Title: Should We Replace Animals with Humans in Research?
1Should we Replace Animals with Humans in Research?
Gary Comstock Professor of Philosophy, NC State
University 2007-08 ASC Fellow, National
Humanities Center
AUTONOMY SINGULARITY CREATIVITY
Duke University 17 March 2008
2Overview
- Abstract
- Definitions
- Assumptions
- Human Pesticide Toxicity Testing (HPTT)
- Arguments against HPTT
- Arguments for HPTT
- Objections
- Conclusions
3Abstract
- I argue for three claims
- Our intuitive rejection of human pesticide
toxicity testing (HPTT) is not justified - 2. In large part, this is because cost-benefit
analyses of HPTT have not adequately weighed the
costs to research animals and - 3. As we seek to replace, reduce, and refine the
numbers of animals used in research we ought to
consider using more humans.
4Definitions
- Pesticides Naturally derived or synthetically
produced substances designed to kill, suppress or
alter the life cycle of unwanted organisms. - Modern industrial agriculture A
capital-intensive global system of hybrid seeds,
high chemical fertilizer and irrigation inputs to
produce high volumes of commodity crops fed to
animals killed for meat.
5Definitions
- Two-level utilitarianism Always choose the
course of action that results in the best
consequences. - At the level of critical thinking, we ought to
function as act-utilitarians. - In everyday life, however, we ought to function
as deontological thinkers.
6Assumptions
- Most developing countries have agricultural
policies aimed at accommodating increased demands
for meat. - Crop yield increases are not keeping pace with
global food demands. - It is unlikely modern agriculture will be soon
replaced by a post-modern agriculture (e.g.,
small scale, family-owned and operated units
using low-chemical inputs, renewable organic
methods to produce regionally transported
vegetables, soy, fruits, and other non-meat based
products, etc.)
7Assumptions
- Global demand for food could easily double. Of
particular concern are Asia and Africa over the
next 50 years - Many of the genetic resources required for the
development of such improved crops are being
destroyed. - A number of the ecosystem services underpinning
productivity are under increasing threat. - The environmental and human health consequences
of fertilizer and pesticide application in
intensive production systems are of growing
concern. - Extreme climatic events accompanying global
climate change may be expected to present an
additional threat to food security for example,
to pest control and crop yields. - Gretchen Daily and Partha Dasgupta, Food
production, population growth, and the
environment. Science 281 (August 28, 1998) p.
1291.
8Assumptions
- Scientific research policies are supported by the
sanctity of life ethic. - Extensive rights for humans to freedom, food, and
opting-out of experiments that might save others
lives. - Minimal rights for animals to minimally painful
invasive procedures and humane deaths.
9Assumptions
- Two-level utilitarianism is the best available
ethical theory. - Make routine daily decisions on the basis of
Intuitive Level System (ILS) rules rights and
deontological considerations. - Overturn ILS rules only when Critical Thinking
demands it.
10Human Pesticide Toxicity Testing
(HPTT)Historical background
- Prior to 1960s, safe levels of pesticide exposure
are determined by - Intentional dosing of experimental animals
- Animals of different species and sexes exposed to
various levels of pesticides. - The reference dose is the maximum amount of daily
exposure that can occur with reasonable
certainty of no harm. - The tolerance level is the amount of pesticide
expected to be in or on the food at harvest.
11From The English Patients
12Human Pesticide Toxicity Testing
- Prior to 1960s, safe levels of pesticide exposure
are determined by - Intentional dosing of experimental animals
- One million animals of different species and
sexes exposed to various levels of pesticides
then killed. - The reference dose is the maximum amount of daily
exposure that can occur with reasonable
certainty of no harm. - The tolerance level is the amount of pesticide
expected to be in or on the food at harvest. - 2. Epidemiological studies of normally exposed
humans
13Epidemiological studies of people who work with
pesticides (normally exposed) Jaga K, Dharmani
C. Sources of exposure to and public health
implications of organophosphate pesticides. Rev
Panam Salud Publica. 200314(3)171-85 Accessed
3/15/2008 at journal.paho.org/uploads/Vol14N3/a04f
ig02.gif
14- Epidemiological meta-analyses of literature to
test hypotheses of correlations
15Historical background of HPTT
- More accurate data on pesticide safety could lead
to relaxed regulations on certain pesticides
leading to increased crop production. - In 1960s, to obtain more accurate data, private
companies begin a third method of testing HPTT. - Food Quality Protection Act (FQPA) in 1996 adds
an additional 10x safety factor to existing
pesticide tolerances to protect children.
16From The English Patients
17HPTT
- Pesticide industry response to FQPA
- Test pesticides directly on humans.
- Eliminate the interspecies 10x safety factor.
18From The English Patients
19The proposal
- Test pesticides directly on humans rather than
rely on extrapolations from animals. - Animal tests may establish that a toxicant causes
malignant tumors when the animal is exposed to
high doses for prolonged periods. - These animal results do not establish safe levels
for humans exposed to low doses intermittently. - The safety factor used to translate animal
results to humans to account for the fact that
humans may be more sensitive to the compound
being tested is arbitrary (in the context of
FIFRA, the EPA uses a factor of 10x. But why not
9x or 11x? Or 100x? 100,000x?) - To establish more accurate safe levels
- for humans, test directly on humans.
20Results
- Between 1996 and 2001, EPA received approximately
4 HPTT test results per year measuring minor,
reversible systemic toxic effects (trembling,
nausea, or headaches resulting from chemical
changes in the nervous system). - Protections for Subjects in Human Research
- Federal Register February 6, 2006 (Volume 71,
Number 24) Rules and Regulations Page
6137-6176. Accessed 3/14/2008 at
http//www.epa.gov/EPA-GENERAL/2006/February/Day-0
6/g1045.htm
215. Arguments Against HPTT
- EPA's Latest Human Pesticide Testing Rule Called
Illegal, Immoral - WASHINGTON, DC, January 25, 2006 (ENS) - Three
U.S. legislators are asking the U.S.
Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) to withdraw
a planned rule to permit pesticide
experimentation on humans, including pregnant and
nursing mothers and children. . California
legislators Senator Barbara Boxer, and
Representatives Henry Waxman and Hilda Solis
released details of the rule and called on EPA
Administrator Stephen Johnson to withdraw the
rule, calling it a "profound moral and ethical
breach.
22Environmental Working Group, The English
Patients Human Experiments and Pesticide Policy
1998
23(No Transcript)
24Intuitive level system (ILS) rule Do no
harm. Implication No-test We should not
test pesticides on people.
25- Critical Thinking arguments for No-test
- 1. We do not need pesticides alternative
agriculture. - Pesticides damage nontarget organisms,
including workers. - Workers often are disempowered migrants.
- Pesticides can harm consumers who eat the
foods. - Test on animals, not people.
- Direct dosing can harm humans long-term.
- HPTT does not benefit the research subjects
enrolled in the tests.
26- Arguments against No-test
- Pesticides lower food costs to the nation's
poorest and neediest children. - Industrial agriculture will not soon be
replaced. - Developing pesticides is costly, risky, and
time-consuming. - Food costs will rise if pesticides become more
expensive. - Pesticide testing harms animals the interests
of sentient animals count. - All humans used have have given informed
consent. - Self-interest is not a source of self-deception
in this case - We have substantial knowledge benefits are
significant, risks are minimal.
27 Mice and rats seem to have episodic
memory. Rats seem to lead biographical lives.
Episodic-like memory Rats prefer items on the
basis of recollections of what they have
previously seen, where they saw it and when it
saw it (Kart-Teke et al, 2006). Rats can
remember without any cueing influences what they
saw and where depending on which past situation
they are being asked to remember. (Eacott et al,
2005)
28Mice and rats seem to have metacognition.
Rats have metacognition, the ability to reflect
on ones own beliefs. Rats given a choice to
take a test they are likely to fail or refuse to
take it can base their decisions on their own
assessment of the state of their knowledge and
the likelihood they will pass. ------------- The
rats were more likely to decline these difficult
tests, as would be expected if the rats knew that
they did not know the correct duration-discriminat
ion response. Allison L. Foote and Jonathon D.
Crystal, Metacognition in the Rat, Current
Biology 17 (20 March 2007), pp. 551-555.
Accessed 3/10/2008 at http//www.sciencedirect.co
m/science?_obGatewayURL_methodcitationSearch_u
oikeyB6VRT-4N97NYR-N_originSDEMFRASCII_version
1md501a05b98c9f393f8360e2fa2807df88b
29- The regulatory process does not take into account
the following - that natural chemicals make up the vast bulk of
chemicals to which humans are exposed - that the toxicology of synthetic and natural
toxins is not fundamentally different - that about half of the chemicals tested, whether
natural or synthetic, are carcinogens when tested
using current experimental protocols - (4) that testing for carcinogenicity at
near-toxic doses in rodents does not provide
enough information to predict the excess number
of human cancers that might occur at low-dose
exposures - (5) that testing at the maximum tolerated dose
(MTD) frequently can cause chronic cell killing
and consequent cell replacement (a risk factor
for cancer that can be limited to high doses),
and that ignoring this effect in risk assessment
greatly exaggerates risks.
Lois Swirsky Gold, T. Slone, B. Ames, What do
animal cancer tests tell us about human cancer
risk? Overview of analyses of the Carcinogenic
Potency Database, Drug Metabolism Reviews, 30(2),
359-404 (1998). Accessed 3/15/2008 at
http//potency.berkeley.edu/pdfs/DrugMetabolismRev
iews.pdf
30The human diet is full of chemicals. We dont
know which are dangerous. E.g., there are 1000
natural chemicals in roasted coffee. Only 28
have been tested for carcinogenicity in rodents.
19 were positive. Lois Swirsky Gold, T. Slone,
B. Ames, What do animal cancer tests tell us
about human cancer risk? Overview of analyses
of the Carcinogenic Potency Database, Drug
Metabolism Reviews, 30(2), 359-404 (1998).
Accessed 3/15/2008 at http//potency.berkeley.edu/
pdfs/DrugMetabolismReviews.pdf
31- Definition and scope of costs considered in
assessment by the Home Office - 1. Costs are defined as the adverse effects, i.e.
pain, suffering, distress or lasting harm, likely
to be experienced by the animals used during the
course of a study. - 2. This includes any material disturbance to the
normal health, i.e. to the physical, mental and
social well-being, of the animal, and covers
disease, injury and physiological or
psychological discomfort. - 3. Both immediate effects, e.g. transient
discomfort from injections, and longer-term
effects, e.g. the subsequent toxic effects of
test materials, are included. - 4. Costs arising from both regulated scientific
procedures and husbandry and care systems are
considered, and all of the interventions applied
to an animal or group of animals from the time
they are issued from stock until they are killed
or discharged from the controls of the Act are
covered. - 5. Regulation of scientific procedures starts at
the level of adverse effect cause by skilled
insertion of a hypodermic needle, or equivalent,
depending on the procedure. It is noted that the
following procedures are covered - minor procedures with adverse effects below the
above threshold, if they are part of a series or
combination whose cumulative adverse effects are
above the threshold for regulation - procedures performed wholly under general
anaesthesia - procedures for generation and breeding of animals
likely to suffer adverse effects, i.e. - breeding animals with harmful genetic defects
- manipulation of germ cells or embryos to alter
the genetic constitution of the resulting animal
and - subsequent breeding of such genetically modified
animals. - 6. Any expected adverse phenotypic expression in
harmful mutant or genetically modified animals is
included. - 7. Costs arising from both acts of commission,
e.g. dosing or sampling, and omission, e.g.
withholding food or water, are considered. - 8. Costs are assessed with reference to the
biology, behaviour and life-style of the species
concerned for example, although partial facial
weakness may be a relatively minor adverse effect
in humans, it has serious consequences for
cud-chewing animals. - The Animal Procedures Committee, Review of
cost-benefit assessment in the use of animals in
research, June 2003. Accessed 3/15/2008 at
http//www.apc.gov.uk/reference/costbenefit.pdf
32Conclusion
- I have argued for three claims
- The ILS rule against intentionally dosing humans
with pesticides is not justified at the level of
critical thinking - Cost-benefit analyses of the subject have not
adequately weighed the costs to animals and - As we seek to replace, reduce, and refine the
numbers of animals used in research we ought to
consider increased use of humans as a means to
this objective.