Title: Constructional%20Profiles%20as%20the%20Basis%20of%20Semantic%20Analysis
1Constructional Profiles as theBasis of Semantic
Analysis
- Suzanne Kemmer
- Rice University
- kemmer_at_rice.edu
2Introduction
- Construction Grammar defines constructions as
linguistic units that necessarily have some
non-compositional semantics - Constructions have some aspect of meaning that is
not reducible to (or predictable from) its
component parts (or other constructions) - And, constructions are argued to be necessary as
a construct in any theory of grammar.
3Why do we need constructions?
- One argument for the indispensability of
constructions in grammatical analysis comes from
Coercion Effects (Michaelis 2002) -
4Constructions and Coercion
- Constructions explain how certain expected
semantic anomalies do not materialize. - Give me some pillow!
- Elements in some sentences are expected to clash
by virtue of their incompatible semantics, based
on their distribution outside the construction.
5Constructions override lexical meaning
- Constructions fill in semantic substance and
overcome semantic incompatibility of component
parts through override - I slept my way across the Atlantic.
- Sleep -- lack of motion specification
- Sentence as a whole -- describes motion with
concomitant sleeping - pit the cherries, dust the furniture, bone the
filet - conventionalized semantic elements added
motion, directionality
6Investigating constructional semantics
- We can investigate the semantics of constructions
in various ways. Most relevant here - 1. Observation of distributional properties at
sentence level contrasts - Syntactic properties are diagnostics or clues to
semantics (as per assumption of close nature of
the syntax-semantics relation of CL) via
acceptability patterns in arrays of minimally
contrasting examples. - Semantics can be investigated by observing
lexical items in mainly clause level contexts and
observing anomalies and compatibilities that make
such utterances less or more acceptable. - Lakoff (1987) and Langacker (1987, 1991) inter
alia analyze constructions (also lexical items)
using this methodology. - 2. Observation of distributions of recurring
elements in a construction in large samples of
language use
7Investigating constructional semantics
- We should employ any methodologies that prove
useful - Ideal convergence of multiple sources of
evidence gathered via different methodologies - Second method gives insight into some semantic
properties otherwise inaccessible (on assumption
that frequency is a reflection of degree of
entrenchment, which is itself a part of the
system).
8What else can we get?
- Observing constructions also contributes to an
understanding of the important mechanism of
coercion - How does it work?
- How and to what extent can it forestall anomaly?
9Who uses corpora?
- Who uses large corpora for the cognitive semantic
analysis of constructions? Consider - British, Scandinavian, and American schools
- Sinclair, Stubbs, Stig Johanson, Hunston and
Francis, Biber. Corpora yes, cognitive semantic
analysis of constructions, no or minimal - Construction Grammarians
- Fillmore, Kay, Michaelis, Croft, Goldberg, and
their students - Corpora, no or not primarily semantic analysis
is largely done by the first method above - Corpus-construction grammarians
- Boas, Fried, Gries, Lambrecht, Michaelis, Östman,
Stefanowitsch, and students. - Corpora yes, cognitive semantic analyses, yes
- Corpus-cognitive linguists
- Barlow, Geeraerts, Kemmer, Verhagen and students
- Corpora yes, cognitive semantic analysis yes
10Results and ongoing research
- The Corpus-CG and Corpus-CL groups have in
common - Bottom-up analyses of particular constructions.
Visual inspection for patterns via keywords in
context and sorting typically statistical data - Analysis of constructional semantics,
identification of theoretical issues in CL/CG - Inclusion of units at varying levels of
specificity - See Rohde 2001 Boas 2002, Gries and
Stefanowitsch 2002 Stefanowitsch and Gries 2003,
Fried 2004 studies appearing in Achard and
Kemmer 2004 and Oestman and Fried 2005 Hilpert
(to appear a, to appear b) Taylor (to appear)
11Diachrony
- Several diachronic studies in Cognitive
Linguistics and, more recently,Construction
Grammar, using corpora - Carey 1994, Israel 1995, Ziegler 2002, Diewald,
Gilquain 2004, Kemmer and Hilpert (forthcoming)
12Two perspectives
- Focus on individual lexemes
- many analyses of 1980s and 1990s still current
- polysemy networks containing linked senses of
prepositions, cases, etc. - Kemmer (1993) added links to conceptually
neighboring concepts in a multidimensional
semantic/conceptual space (constructions not
foregrounded) - Focus on the construction as the unit of
observation - Integration of lexical semantic information with
constructional specifications
13Item-based vs. construction-based
- Lexical perspective continues a venerable
tradition of focus on words - Words (more precisely lexical roots) are
certainly salient cognitive units - Speakers access word meaning much more easily
than meaning of linguistic units of other sizes
or greater schematicity - sublexical morphemes, constructions, sentences
- BUT there are some very good reasons for the
switch to viewing linguistic knowledge from a
constructional perspective.
14Motivating the constructional view
- Traditional polysemy networks, although focusing
on lexical items, actually require observation of
lexemes in larger chunks of language - Linked senses are structured by relative
closeness of senses as determined by the
closeness of the relations of the contextualized
examples (representing types of uses) - The standard methodology for distinguishing word
senses is to posit different linguistic contexts
in which the senses become clearly disambiguated - Example the ring on my finger vs. he was knocked
right out of the ring - the desired sense is the only one compatible with
the surrounding material. - Thus Distribution of lexical items in larger
structures is how we can tell the range of
senses/uses of a lexical item
15Senses often correlate with constructions
- For relational units, the various senses of a
given lexical unit are actually associated with
constructions. - Static vs. dynamic senses of the English
prepositions are parts of constructions that
express static and dynamic spatial senses - over (the hill) hovered over the hill, flew
over the hill - Prep NP V-static over NP, V-motion over NP
- Senses of verbs are in most cases correlated with
different constructions (e.g. roll (tr., caused
motion) vs. roll (intr., autonomous motion) - Following Goldberg (1995), Construction
Grammarians reject positing extra verb senses - an extra sense for sneeze in she sneezed the
napkin off the table seems absurd
16Bidirectional links
- But Langacker (2003) points out there are many
verbs that have a strong associative link to a
particular construction. - give is extremely frequent in the ditransitive
compared to other verbs - The ditransitive construction is extremely
frequent with give. - The usage-based model predicts, based on
frequency, that there is a highly
conventionalized link to the ditransitive that is
part of our knowledge of give. - If so, give is an access point to the
ditransitive construction and its associated
frame - Hypothesis The links between lexical item and
constructions reach in both directions - We posit both nodes as units if both are
conventionalized. Give may activate the
ditransitive just as the construction primes the
word.
17The usage-based model
- In the usage-based model, links in a linguistic
knowledge network are viewed as activation
pathways with potentially bidirectional
activation flows (cf. Lamb 2000) - Predicts that strongly entrenched links could
potentially go in either direction. - Converges with findings from neurology suggesting
that links between neurons and between cortical
columns have physically distinct pathways that
can have differential activation strength.
18Predictions
- Viewing the links as potentially bidirectional
makes some predictions - It should be possible for there to be
dissociation between the relative strengths of an
activation path directed from a lexical item to a
construction, and another in the opposite
direction. - Evidence from distributional studies show that
some individual verbs occur very frequently in a
particular construction, like wriggle in the way
construction (the baby wriggled its way out of
the playpen) and not at all often, relatively
speaking, outside the construction. Other
examples in Gries and Stefanowitsch 2005.
19The usage-based model
- Conversely, a verb like make occurs frequently in
the construction, but only as a function of its
high overall frequency. - Thus, affinity to the way construction not as
tight - Prediction wriggle should have a stronger
priming effect on the construction than make. - Psycholinguistic evidence Goldberg 2004 for give
and ditransitive - Still, the way construction does have a strong
link to make. - Prediction
- The construction allows a wide range of verbs,
but given no special semantic properties the
speaker desires to convey, the speaker is likely
to choose make over other possibilities because
s/he has a great deal of experience of that
choice, which effectively increases, by repeated
memory, the likelihood of activation of make
--unless there is some overriding reason to make
another choice (desire to express manner of
motion, for example). - I think of this as long-term priming.
20More on preference for constructions
Constructions and events
- Argument structure constructions are used to form
more event-sized conceptualizations than single
words. - Such constructions are likely to be rather
crucial units relied on by speakers to make an
intial chunking of reality into a manageable and
manipulable portion of conceptual structure, as
suggested by Talmy 2000. - If this is true, then starting from the
construction and investigating it with regard to
what smaller units occur in it habitually/conventi
onally is more likely to result in an analysis
that is something like what the speaker knows
about how words are used. - The speakers main experience with word usage, in
fact, even from the earliest childhood, is in the
context of larger utterances that the adult uses
before child can even talk generally speaking,
constructions.
21Constructions and events
- The constructional perspective puts us into the
realm of meaning which is much more like the
kinds of meanings that speakers deal more
normally with in language use. - It is difficult for speakers to define either
words or constructions - When they do try to define words, they have to
activate memories of contexts similar to those in
which they heard the word used, i.e. they have to
try to come up with an imagined context similar
to one they experienced, or an actual remembered
context for the usage. - Novice linguists sometimes try to imagine a
situational context without a linguistic context,
which gives them much more trouble coming up with
accurate usage generalizations. - They have to be trained to think of specific
linguistic utterances that will allow them to
more precisely explore the word in something more
like the typical contexts in which they have
heard it, and in which it contributes to an
overall semantically integrated
conceptualization. - It is an empirical fact that speakers most
typically process constructions in usage, and not
isolated words, given that most conversation
occurs in construction-like chunks and not in
isolated words.
22Constructions and processing
- Constructions have more utility to speakers for
choosing compatible lexical items to use with the
construction than vice versa. - But by hypothesis, lexical elements which hardly
occur outside the construction can trigger it, so
that the effects might go the other way. - In the process of arguing for the necessity of
constructions in grammatical theory, some
important empirical findings about constructions
have emerged (Goldberg 2004) - Speakers use constructions to interpret lexical
items - Argument structure constructions aid children to
learn new words - These findings make perfect sense if we accept
the view that constructions are extremely
important in language processing, even if they
are below the level of consciousness (like most
grammatical units). - For purposes of guiding the precise activation
links that will allow instantaneous accessing of
appropriate lexical items, constructions, on this
view, would be crucial processing units.
23Rohde 2001
- Study of the relations between lexical items and
constructions that used substantial corpora as a
basis for drawing cognitive semantic conclusions
about those relations - Rohde investigated a large range of English
motion verbs and their co-occurrence patterns
with a range of prepositions in a motion verb
preposition NP schematic construction frame. - Found strong distributional correlations between
motion verbs and particular prepositions and in
fact with particular senses of the prepositions,
essentially corresponding to particular
constructions. - Results showed that particular verbs have
affinities for particular prepositions and vice
versa (and--links have different strengths as
measured by frequency.)
24Rohde 2001
- Patterns of affinity were also found at a more
general level, involving verb classes defined by
particular semantic properties. - Affinity patterns reflect entrenched semantic
properties of particular units - Heightened compatibility makes the lexical items
more likely to be used in the constructions
reduced compatibility reduces the frequency of
the less compatible item. - Compatibility has a strong effect on frequency.
- For example, the verb escape occurs extremely
frequently with source prepositions like from.
Most other motion verbs in English prefer goal
prepositions, with varying degrees of preference.
Rohde concluded that a source (rather than
goal) image schema is strongly conventionally
linked with the lexical item escape. - We can also take the constructional point of view
of the same phenomenon and say that the
semi-specified construction X escape PREP N is
strongly conventionally linked with source
prepositions, whereas a similar construction with
go is linked with goal prepositions.
25Coercion in action
- Evidence for the key role of constructions in
constructing meaning comes from acceptability
judgments. - It has been demonstrated that constructions
coerce lexical items interpretations (Michaelis
2002 and refs.), whereas the opposite has not
been demonstrated. -
- Example of semantic incompatibility
- I walked into the room.
- I squinted into the room.
- supposed to be badand is if one is thinking of
the caused motion construction
26Coercion
- Now consider
- (1) She looked into the room.
- (2) She squinted into the room.
- (3) She squinted through the window.
- (4) She squinted through her glasses.
- (5) She peered through her glasses.
- (1) expresses an event of directed vision. (2)
has a context involving a somewhat similar visual
event for squint. Its better than it was on
previous slide. - Subsequent sentences get better and better. Why?
27Coercion explained
- The frame semantics of squinting are associated
with a typical purpose of squinting to make ones
vision better temporarily by deforming the cornea
with the surrounding eye muscles. - Squinting, then, often occurs to improve vision
that is somehow impaired, possibly in part due to
a barrier to vision such as intervening dirty
window glass. The more overtly we add a barrier
to visibility to the context, the more we improve
semantic compatibility and thus the better the
sentence sounds. - Claim these sorts of acceptability judgments tap
into some speaker knowledge relating knowledge of
lexical items to constructions. (That knowledge
itself was originally derived from experience
using those lexical items in larger
constructions). - If true, we would expect that speaker knowledge
to be also reflected in frequency patterns. In
fact we predict the following
28Predictions (requiring testing)
- An increasing degree of semantic distance of a
lexical item (i.e. it contains some
incompatibility) from a conventional construction
type will - increase unacceptability of the particular
lexical item in the construction - increase need for additional linguistic context
with additional elements that highlight some
compatible semantic feature of the target item,
if that item is to be produced or interpreted as
acceptable. - This is the implicit basis of many linguistic
arguments for semantics of particular elements.
29More predictions
- If an utterance is ambiguous, increasing
distance/incompatibility should decrease the
likelihood that the reading with greater semantic
incompatibility will be chosen. - Decrease likelihood of production (compared with
more compatible units) - Decrease frequency of production (compared with
more compatible units) - Extra contextual information (linguistic or just
situational) needed for production. - I gave it a little kick.
- I gave the lever (on the handlebar) a little kick
with my thumb.
30Problems
- How to quantify, or at least operationalize how
to judge, degree of semantic similarity/difference
, closeness/distance, compatibility/incompatibilit
y. - How to corroborate Linguists interested in
semantics can usually come up with an analysis
that is intuitively satisfying, but little
psycholinguistic experimentation has been done - Productivity When is coercion OK, when not?
- Individual variation
- Relation of innovation of an extension and its
spread
31Testability
- Unfortunately, the last two predictions, about
the decreasing likelihood of production (and
hence decreasing frequency) are rarely examined. - The subtle contrasts used by linguists to test
the boundaries of a construction are rarely
present in corpora, because as phenomena near the
boundary there are not likely to be many
instances, if any. - However, we can still make the prediction and
with a large enough corpus, it should be borne
out if the assumptions about the relation of
usage and linguistic knowledge are correct.
32Potential problems
- Why dont Construction Grammarians in general try
to determine the semantics of constructions they
work on by examining the lexical items that
occur in them most often? Why is this left to a
few Corpus-construction people? - Possible reasons
- Not all constructions occur with specific
recurrent lexical items (or classes of items). - (but even some surprisingly , general argument
structure constructions do, like the passive)
33Potential problems
- Or, it might be more serious
- Construction Grammarians (unlike Cognitive
Linguistics) often do not take the usage-based
model to heart and concern themselves with
mechanisms of production and interpretation. - Construction grammar analyses and theoretical
descriptions simply do not bring processing
considerations into the picture. - Possibly this is because Construction Grammarians
are agnostic about what exactly happens when
language is used. - Backgrounding of processing aspects to CG may be
the real reason that it is so rare to encounter
investigations which use frequency data by most
Construction Grammarians.
34Corpus-Construction Grammarians and Corpus
Cognitive Linguists
- The small community of Corpus-Construction
Grammarians also are by and large strong
cognitivists familiar with the implications of
the usage-based model for the theoretical side of
the model, and not just for selection of data. - So they, along with Corpus Cognitive Linguists,
are prepared to use corpora seriously to discover
the cognitive semantics associated with
constructions.
35The make-causative
- The make-causative shows strongly marked
distributional asymmetries suggesting it is not a
generalized causative construction, but instead
has specific semantic properties. - 3 senses of the make causative construction in
English - mechanical action we need to make it work
- emotional reaction it made me feel good
- compulsion make you cant make me marry him
- Frequency patterns of animacy of causer and
causee shows clusterings of 3 senses. (Kemmer
2001 2005)
36The make-causative
- The make causative has its own unique
constructional profile of elements that
typically occur with it and which relate to its
function as a construction. - These characteristic distribution patterns are
found with many constructions - (e.g. English passive Dutch laten and doen
causatives German lassen causative English let
and have causatives into causative, split
infinitives, Swedish future) - Also, distributional observation of the predicate
complements of make causative show a relatively
narrow range of complements. - The set of verbs with the widest range of
predicates, correlating semantically with the
compulsion use, is the least frequent in type
frequency and overall token frequency. - But most predicates are far more restricted,
falling into a number of semantic subclasses, but
grouping into just 2 main classes (mechanical
action emotional reaction). These also correlate
with the clusterings of animacy causer/causee
types. Thus we have two pieces of formal,
distributional evidence that fingerprint the
construction their interpretation results in a
unique semantic profile the constructional
semantics of the make causative.
37The make-causative
- Moreover, the make causative shows some
interactional effects between the predicates
selected in it, and the senses of those
predicates. - The make causative effectively constrains the
interpretation of the predicates, where they are
polysemous. - For example,
- In
- I worked
- And
- It worked
- The default readings are senses of work that
respectively correlate with the animate and
inanimate subjects in the examples.
38The make-causative
- However, in the make causative, the sense of work
that occurs cannot be predicted from the animacy
of the causee - It really made it work.
- It really made him work.
- In the causative construction, the second example
is at best ambiguous, but more likely to be
interpreted as mechanical action make rather than
social compulsion make.
39The make-causative
- Such coercion effects reveal the nature of make
OBJ INF as a conventionalized construction - specifically support the analysis of the make
causative as being primarily about mechanical
action causation, and secondarily its extension
emotive reaction causation. - the least motivated (entrenched) sense of the
make causative is one which a human is acting on
another human to socially compel him/her to do
what the causer wishes to be done. - The normal sense of work (voluntarily perform
labor) is suspended here because the entrenched
constructional semantics induce a reading of
involuntary, mechanical action. The sentence
could perhaps be used that way, but it would take
some supporting semantic factors that were
coherent with a social frame of strong social
force. - This would be required to overcome the slight
semantic incompatibility of the construction with
the normal sense of work that is found with human
subjects.
40The historical dimension
- We can also observe the history of the make
causative. In this history we find support for
the 3 senses of the current construction, and
find their progressive emergence as distinct
senses. - When we examine the history of the construction,
we find that the construction did not always
occur with its current constructional profile. - In fact, in the earliest days in our data, make
was primarily a main verb taking nominal and
adjectival predicates. - It began to take infinitives in early middle
English (the do-causative was fading by then),
but as an early causative construction it was
principally (most frequently in type and token
frequency) used with the Mechanical Action type
of predicates. - Although all 3 senses were apparently attested,
the primary use as attested by frequency was the
Mechanical action. - As time went on, emotion predicates began to
dominate, as they still do today, but the number
of compulsion predicates began to move past the
one or two found in the earlier periods. - Furthermore, we find the beginnings of coercion
effects as the construtcion begins to exert an
interpretational effect on the senses of verbs
like work and look. - Effectively, it begins to coerce its component
elements to become more compatible with its own
developing semantics.
41The historical dimension
42The historical dimension
- Observing the history of make in this way allows
us to see for the first time how a construction
emerges by gradual extension by speakers until we
can see the construction exert a coercion effect
which itself motivates the analysis of the make
causative as a full-fledged construction. - The development followed a trajectory of a
changing constructional profile in terms of
preferred predicates and predicate types. - The changing constructional profile is itself
both a symptom and a mechanism of change, because
speakers are sensitive to frequency and the
changing frequency will have the effect of
inducing them to reorganize the construction into
one increasingly resembling the modern make
causative in its constructional profile and its
semantic characteristics, including coercive
force.
43Conclusions--Utility of Constructional Profiles
- Observing constructional profiles from large sets
of usage data allows us to draw conclusions about
the semantics of a construction - Examining diachronic changes in constructional
profile shed light on the emergence of the
construction
44Conclusions Coercion diachrony
- We can find early evidence for the
conventionalization of a construction by
pinpointing the first visible instances of
coercion. - These show that the construction has acquired
some semantics of its own that can override
semantic anomalies. - Constructions are a fruitful perspective for not
only synchronic, but diachronic investigation.
45Acknowledgments
- Thanks to Martin Hilpert Michael Barlow
46References
- Achard, Michel, and Suzanne Kemmer, eds. 2004.
Language, culture and mind. Stanford CSLI
Publications. - Barlow, Michael, and Suzanne Kemmer, eds. 2000.
Usage Based Models of Language. Stanford CSLI
Publications. - Hilpert, Martin. To appear (a). On Swedish future
constructions. Proceedings of the High Desert
Linguistic Society Meeting. Albuquerque HDLS. - Hilpert, Martin. To appear (b). Collograms in the
English split infinitive and other grammatical
constructions. Constructions. Special issue on
Collostructional Analysis. - Kemmer, Suzanne. 2001. Causative constructions
and cognitive models The Make Causative.The
First Seoul International Conference on Discourse
and Cognitive Linguistics Perspectives for the
21st Century, 803-846. Seoul Discourse and
Cognitive Linguistics Society of Korea. - Kemmer, Suzanne and Martin Hilpert. 2005.
Constructional grammaticalization in the English
make-causative. Presented at ICHL in Madison,
Wisc. August 2005. - Kemmer, Suzanne and Arie Verhagen. 1994. The
grammar of causatives and the conceptual
structure of events. Cognitive Linguistics 5(2),
115-156. - Lakoff, George. 1987. Women, fire and dangerous
things. Case studies. Chicago University of
Chicago Press. - Langacker, Ronald. 1987,1991. Foundations of
Cognitive Grammar Vols I and II.
47References, cont.
- Michaelis, Laura A. 2005. Entity and event
coercion in a symbolic theory of syntax. In
Jan-Ola Oestman and Miriam Fried, eds.,
Construction Grammar(s) Cognitive Grounding and
Theoretical Extensions. (Constructional
Approaches to Language 3.) Amsterdam Benjamins,
45-88. - Stefanowitsch, Anatol, and Stefan Gries. 2003.
Collostructions Investigating the interaction of
words and constructions. International Journal of
Corpus Linguistics 8, 209-243. - Taylor, Christopher. To appear. The X to where Y
construction. Proceedings of the 31st Meeting of
the Berkeley Linguistic Society (BLS 31, 2005).
Berkeley BLS.