Title: The ontogeny and phylogeny of cultural cognition
1The ontogeny and phylogeny of cultural cognition
2Some uniquely human abilities
- language
- artifacts and technology
- social practices and institutions
-
3Some uniquely human abilities
playing hide-and-seek
showing your vacation photos
attending a summer school
- language
- artifacts and technology
- social practices and institutions
-
holding open a door for someone
playing a duet
helping your kid with homework
driving in traffic
taking a walk together
pointing out interesting sights for others
co-authoring a paper
playing house with your kid
cooking dinner together
planning a party
playing cards
holding a ladder steady for someone
going on a date
meeting for lunch
4- All involve sharing and collaboration.
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
5- All involve sharing and collaboration.
- We propose that the crucial difference between
human cognition and that of other species is the
ability to participate with others in
collaborative activities with shared goals and
intentions shared intentionality.
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
6- All involve sharing and collaboration.
- We propose that the crucial difference between
human cognition and that of other species is the
ability to participate with others in
collaborative activities with shared goals and
intentions shared intentionality. - Two components
- understanding of others goals and intentions
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
7- All involve sharing and collaboration.
- We propose that the crucial difference between
human cognition and that of other species is the
ability to participate with others in
collaborative activities with shared goals and
intentions shared intentionality. - Two components
- understanding of others goals and intentions
- motivation to share psychological states with
others
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
8- By 9-12 months, infants understand something
about others goals and intentions. - Young children with autism and chimpanzees may
have (at least) some understanding of others
goals. - An understanding of others goals and intentions
gives individuals a tremendous social advantage
in terms of being able to explain and predict
others behavior. - But neither is enough to explain some interesting
aspects of human cognition. - In addition
9Sharing intentions
10Understanding of others goals and intentions
Attention
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
11Sharing intentions
SHARED GOAL
SHARED GOAL
Joint Attention
Decision-Making
Decision-Making
relevant reality
relevant skills, knowledge
relevant reality
relevant skills, knowledge
JOINT INTENTION
JOINT INTENTION
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
12- Sharing and coordinating psychological states
- Motivation to share (joint emotions, attention,
interest) - primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration (joint goals, intentions)
- role reversal
- helping
- cooperation and collaborative activities
13- Sharing and coordinating psychological states
- Motivation to share (joint emotions, attention,
interest) - primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration (joint goals, intentions)
- role reversal
- helping
- cooperation and collaborative activities
14Motivation to share in infants
- Early proto-conversations (early infancy sharing
emotions) (e.g., work by Rochat Striano,
Stern, Trevarthen)
15Motivation to share in infants
- Early proto-conversations (early infancy)
- (e.g., work by Rochat Striano, Stern,
Trevarthen) - Joint attention (by 9 months sharing attention
interest triadic) (e.g., work by Bakeman
Adamson, Tomasello, Trevarthen)
1613-month-old infant in joint attentional
engagement
17Motivation to share in infants
- Early proto-conversations (early infancy)
- (e.g., work by Rochat Striano, Stern,
Trevarthen) - Joint attention (by 9 months)
- (e.g., work by Bakeman Adamson, Tomasello,
Trevarthen) - Declarative gestures
- (e.g., work by Bates, Bruner, Camaioni,
Lempers, Tomasello) - shows (around 10 months)
- points (around 12 months)
183-year-old child pointing declaratively
19Motivation to share in infants
- Early proto-conversations (early infancy)
- (e.g., work by Rochat Striano, Stern,
Trevarthen) - Joint attention (by 9 months)
- (e.g., work by Bakeman Adamson, Tomasello,
Trevarthen) - Declarative gestures
- (e.g., work by Bates, Bruner, Camaioni,
Lempers, Tomasello) - shows (around 10 months)
- points (around 12 months)
- Moore not sharing, just attention to self
20 Liszkowski, Carpenter, Henning, Striano,
Tomasello (2004)
- 12-month-olds
- Toys were activated to elicit pointing. E
reacted to infants points in one of four
different ways - Ignore
- Look only to Event
- Look only to Face
- Joint Attention
21 - Infants were most satisfied in the Joint
Attention condition - in Joint Attention, they pointed more across
trials - in the other conditions, within trials, they
repeated points more often. - 12-month-olds point to share attention and
interest.
22Liszkowski, Carpenter, Tomasello (in press)
- When E misunderstands the infants referent, even
if E reacts with excitement, 12-month-old infants
repeat their pointing. Infants pointing is thus
about specific objects or events, not just to
obtain an adult reaction. (also evidence of
collaborative communication message repair)
23 Motivation to share in chimpanzees and children
with autism
- Early proto-conversations no (?)
- (autism e.g., work by Hobson)
- (chimpanzees Tomonaga et al.)
- Joint attention no
- (autism e.g., work by Carpenter, Charman,
Dawson, Mundy, Wetherby) - (chimpanzees e.g., Bard Vauclair,
Tomasello Carpenter) - Declarative gestures no
- (autism e.g., work by Baron-Cohen,
Carpenter, Loveland, Mundy, Sigman) - (chimpanzees e.g., Gomez, Tomasello
Carpenter)
24- Sharing and coordinating intentions
- Motivation to share
- primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration
25- Collaboration
- Bratman (1992)
- shared goal each participant has the goal that
we (in mutual knowledge) do X together - coordination of plans/intentions. This requires
that - each participant understands both roles of the
interaction (so can reverse roles if needed) and - each can help the other with his role if needed.
26- Sharing and coordinating intentions
- Motivation to share
- primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration
- role reversal
- helping
- cooperation and collaborative activities
27Carpenter, Tomasello, Striano (2005)
Tomasello Carpenter (2005)
- 12- and 18-month-olds, children with autism,
chimpanzees - E performed one role of an action and had S
perform the other, e.g., - E hid a toy for S to find, or
- E held out a plate for S to place a toy on it.
- Test E gave the object(s) for the other role
to S and waited. - Does S perform Es previous role (hiding or
offering) toward E (while looking at her)?
28Hiding Task
p.053
- Infants sometimes reversed roles (hid the toy
for E or held out the plate for her) and looked
to E. - Children with autism and chimpanzees sometimes
performed these actions but did so without looks
to E. - They may have been reversing at an action,
rather than a psychological level. (see also
work by Hobson)
Offering Task
29- Sharing and coordinating intentions
- Motivation to share
- primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration
- role reversal
- helping (see also Kuhlmeier, Wynn, Bloom,
2003) - cooperation and collaborative activities
30Liszkowski, Carpenter, Striano, Tomasello (2006)
- 12- and 18-month-olds
- Infants watched E repeat an action (e.g.,
punching holes) with a target object. - The target and a distractor object were
displaced. - E began looking around.
31- Infants pointed to help the adult by informing
her about the location of the object she was
looking for.
32 Warneken Tomasello (2006) 18-month-old
infant
33 Warneken Tomasello (2006) nursery-reared
chimpanzee
34Warneken Tomasello (2006)
- 18-month-olds help instrumentally, in many
different situations - chimpanzees only when E reached
- but other positive results for chimpanzees too
- helping doesnt necessarily involve a shared
goal understanding others individual goal is
sufficient
35- Sharing and coordinating intentions
- Motivation to share
- primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration
- role reversal
- helping
- cooperation and collaborative activities
36- Sharing and coordinating intentions
- Motivation to share
- primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration
- role reversal
- helping
- cooperation and collaborative activities
- communication (Clark, 1997 Golinkoff, 1993
Sperber Wilson, 1986 Tomasello et al., 2005,
submitted)
37Behne, Carpenter, Tomasello (2005) Hare
Tomasello (2004)
- 14- to 24-month-olds,
- chimpanzees
- E hid a toy/food in one of
- two opaque containers.
- E indicated the toys
- location by pointing or gazing
- ostensively at the correct
- container.
38Infants
Chimpanzees
- Infants pass this test.
- Chimpanzees do not use cooperative/communicative
(pointing) cues but do use competitive (reaching)
ones.
39- Sharing and coordinating intentions
- Motivation to share
- primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration
- role reversal
- helping
- cooperation and collaborative activities
- communication
- instrumental
40Warneken, Chen, Tomasello (2006) Liebal et al.
(in prep.)
- 18- and 24-month-olds, chimpanzees (and children
with autism) - Social and instrumental games
- E1 E2 demonstrate how to operate the apparatus.
- E1 cooperates with the child to perform the joint
activity. - E1 refrains from the activity for 15 seconds.
41Warneken, Chen, Tomasello (2006)
42 Warneken, Chen, Tomasello (2006)
43- By 18 months, infants are able to cooperate with
an adult to achieve a joint goal. - When the adult stopped playing his role, children
communicatively requested his continued
participation. - New results at least by 2 years, children do
this even in tasks in which they could achieve
the goal individually (Gräfenhein, Behne,
Carpenter, Tomasello, in prep.) - Chimpanzees (and children with autism) showed far
less coordination of roles and no communicative
requests for their partners continued
participation. - Chimpanzees showed no interest in the social
games. -
44- Sharing and coordinating intentions
- Motivation to share
- primary intersubjectivity
- joint attention
- declarative pointing
- Collaboration
- role reversal
- helping
- cooperation and collaborative activities
- communication
- instrumental
- pretense
45Pretense
- Pretend play is (probably) uniquely human.
- It is often collaborative (shared goal, roles,
etc.). - Some consider it to be the earliest form of true
collective intentionality in infancy (Rakoczy,
2006). - It sets the stage for later collective beliefs
(money, marriage, government).
46Summary
- In addition to their understanding of others
goals and intentions, infants demonstrate - the motivation to share psychological states with
others - and
- ability to cooperate and coordinate intentions
with others. - Chimpanzees and children with autism do not show
evidence of either of these additional
motivations or abilities.
47Where does it come from?
48(a sketchy)Phylogenetic hypothesis
- Primates are competitive. Humans in addition
are cooperative they evolved skills and
motivations for collaborating.
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
49(a sketchy)Phylogenetic hypothesis
- Primates are competitive. Humans in addition
are cooperative they evolved skills and
motivations for collaborating. - How?
- Individuals or groups who could collaborate more
effectively had a selective advantage (more food,
better shelter protection, etc.).
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
50(a sketchy)Phylogenetic hypothesis
- Primates are competitive. Humans in addition
are cooperative they evolved skills and
motivations for collaborating. - How?
- Individuals or groups who could collaborate more
effectively had a selective advantage (more food,
better shelter protection, etc.). - The abilities to share and collaborate made
language and other cultural learning and creation
possible.
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
51(a sketchy)Phylogenetic hypothesis
- Primates are competitive. Humans in addition
are cooperative they evolved skills and
motivations for collaborating. - How?
- Individuals or groups who could collaborate more
effectively had a selective advantage (more food,
better shelter protection, etc.). - The abilities to share and collaborate made
language and other cultural learning and creation
possible. - Over cultural-historical time, repeated,
habitual instances of sharing intentions resulted
in social practices and institutions (e.g.,
marriage, money, government).
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
52(a sketchy)Ontogenetic hypothesis
- Two interweaving lines of development
- understanding of goals and intentions
- motivation to share psychological states and
activities
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
53(a sketchy)Ontogenetic hypothesis
- Two interweaving lines of development
- understanding of goals and intentions
- motivation to share psychological states and
activities - The motivation to share transforms the
understanding intentions line, resulting in
shared collaborative activities.
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
54Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
55Motivation to share
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
56Motivation to share
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
57Motivation to share
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
58Motivation to share
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, Moll (2005)
59Tomasello Carpenter (in press)
60Take-home message
- What is unique about human cultural cognition is
the motivation to share and the ability to
collaborate. - The interaction of
- a biological adaptation
- cultural-historical processes, and
- individual development
- results in uniquely human abilities
ranging from language to taking a walk together,
all of which involve shared intentionality.