicate with each other in unique
ways. - Most obviously, linguistically, with socially
learned, intersubjectively shared symbols - But also gesturally. Many of the most important
gestures humans use - e.g., for greeting or
leaving, for threatening or insulting, for
agreeing or disagreeing - are also socially
learned, intersubjectively shared, symbolic
conventions that vary across cultures in much the
same way as linguistic symbols. - This requires both mindreading (theory of mind)
and the ability/motivation to cooperate with
others. - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
3Outline
- Development
- Theoretical issues
- Pointing
- Pointing basics
- Infants
- Apes
- Children with autism
4Development of communication in infants
Milestone Average Age (months)
Babbling (e.g., bababa) 7
? Joint attention, anticipatory smiles by 8-9
Comprehends a word 9
Showing 9-10
Giving 12
Pointing 12
Comprehends 50 words 13
Produces first word 13 (range 9-16)
Produces 10 words 15 (range 13-19)
Produces 50 words 20 (range 14-24)
Produces word combinations 21 (range 18-24)
Adamson (1996) Carpenter, Nagell, Tomasello
(1998)
5Development of communication in infants
Milestone Average Age (months)
Babbling (e.g., bababa) 7
? Joint attention, anticipatory smiles by 8-9
Comprehends a word 9
Showing 9-10
Giving 12
Pointing 12
Comprehends 50 words 13
Produces first word 13 (range 9-16)
Produces 10 words 15 (range 13-19)
Produces 50 words 20 (range 14-24)
Produces word combinations 21 (range 18-24)
Adamson (1996) Carpenter, Nagell, Tomasello
(1998)
6Development of communication in infants
Milestone Average Age (months)
Babbling (e.g., bababa) 7
? Joint attention, anticipatory smiles by 8-9
Comprehends a word 9
Showing 9-10
Giving 12
Pointing (ToM, coop., complexity) 12
Comprehends 50 words 13
Produces first word 13 (range 9-16)
Produces 10 words 15 (range 13-19)
Produces 50 words 20 (range 14-24)
Produces word combinations 21 (range 18-24)
Adamson (1996) Carpenter, Nagell, Tomasello
(1998)
7- Theoretical debate
- Lean versus rich interpretations of gestures
in 12-month-old infants and apes - social-cognitive understanding
- lean just trying to achieve certain behavioral
effects in others (see others as causal but not
mental agents influence behavior) - rich attempting to influence the
intentional/mental states of others (transfer a
mental message influence mind) - motivation
- lean to achieve own goals (e.g., get object or
attention from adult) - rich also for others (inform, help, share)
cooperative structure - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
8- Pointing basics
- In itself, pointing is nothing. When faced with
a pointing finger, most animals and very young
infants simply stare at the finger. - Even understanding the directional nature of
pointing is not enough to comprehend a full
communicative act. It is possible to follow
someones point but not know what he means by it.
To illustrate - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
9Tomasello, Call, Gluckman (1997)see Call
Tomasello (2005) for a review
- In a food finding context, a human points to one
of two opaque containers. - Apes follow the point - but then choose
randomly.
10- Why?
- Either apes dont know what E was directing their
attention to (exactly what E was referring to),
or else they dont know why E was directing them
to it (what Es motive was). - what precise referent is not bucket as physical
object but bucket as location of food - why not just to show bucket, to inform them of
the location - Pointing can be incomprehensible without some
form of shared context or common ground. To
correctly identify the referent, the recipient
needs to assume the point is relevant to
something she and the pointer share. - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
11Importance of shared context (what why)
12Importance of shared context (what why)
13- Shared context can help you determine what the
other is pointing to (and often why) - Expressions of attitude can also help you
determine why.
14Attitude (observable cue to motive why?)
15Attitude (observable cue to motive why?)
16- More pointing basics
- A pointer thus combines an act of reference with
an expression of motive, with the desire that the
recipient attend to both of these, and from this
infer the pointers overall intention - what the
pointer wants the recipient to do - by finding
some relevance to their common ground. - involves understanding of intentions and shared
experience - This entire process is inherently collaborative
communicator and recipient work together to
identify the intended referent, as well as the
pointers larger intention (Clark, 1996). - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
17- More pointing basics
- Cooperative communicative acts involve an
additional type of intention as well a
communicative intention or intention about the
communication specifically (Grice, 1957 Sperber
Wilson, 1986). - When a person points to a tree for me, she not
only wants me to notice the tree, she also wants
me to notice her desire that I notice the tree.
This additional tier is necessary to instigate in
me the kinds of relevance inferences required to
identify the communicator's reason for
communicating (her motive). - if instead she leans back and I see the tree, I
dont need to make those kinds of inferences - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
18- More pointing basics
- She intends that I attend to X (and wants us to
know this together) for some reason relevant to
our common ground.
19- More pointing basics
- She intends that I attend to X (and wants us to
know this together) for some reason relevant to
our common ground. - Apes do have some understanding of others
intentions and attention. Either - do not have a joint attentional frame (common
ground) with the human that enables them to
determine reference (Shes pointing to the
bucket. Im searching for the grape I dont
care about the bucket.) - do not understand the communicative intention,
i.e., that the human wants them to know that she
has an intention with respect to them or - do not understand the informing/helping motive
(cooperative intention) of the human in this
situation. - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
20Complexity of pointing
21Complexity of pointing Adult examples
Standing in line at the bank, one person points for another in the adjacent line to a scarf she has inadvertently dropped on the floor. Gloss "You dropped that."
On a river bank next to a noisy waterfall, a person hands me a book up (I am on top) for safekeeping as she climbs up. She points to the tip of a pencil protruding from the book. Gloss "Don't let this fall out".
In a bar, to a bartender, a person simply points to his empty shot glass. Gloss "I'll have another".
In airplane, I am standing up idly near the bathrooms. A man approaches and points to the bathroom door with a quizzical expression. Gloss "Are you waiting for the bathroom?"
One person to another in line, informing them of a gap in the line ahead of them. Gloss "Hey. Move up."
I approach my parked car and a truck has it blocked in. I look to the driver with an apologetic expression and point to my blocked-in car. Gloss "Sorry, but you have to move to let me out".
Tomasello, Carpenter, Liszkowski (submitted)
22Complexity of pointing Infant examples (11-13
months)
As Dad prepares to leave J points to door.
Mom pouring water J points to his glass to tell her to pour him some.
Mom tells J not to touch her hot teacup later he points to it and says "No."
Mom asks where J got something. J points out the door, saying There.
J watches as Dad arranges Christmas tree when Grandpa enters room J points to tree and says "Oh!"
J bumps his head. When Mom comes he points to offending object.
Points to sky to sound of airplane out the window (can't see).
After eating points to bathroom anticipating going to wash hands.
Mom is looking for magnet. L points to basket of fruit it is hidden in.
L pulled lamp halfway off wall. Dad comes in, L points to show what happened.
T leads Dad around house by pointing, until they find Mom.
Carpenter et al. (in preparation) Tomasello,
Carpenter, Liszkowski (submitted)
23Complexity of infant pointing
- Many motives, meanings
- important because classically infant pointing was
thought to have only two main functions - imperative to request objects
- declarative to share attention and interest to
objects or events - ape pointing apparently only imperative
24Complexity of pointing Infant examples (11-13
months)
As Dad prepares to leave J points to door.
Mom pouring water J points to his glass to tell her to pour him some.
Mom tells J not to touch her hot teacup later he points to it and says "No."
Mom asks where J got something. J points out the door, saying There.
J watches as Dad arranges Christmas tree when Grandpa enters room J points to tree and says "Oh!"
J bumps his head. When Mom comes he points to offending object.
Points to sky to sound of airplane out the window (can't see).
After eating points to bathroom anticipating going to wash hands.
Mom is looking for magnet. L points to basket of fruit it is hidden in.
L pulled lamp halfway off wall. Dad comes in, L points to show what happened.
T leads Dad around house by pointing, until they find Mom.
Carpenter et al. (in preparation) Tomasello,
Carpenter, Liszkowski (submitted)
25Complexity of infant pointing
- Many motives, meanings
- important because classically infant pointing is
thought to have only two main functions - imperative to request objects
- declarative to share attention and interest to
objects or events - ape pointing only imperative (?)
- Absent referents
- important because this is taken to be a hallmark
of uniquely human language also evidence that it
is communication on a mental level
26Complexity of pointing Infant examples (11-13
months)
As Dad prepares to leave J points to door.
Mom pouring water J points to his glass to tell her to pour him some.
Mom tells J not to touch her hot teacup later he points to it and says "No."
Mom asks where J got something. J points out the door, saying There.
J watches as Dad arranges Christmas tree when Grandpa enters room J points to tree and says "Oh!"
J bumps his head. When Mom comes he points to offending object.
Points to sky to sound of airplane out the window (can't see).
After eating points to bathroom anticipating going to wash hands.
Mom is looking for magnet. L points to basket of fruit it is hidden in.
L pulled lamp halfway off wall. Dad comes in, L points to show what happened.
T leads Dad around house by pointing, until they find Mom.
Carpenter et al. (in preparation) Tomasello,
Carpenter, Liszkowski (submitted)
27Complexity of infant pointing
- Many motives, meanings
- important because classically infant pointing is
thought to have only two main functions - imperative to request objects
- declarative to share attention and interest to
objects or events - ape pointing only imperative (?)
- Absent referents
- important because this is taken to be a hallmark
of uniquely human language also evidence that it
is communication on a mental level - Natural observations are interesting but
experiments are needed.
28Experiments
- Common ground
- Informative (helping) motive
- Absent referents
- Communicative intention
29Common ground
- Infants begin participating in joint attentional
engagement by 9 months (more on that tomorrow) - By 14 months, they can use joint attentional
frames/common ground to interpret others points.
30 Behne, Carpenter, Tomasello (2005)
- 14- to 24-month-olds
- Following a visible hiding warm-up, E hid a toy
in one of two opaque containers. - E indicated the toys location by pointing or
gazing at the correct container. - Even the youngest infants chose the correct
container more often than chance.
31Liebal, Behne, Carpenter, Tomasello (in
preparation)
- 18-month-olds
- In each of two Common Ground conditions, infants
participated in a different shared activity
(cleaning up or stacking) with an adult, then
that adult pointed (There!) at a target object.
- In a third, No Common Ground condition, to test
whether infants were really using common ground,
infants shared a frame with one adult and then a
different adult pointed (There!).
that adult pointed
a different adult pointed
32Liebal, Behne, Carpenter, Tomasello (in
preparation)
- Even though the adults pointed in exactly the
same way in each condition, infants interpreted
the point differently depending on the common
ground they shared with the adult - In the Common Ground conditions, infants
responses were appropriate to the previous shared
activity. - In the No Common Ground condition, they continued
the previous activity less than in the
corresponding Common Ground condition, instead
mostly interpreting the new adults point as a
declarative.
- Infants used their common ground with specific
partners to interpret their partners gestures.
33Complexity of infant pointing
- Many motives, meanings
- important because classically infant pointing is
thought to have only two main functions - imperative to request objects
- declarative to share attention and interest to
objects or events - Infants do point imperatively and declaratively
(more on this tomorrow). They also point to
inform others of things they do not know.
34Liszkowski, Carpenter, Striano, Tomasello (2006)
- 12- and 18-month-olds
- Infants watched E repeat an action (e.g.,
punching holes) with a target object. - The target and a distractor object were
displaced. - E began looking around.
- Infants pointed to inform the adult about the
location of the object she was looking for.
35Complexity of infant pointing
- Common ground
- Many motives, meanings, including to inform
others. - Absent referents
- important because this is taken to be a hallmark
of uniquely human language also evidence that it
is communication on a mental level
36Liszkowski, Carpenter, Tomasello (submitted)
Referent Present Phase
- 12-month-olds
- A puppet appeared E attended and emoted
(positively or neutrally) either to it or to the
blank screen on the other side. After the puppet
disappeared, E turned to the infant. - In the first phase, infants pointed more often
when E attended to the screen than to the puppet
(to inform). - Infants also pointed when the referent was
absent, differentially depending on how E had
reacted before.
Attend Event
Attend Screen
Referent Absent Phase
37Complexity of infant pointing
- Common ground
- Many motives, meanings, including to inform
others. - Absent referents
- Understanding of communicative intention (tree
example)
38Behne, Carpenter, Tomasello (2005) control
condition
- E pointed or gazed to the correct container
but in a distracted, non-communicative manner. - In this condition, children performed at chance
levels.
39- Theoretical debate
- Lean versus rich interpretations of gestures
in 12-month-old infants and apes - social-cognitive understanding
- lean just trying to achieve certain behavioral
effects in others (see others as causal but not
mental agents influence behavior) - rich attempting to influence the
intentional/mental states of others (transfer a
mental message influence mind) - motivation
- lean to achieve own goals (e.g., get object or
attention from adult) - rich also for others (inform, help, share)
cooperative structure - Tomasello (in press) Tomasello, Carpenter,
Liszkowski (submitted)
40- Infants
- Communication on mental instead of behavioral
level ? - Evidence (already reviewed) of understanding of
attention and intentions by 12 months - Absent referents (Liszkowski et al., submitted)
- Misunderstandings
- Shwe Markman (1997) when 2½-year-olds request
something from an adult, and the adult
misunderstands but gives them what they wanted
anyway, they still attempt to correct the
misunderstanding. This suggests that they had
both the goal of getting the object and the goal
of having the adult understand their message or
communicative intention. - Prosocial motivations
? - to achieve own goals, of course, but also
prosocial to inform (help), to share.
41- Apes
- Communication on mental or behavioral level
? - Evidence (already reviewed) of understanding of
perception and goals - but not attention and
intentions (?) - not much (if any) gesturing about absent
referents in non-language-trained apes - No prosocial motivations
? - to achieve own goals only no evidence of
gesturing to inform (help others, without benefit
for themselves) or share (tomorrow).
42- Children with autism
- Communication on mental or behavioral level
? - Evidence (already reviewed) of understanding of
perception and goals - but not attention and
intentions (?) - ?
- No prosocial motivations
? - probably to achieve own goals only no evidence
of gesturing to share (no studies on informing).
- Also general difficulties with communicative
intentions (e.g., common ground, language of the
eyes see Sabbagh, 1999, for a review).
43Summary
- By 12 months, when they first begin pointing,
infants already show the basics of uniquely human
communication, supporting the rich view. - Support for the social-pragmatic view of language
acquisition (Bruner, Tomasello, etc.) - More studies are needed, but so far, leaner
interpretations of communication in apes and
children with autism fit best.
44(No Transcript)
45- Hare and Tomasello (2004) hid food in one of two
buckets and then, in one condition, pointed to
the bucket containing the food in order to inform
the ape where it was. In this case, as in
previous studies, the apes searched randomly.
The novelty was in the second condition. Here E
began by establishing with each ape a competitive
relationship over the food, and then later
reached toward one of the two buckets in a vain
attempt to open it (the reaching was impeded).
Now, surprisingly, even though the superficial
behavior of the human was highly similar to that
in the pointing condition - in both cases the
human stretched out his arm toward the correct
location - the apes in this condition suddenly
knew where the food was. In this case, the apes
had to discern the goal of the human - to get
into that bucket - and then infer why he wanted
to do this because there is something good
inside. This cognitive process is quite complex
on its own terms, but the key point is that it
includes none of the crucial elements of shared
intentionality from our analysis of the
interpersonal structure of pointing. The apes'
understanding of the human's reaching is of
individual goals or intentions toward things, not
communicative goals or intentions toward
themselves. There is thus no question of a joint
attentional frame or common ground, or of
communicative or referential intentions, or of
any assumptions of helpfulness or other
interpersonal motives. - Following Tomasello et al. (2005), we may thus
attempt to characterize the essential elements in
the comprehension and expression of human
pointing as a communicative act by viewing them
from the perspective of shared intentionality.
Whereas apes' understanding of the goal of a
reaching person is essentially an act of
individual cognition, humans' understanding that
others are pointing out things for them because
of their presumed relevance to some common ground
or joint attentional frame is an act of
interpersonal cognition involving shared
attention and knowledge, along with some motive
for helping or sharing with others.
46Infants
Chimpanzees