Title: General and Specific Cognitive Abilities
1General and Specific Cognitive Abilities
2Cognitive Abilities
- Specific cognitive abilities
- E.g., verbal ability, spatial ability, memory,
speed of processing - General cognitive ability (g)
- Often used to be called intelligence
3Hierarchical Models
- Very prevalent in cognitive sciences
- Work on the premise of interconnected levels
- Different units in each level
- Specific units might interconnect within and
between levels
4Hierarchy of Cognitive Ability
General cognitive ability (g)
Specific cognitive ability
Individual tests
5Interactions
- Hundreds of individual psychological tests used
in assessment - Moderate correlation between performance on
different specific cognitive abilities - E.g., do well on spatial, probably do well on
memory
6Correlations
- Not empirical correlation is not causation
- Correlations can not tell why/how one factor
relates to another, just the degree to which they
do (or do not)
7Genetic Regulation
- Not really much question that there is
heritability involved in cognitive ability - Specific gene and environmental control, however,
is still pretty much unknown in humans - Better understanding in nonhumans
- Empirical testing can be conducted
8Intelligence Testing
- Various intelligence (IQ) tests
- Alfred Binet
- Identify students needing special help
- First test, 1905
- Revised to Binet-Simon (1908, 1911), then
Stanford-Binet (1916)
9Studies in Human Intelligence
- Early adoption studies (Burks 1928 Leahy 1935)
- IQ correlates higher in nonadoptive families than
in adoptive families - Adopted away childrens IQ correlates with their
biological parents (Skodak Skeels 1949) - This is increasingly true as child ages
- 1960 Louisville Twin Study, longitudinal study of
environment and genetic effects begun
10Heritability and Intelligence Correlations
- First degree relatives, 0.45
- Adopted away children and biological parents,
0.25 - Sibs adopted apart, 0.25
- MZ, 0.85
- DZ, 0.6
- MZ raised apart, 0.75
11Couple Complications to This
- Assortative mating
- Nonadditive genetic variance
12Assortative Mating
- Non-random mating when mates have similar
features/characteristics - Important for our discussion
- Affects estimates of heritability
- In first-degree relatives can inflate
heritability - E.g., sibs are more similar in trait because
parents are similar for same trait - In twins, though, can underestimate heritability
- Raises DZ correlations because theyre 1st degree
relatives, so lessens difference b/t MZ and DZ
twins
13Nonadditive Genetic Variance
- Additive genetic effects
- Alleles at locus and across loci add up
- Nonadditive effects
- Effects of alleles different in presence of other
alleles - Dominance
- Alleles at same locus interact
- E.g., heterozygous phenotype different from
homozygous dominant phenotype - Epistasis
- Alleles at different loci interact to affect
behaviour phenotype of different genes
suppressed or expressed - Emergenesis
- Epistatic effects producing extraordinary
effects wont be heritable due to interactive
nature
14General Intelligence
- Charles Spearman
- Schoolchildrens grades across unrelated subjects
positively correlated - Proposed general intelligence
- Initial interpretation that variation in
intelligence due to - Factor specific to an individual mental task
- A general factor, g, that governs performance on
all cognitive tasks - Ignored group factors, however need factor
analysis to identify this
15g-Factor
- Is g real?
- What is the actual interaction between specific
and general cognition? - Correlations
16g-Loading
- Tests of cognitive ability derive most of their
validity from the extent to which they measure g - g-loaded if quantifiable measure(s) of a task
correlate highly with g - Primary goal of IQ tests is to create reliable
and valid tests thus, the tests tend to be
intentionally g-loaded
17Non-specificity
- However, g not specific to any particular domain
of knowledge or mental skill - Also, seems independent of cultural content
- Support idea that g is real and not simply an
artifact of particular opportunities to learn
specific skill sets
18Biological Correlates
- Brain size correlate with g, 0.4
- Various brain wave activity and g, 0.5-0.7
- Speed of nerve conduction with g, 0.4
- Even elementary cognitive tasks (ECTs) correlate
with g (tasks like identify the colour of a
light, number of figures on a page, etc.)
19g
- g widely accepted
- Seems to have moderate to high heritability
- That said, less clear what g really is
- Single general process?
- E.g., information processing speed, executive
function - Interaction/intersection of specific cognitive
functions? - Frequently, g used synonymously with
intelligence
20QTL and g
- Highly likely that many separate components
contribute - Polygenic
- Environment
- Effect at what level?
- Elementary properties, specific cognitive
ability, general cognitive ability
21Top down
- Genes act directly on g
- E.g., perhaps through neural activity speed, etc.
General cognitive ability
Genes
Specific cognitive abilities
Elementary processes
22Bottom Up
- Genes affect each basic element of information
processing - Highly reductionistic model
General cognitive ability
Specific cognitive abilities
Genes
Elementary processes
23Multi-level Interaction
- Unique genetic effects at each level, but also
genetic effects in common across levels
General cognitive ability
Genes
Genes
Specific cognitive abilities
Genes
Elementary processes
24Evidence
- Some support for top down
- Modularized view of brain function would fit well
with bottom up - However, multivariate genetic analysis supports
multi-level interaction - Keep in mind, this model incorporates elements
from both top down and bottom up
25Non-human Animal Models
- Can look for g-like abilities in non-humans
- Look for specific cognitive abilities that are
directly comparable across species (e.g., spatial
ability)
26Maze Dull/Maze Bright
- Tolman and Tyron
- Selectively bred rats for ability in maze
learning - Maze bright rats showed few errors, maze dull
rats many errors after few generations
Maze dull
20
Mean Errors
Maze bright
0
1
22
Generations
27Heritability for Learning
Bovet et al. (1969)
28Heritability in Learning
Bovet et al. (1969)
- In and of itself, not that novel, unexpected,
surprising - But, environmental effects can come in
29Genotype-Environment Interaction
- Cooper Zubek (1958)
- Enriched, restricted, standard lab conditions
- Enriched improves MD, not MB
- Restricted detrimental to MB, not MD
30Popularity of Mice
- Mouse genome
- Can test for specific gene effects
- E.g., transgenic critters
- Very useful for genotype-environment interactions
with respect to cognitive abilities - Obviously, more difficult in humans, but starting
to get there
31Caspi et al. (2007)
- Childrens intellectual development
- Interaction of genetic and environmental
experience - Breastfeeding
- IQ scores
32Breastfeeding
- Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LC-PUFAs)
- Present in human milk, absent in cows milk
- Specifically, DHA (docosahexaenoic acid) and ARA
(arachidonic acid) - Required for efficient neurotransmission, neurite
outgrowth, dendritic arborization, and neuron
regeneration post cell injury - DHA and ARA accumulate in human brain in early
postnatal months - Higher concentrations in breastfed than formula
fed infants
33Effect on IQ
- Breastfed children have higher IQs than
non-breastfed children - Effect persists into adulthood
- Not due to SES or other culture-specific factors
- Important to control for, as in Western
countries, higher SES is related to higher IQ,
and higher SES women are more likely to breastfeed
34Non-human Animal Models
- Animals deprived in n-3 fatty acids show neuronal
deficits in memory, sensory, and visual abilities - DHA supplementation in rodents and nonhuman
primates increases DHA concentrations enhances
performance on learning, memory, and problem
solving tasks
35FADS2
- Chromosome 11 candidate gene
- Role in modification of dietary fatty acids
- Encodes delta-6 desaturase, the rate limiting
step on the metabolic pathway for ARA and DHA
production - Hypothesis cognitive advantage of breastfeeding
related to genetic differences in LC-PUFA
metabolism, specifically at FADS2
36Markers and Subjects
- Used two SNPs
- Genetic polymorphisms rs174575 and rs1535
- Strong linkage disequilibrium through promoter
and intragenic region of FADS2 (and also FADS1,
another gene involved in fatty acid metabolism) - Over 1000 New Zealand children born 1972-73, IQ
measures at age 7, 9, 11, 13 - Over 2200 children from British twins born
1994-95 IQ measured at age 5
37IQ Outcomes and Genotype
110 105 100 95 90
New Zealand Cohort
British Cohort
Mean IQ
CC CG GG CC CG GG Genotypes
Not breastfed
Breastfed
Overall, breastfed children had IQ scores 5.6 and
6.3 points higher than non-breastfed children in
New Zealand and British cohorts, respectively.
About 90 either CC or CG.
38Genotype and IQ
- Dominant effect of C allele in response to
breastfeeding - New Zealand breastfed children with C allele
showed 6.4 IQ-point advantage (plt0.001) compared
to non-breastfed children GG homozygotes gained
no advantage from breastfeeding - British breastfed children with C allele showed
7.0 IQ-point advantage (plt0.001) GG had no
advantage from breastfeeding - Averaging, this equates to a 6.8 IQ point
advantage, or 0.48 standard deviation units in
the general population
39rs174575
- Genetic moderation of breastfeeding effects on IQ
not likely directly due to rs174575 - Actual molecular mechanism of influence by
rs174575 is currently unknown - May be that rs174575 influences biosynthesis of
LC-PUFAs from dietary precursors, possibly
through increased transcriptional activity
40Application
- Earlier studies looking at neurodevelopment of
infants fed DHA-supplemented vs. unsupplemented
formula - Results inconclusive
- Current research may offer explanation genetic
heterogeneity in fatty acid metabolism may dilute
supplemental effects
41Application
- FADS2 locus has not appeared on the first
genome-wide scans for intelligence - Such scans identify genes with associations with
phenotypes regardless of participants
environments ineffective for detecting genes
whose effects are conditional on environmental
exposure - In contemporary Western samples, significant
portion of population is not breastfed this
would conceal link between FADS2 variation and IQ
42Heritability and Maturation
- Early twin studies investigated development
(e.g., Galton, 1876 Merriman, 1924) - Heredity increasingly important as you develop
43Developing Twins
44Why
- New genes come into effect
- Positive feedback effect
- IQ increase when adopted by parents with high IQ
- Intellectual experience more self-directed as an
adult - Shared environment effects decrease with age
45Genetic Contributions to Developmental Change
- g is pretty stable, not perfectly so if change
happens, it has a genetic aspect - Genetic effects seem to act at transitional ages
- Infancy to early childhood (e.g., language
acquisition) - Early to middle childhood (e.g., theory of mind)
- Etc.
Gen. factors New gen. factors New gen. factors
Infancy Early childhood Middle childhood
Shared Env.
46Environment Specific Cognition
- Scarr Weinberg (1978)
- Adoption study
- Little similarity for adoptive parents and
adopted children or between adopted siblings on
specific subsets of intelligence test except
vocabulary - Like g, specific cognitive abilities also little
influenced by shared environment (i.e.,
heritability significant factor)
47Academic Performance
- Achievement vs. ability
- Semantics?
- Shared environment 60, heritability 30 (for
6-12 year range) - Heritability effect does increase, and
environment effect decreases with age
48Heritability and Subjects
From Grade 7 Report Card Grades
From High School Achievement Tests
Twin Correlations Subject MZ DZ
History .80 .51 Reading
.72 .57 Writing .76 .50 Arithmetic
.81 .48
Twin Correlations Subject
MZ DZ Social .69 .52 Natural Sciences
.64 .45 English use .72 .52 Mathematics
.71 .51
49School Achievement g?
- Multivariate genetic analysis shows a common
genetic effect explains much of the correlation
between scores in different domains (i.e.,
subjects) - Is this g, or some other measure?
- Some-to-much of this is g, but some is
achievement specific - Implies that achievement scores (within normal
range) that are not due to ability are largely
due to environment
50Overall
- Variance in thirds
- One third of genetic variance of academic
performance is in common with general cognitive
ability - One third of genetic variance is general to
academic performance, independent of general
cognitive ability - One third is specific to each domain
- Means learning abilities are not exactly the same
thing genetically as general cognitive ability