Title: Rational
1Lack of Individual Difference in the Language
Growth Rate from Kindergarten to Eighth
Grade Xuyang Zhang and J. Bruce Tomblin The
University of Iowa, Iowa City
Measures The Item Response Theory (IRT) was used
to calibrate the item difficulty and
discriminating power and persons ability. Only
those items with adequate difficulty level were
entered into the analysis. (See Table 1 for
specific items used) An effort was also made to
balance the number of items measuring each of the
four language areas receptive vocabulary (R-V),
expressive vocabulary (E-V), receptive grammar
(R-G), and expressive grammar (E-G). Prior
analyses demonstrated that these items represent
one latent trait (Tomblin Zhang, 2001) Scores
at each observation interval (kindergarten, 2nd,
4th, 8th grades) were represented as Rasch
scores.
Results Unconditional Model This analysis
revealed that there were significant individual
differences for each parameter.
Growth Functions Growth consists of change over
time. The manner in which the variable of
interest (e.g. language ability) changes with
time can range from simple to complex. The
growth characteristics are described by equations
. The terms in the functions pertain to aspects
of the growth.
Table 2. Growth curve difference between groups.
Conditional Model The groups were significantly
different in starting level (a) rate to
reach the asymptote (1-e-ct) Groups were not
significantly different in the increment (b) (see
Table 2 and figures).
Asymptote
- Statistical Analysis
- Item analysis generated Rasch score of language
level for each child at each grade level. This
score is appropriate for individual growth curve
analysis. - Nonlinear Mixed Modeling from SAS permited the
use of exponential models for growth curve
analysis. Specifically the model employed is - E(yt)ab(1-e-ct)
- astarting level b(asymptote-starting level)
1-e-ct rate of growth
toward asymptote - Unconditioinal Model exponential growth curve
with three random parameters was fitted to the
data to determine if there were significant
individual differences among these parameters. - Conditional Model The diagnostic categories of
LI and Normal were added to the model to
determine whether the groups differed according
to the 3 parameters.
Rational Karmiloff-Smith (1998) argued that
development is the key to understanding
developmental disorders such as SLI. Recent
approaches to growth curve analysis provide an
important tool for describing the basic nature
of language growth.
- Leonard (1998) hypothesized that language growth
in children with SLI could differ from normal
language learners with respect to one or more of
the following parameters - initial levels of development (intercept)
- rate of growth (slope)
- presence and timing of asymptote
Linear growth has an intercept and slope but
does not capture the nonlinear decline in rate.
Thus, growth has no limiting property. Quadratic
growth provides for the nonlinear deceleration in
growth, but assumes that growth then
reverses. Exponential growth captures the
nonlinear aspects of growth without the reversal
in growth found in the quadratic. Unlike the
other functions, an exponential function has an
asymptotic component that represents the limit to
growth. Exponential growth appears to
characterize the data from Rice et al. and
Tomblin and Zhang studies.
Leonard noted that it was not clear whether the
model with or without asymptote was the most
appropriate for SLI
Issue The data from Rice et al. and Tomblin and
Zhang studies were fit with linear and quadratic
growth models. Inspection of the data from these
studies shows that the pattern of development
appears to be an inverse negative exponential or
logarithmic function. Thus, it would seem useful
to study language growth using an inverse
exponential function.
Table 1. Language items used for computation of
language scores
- Rice, Wexler, Hershberger (1998) modeled growth
in tense usage of children with SLI. - Growth of tense was found to be nonlinear for
children with and without SLI. - Significant heterogeneity was found among
children however, the two groups differed only in
their intercept, but no differences were found in
the linear or quadratic terms that reflect rate
of growth.
- Questions
- Does an exponential growth model fit
longitudinal language data? - In what ways does the exponential growth of
language in children with Language Impairment
(LI) differ from typically developing children? - Do children with language impairment eventually
catch up? That is, will children with language
impairment eventually reach the same asymptote as
other children or will their language
development asymptote be lower?
- Discussion
- After having entered kindergarten, most children
follow the same developmental trajectory.
Individual differences in growth during the
school years is mostly accounted for by the
starting level. This is the principal way
children with LI differ from normals. - The only other way children with LI differ from
normals is in the rate of growth toward
asymptote. LI children approach asymptote more
quickly. Thus, asymptote occurs earlier in LI
children than normals. Thus, LI children do not
catch up with normals. - Children with LI show the same amount of growth
during the school years as do normal children.
Thus, they actually have somewhat faster growth
during the early school years than normals, but
loose these gains by achieving asymptote earlier. - However, the group difference is not a clear cut.
Due to measurement error, there is a great
overlap between the two groups. Individual
identity is much more important than group
identity. - Overall, the growth characteristics of most
children during the school years is largely the
same, differing only in overall level. This
suggests considerable constraint in growth
characteristics despite substantial environmental
differences.
- Tomblin and Zhang (2001) examined the growth of
language in children with and without SLI from
kindergarten through fourth grade. - Group differences were found for intercept and
linear and quadratic terms for growth.
- Methods
- Participants
- 519 children assessed at four time points
Kindergarten, second grade, fourth grade, and
eighth grade. - At kindergarten, diagnosis was based on language
measures that were independent of those used for
measurement of growth. - 181 language impairment (LI) Language composite
score below -1.14. - 338 typically developing (TD) Language composite
score above -1.14. -