Title: SLA: Activities for Meaningful Interaction
1SLA Activities for Meaningful Interaction
LING 561/761 09/13/2006
2Computer-Mediated Language Learning Contexts (1)
- Research focusing directly on computer-assisted
language learning has remained largely on the
margins of SLA research (Chapelle, 2004
Hulstijn, 2000) as it has struggled to
contextualize itself within a larger theoretical
framework of SLA (Chapelle, 1998, 2001 Doughty
Long 2003 Salaberry, 2000).
3Computer-Mediated Language Learning Contexts (2)
- Computer-Mediated Communication (CMC)
- Computer-Assisted Language Learning (CALL)
- Intelligent Computer-Assisted Language Learning
- (ICALL)
- Interaction occurs in a written modality.
- Discourse proceeds at a slower pace than in oral
- interaction.
- More time for preparing/repairing utterances.
- Turn-taking sequences/allocation of turns are
unique and - vary according to the specific
medium.
4What are the challenges? (1)
- For SLA Research
- Comparability to traditional SLA research
paradigms. - Can computer-centered language learning be
task-based? - Can attested results from oral interaction be
brought to bear on human-computer interaction? - Modality
- How do we account for the role of modality in
this type of SLA research? - Visual, text-based, written
- No prosodic or paralinguistic cues
- More time for online processing/planning
- Few to no socio-cultural constraints
5What are the challenges? (2)
- For Language Pedagogy
- Traditional classrooms
- How are computer-centered tasks actually used in
traditional language classes? - How might they more effectively complement
traditional teaching? - What are the technical limitations? Can they be
overcome? - Distance education
- Same as above.
6SLA Claims in CALL Research (1)
-
- "Because oral interaction is considered by many
to be important for second language development,
and because synchronous CMC bears a striking
resemblance to oral communication, it seems
logical to assume that language practice through
CMC will reap some of the same benefits for
second language development as practice through
oral interaction." - Pelletieri (2000, p. 59)
7SLA Claims in CALL Research (2)
-
- The findings suggest that computer mediated
communication (CMC) can provide many of the
alleged benefits ascribed to the Interaction
Hypothesis, but with greatly increased
possibilities for access outside of the classroom
environment. - Blake (2000, p. 120)
- "Networked exchanges, since they are text-based
and learners must type out or produce the
structures in question, appear to constitute an
example of forced output ." - Blake (2000, p. 132)
8Feedback in CMC Research (1)
- Claims from CMC research include
- Computer-mediated peer feedback is neither
superior nor inferior to orally provided peer
editing feedback in promoting second language
writing development (Schultz, 2000). - Learners negotiate for meaning, provide feedback
and modify their output when engaged in CMC
tasks, especially in response to lexical and
structural difficulties (Tudini, 2003).
9Feedback in CMC Research (2)
- Claims from CMC research (cont.)
- "Because CMC fosters negotiation of meaning and
form-focused interaction and because students
communicating through this medium have more time
to process and monitor the interlanguage, I
believe that CMC can play a significant role in
the development of grammatical competence."
(Pelletieri, 2000, p. 83)
10Feedback in ICALL Research (1)
- Learners engage with computer programs that
incorporate natural language processing (NLP)
tools, such as syntactic parsers. - NLP tools allow for online processing of
responses and the provision of feedback that is
tailored to the students needs and highly
informative about the nature of their errors
(Nagata Swisher, 1995). - Tasks are highly artificial and bear little
resemblance to naturally occurring oral
interaction.
11Feedback in ICALL Research (2)
- Claims from ICALL research include
- Metalinguistic feedback in ICALL systems promotes
L2 grammatical development which is superior to
(1) traditional (pre-scripted) computer feedback
(Nagata, 1993 Nagata Swisher, 1995) and (2)
translation feedback (Nagata, 1997). - Metalinguistic feedback that highlights the error
in the student input (i.e. input enhancement) is
more effective at eliciting learner uptake than
recasts input enhancement (Heift, 2004).
12Recasts
- Recasts convey needed information about the
target language in context, when the
interlocutors share a joint attentional focus,
and when the learner already has prior
comprehension of at least part of the message,
thereby facilitating form-function mapping.
(Long, in press, p.46) - The efficacy of recasts for promoting language
development also lies in the immediate
juxtaposition of the learners error and the
correct reformulation provided by the more
advanced speaker (Farrar, 1990 Long, 1996
Saxton, 1997, 2005).
13Recasts (2)
- It is claimed that this juxtaposition of learner
error and subsequent reformulation -
- 1. Enhances the salience of the corrected
linguistic form in the feedback (Ishida, 2004
Saxton, 1997) - 2. Provides an opportunity for learners to make a
cognitive comparison between the targetlike model
in the recast and their own nontargetlike
production, thus promoting restructuring of their
interlanguage representation of the form (Long
Robinson, 1998 Mackey Philp, 1998 Oliver,
1995 Philp, 2003).
14Recasts (3)
- These claims about the efficacy of recasts assume
that the learner recognizes that the recasts are
intended as corrective feedback. - Although, it has been argued that
- Recasts are often not perceived as corrective
- feedback in some contexts (Lyster, 1998a
Lyster, - 1998b Lyster, 2004 Lyster Ranta, 1997).
- The implicit negative feedback contained in
recasts - may not be as crucial to L2 development as the
- enhanced salience of the positive evidence
they - provide (Leeman, 2003).
15Recasts Salience (1)
- According to Schmidts (Schmidt, 1990, 1995,
2001 Schmidt Frota, 1986) noticing
hypothesis, in order to become potential
candidates for intake and subsequent learning,
formal linguistic features in the input must
first be noticed by the learner (Schmidt, 1995,
p. 20). - When a learner's incorrect utterance and
subsequent recast convey the same meaning and
differ only in the use of a particular linguistic
form, that form is said to be made perceptually
more salient to the learner (Farrar, 1990 Long,
in press)
16Recasts Salience (2)
- Yet, the extent to which the increased salience
of a linguistic form may be a function of a
recast is constrained by a broad range of
factors, such as - The learners' developmental readiness vis-Ã -vis
the corrected form (Han, 2002 Iwashita, 2003
Mackey Philp, 1998 Philp, 2003) - The length of the recast provided (Philp, 2003
Sheen, 2004) - The number of corrections made in the
reformulation (Bohannon Stanowicz, 1988
Farrar, 1992 Philp, 2003) - The length of the negotiation sequence in which
it occurs (Ellis, Basturkmen and Loewen, 2001) - The degree of elaboration with which it is
delivered (Doughty Varela, 1998)
17Recasts Pedagogical Context (1)
- The salience and ambiguity of recasts are, to
some extent, dependent upon the pedagogical
context in which they appear (Nabei Swain,
2002 Nicholas, Lightbown Spada, 2001 Oliver,
1995 Sheen, 2004). - In communicative, immersion language classrooms,
where the instructional focus is primarily on
meaningful language production, recasts and
repetitions are often difficult for learners to
disambiguate (Lyster, 1998a Lyster, 1998b,
Lyster, 2004 Lyster Ranta, 1997)
18Recasts Pedagogical Context (2)
- In more form-focused classroom environments (e.g.
Ellis, Basturkmen Loewen, 2001 Ohta, 2000
Sheen, 2004) and especially in dyadic laboratory
contexts (e.g. Han, 2002 Iwashita, 2003 Long,
Inagaki, and Ortega, 1998 Mackey Philp, 1998
Philp, 2003) recasts may be more likely to be
noticed as corrective feedback. - One largely unexplored pedagogical context for
recasts is in the emergent environment of
interactive, computer-centered written discourse.
19Recasts Pedagogical Context (3)
- As Long (2006) notes, there is a need for
further research in this area because - (1) the written modality is a robust
environment for manipulating the degrees of
saliency of target items and - (2) findings from this line of research may have
important pedagogical implications for teachers
and materials developers in distance language
instruction programs.
20Recasts Pedagogical Context (4)
- Ayoun (2001) is the only published study to date
to explore the effects of recasts on L2
development in the written modality. - Ayoun found that written recasts produced
statistically significantly greater gain scores,
compared to explicit grammatical instruction, in
the aspectual distinction of distinction the
French passe compose and imparfait. - Relative to written models, exposure to written
recasts produced greater, though not significant
gains
21Recasts Pedagogical Context (5)
- Ayoun's findings, however, are difficult to
compare to findings on oral recasts for several
reasons -
- 1. Output was highly constrained and artificial.
- 2. Recasts were provided regardless of the
presence of - errors in the learners utterances.
- 3. No empirical claims were made about how the
written - modality may have impacted the
effectiveness of recasts.
22Overview
- This study will explore
- Â
- The effects of interactional mode (written vs.
oral) on the efficacy of recasts in promoting
both short and long-term developmental gains in
ESL question formation. - The extent to which interactional context might
influence learners' perceptions about the recasts
provided in response to their errors.
23Methodology Overview
-
-
- This study will employ a pre-test/post-test/delay
ed post-test design with a series of experimental
treatment tasks designed to isolate the context
of interactional mode (oral interaction vs.
computer-generated interaction) as the primary
independent variable.
24Participants
- Participants for this study will be recruited
from the English as a Foreign Language Intensive
Program at Georgetown University. All
participants will be offered monetary
compensation, and will be paid for each session
that they attend.
25Experimental Groups
- 1. Oral Interaction Recast Participants in this
group will participate in dyadic, communicative
tasks with a native speaker and receive intensive
recasts in response to their errors related to
question formation. - 2. Computer Guided Interaction Recast
Participants in this group will participate in
communicative tasks on a computer and receive
intensive recasts generated by the software in
response to their errors related to question
formation. - 3. Control Participants in this group will only
take the pre and post-tests.
26Target Form
- ESL question formation
- Well attested in SLA research (e.g. Adams, 2004
MacDonough, 2005 Mackey, 1999 Mackey
Philp,1999 Pienemann, 1998 Pienemann
Johnston, 1987 Pienemann, Johnston
Brindley,1988 Philp, 2003 Silver, 2000 Spada
Lightbown, 1993,1999 White, Spada, Lightbown
Ranta, 1991) - Well suited to focused feedback generated by NLP
tools.
27Developmental Sequences
28Operationalizing Recasts (1)
- Only errors related to question formation will be
corrected (targeting Stage 3-gt4 and Stage 4-gt5
development). - Lexical, phonological/typographic, false starts,
hesitations and all other errors in the learners'
speech and writing will not be subject to
corrective feedback. - No additional emphasis will be placed on the
source of the error in either written or oral
modes. - Recasts will reformulate ill-formed utterances in
their entirety (no partial or segmented recasts
will be provided). -
29Operationalizing Recasts (2)
- Following Long et al. (1998) and Leeman (2003),
no opportunity for modified output will be
provided in either mode. In both the oral and
computer guided interaction conditions, recasts
will be followed by a prompt to continue. - The decision to not allow for any uptake or
repair is motivated primarily by the desire to
control and isolate the effect of recasts from
those of modified output. - L2 development and learners' perceptions of
recasts will be measured by gains from pre-test
to post-test performance and reported noticing in
stimulated recall protocols.
30Experimental Schedule
31Treatment/Assessment Tasks (1)
- Tasks will be counter-balanced and will include
numerous contexts for Stage 4 and Stage 5
questions to occur. - Assessment tasks will be administered in both
oral and written modes to all groups. - Following the recommendation of Schneiderman
(1992), the computer-guided tasks will be created
in an anthropomorphic context to engage the
learner in oral-like interaction. - A virtual interlocutor will introduce each task,
allocate turns and provide recasts in response to
errors.
32Treatment/Assessment Tasks (2)
- The oral and written tasks will include
- Spot the difference
- Picture sequencing
- Picture Matching
33Example Task