Title: The Headturn Preference Paradigm
1The Headturn Preference Paradigm
- Barbara Höhle
- University of Potsdam
- and
- Free University of Berlin
2The headturn preference paradigm
- First use
- Hirsh-Pasek, K. et al. (1987) Clauses are
perceptual units for infants. Cognition 26,
269-286.
3(No Transcript)
4Features of the paradigm
- acoustic stimuli are presented from one of two
loudspeakers on the sides of the testing booth - an at least 45 headturn should be necessary for
the child to fixate the lamp - times of turning away are not included into the
calculation of the orientation time - a trial is finished when the child looks away for
more than two seconds
5Features of the paradigm
- it is not possible to present single stimuli in
this paradigm - to create enough variation the acoustic stimuli
should have a duration of 16 to 20 seconds - this duration can be achieved either by
repetitions of a single stimulus or by the use of
a sequence of different words or sentences
6Rationale of the Paradigm
- the child learns the contingency of her headturn
and the stimulus presentation - the more interesting the stimulus is for the
child the longer the duration of the headturn - results can be interpreted as reflecting a
preference for one of the types of stimuli
presented - results also reflect the ability of the infants
to discriminate the types of stimuli
7The two basic versions of the method
- Without familiarisation
- experiment starts immediately with presentation
of the test stimuli (warming-ups) - with familiarisation
- the child is presented with a specific kind of
stimulus at the beginnig of the experiment (e.g.
two words) until a criterion has been reached - immediately after the familiarisation the testing
phase is started
8What to use these versions for?
- without familiarization
- reflects the spontaneous preference of the child
for one of the types of stimuli used - gives an insight into the knowledge the child
brings into the experiment - e.g. presentation of grammatical and
ungrammatical sentencs (Santelmann Jusczyk,
1998) - e.g. presentation of phonotactically legal and
illegal pseudowords (Friederici Wessels, 1993) - e.g. presentation of trochaic and iambic words
(Jusczyk, Cutler Redanz, 1993)
9What to use these versions for?
- with familiarization
- has first been used to study word detection
(e.g. presentation of isolated words in the
familiarization, presentation of passages with or
without these words in the test phase Jusczyk
Aslin, 1995) - can also be used to study learning mechanisms
- (e.g. presentation of a continuous speech stream
during familiarization, presentation of parts of
the stream and new stimuli during testing
(Saffran, Aslin Newport, 1996)
10What to use these versions for?
- to study discrimination abilities
- (e.g. familiarization/habituation with one type
of stimulus, testing with this old stimulus and
an additional one familiarization with syllable
/ba/, testing with /ba/ and /pa/, differences in
orientation time show discrimination
11Subjects
- no special criteria for subject selection
(monolingual, fullterm etc.) - ages tested 4 month-olds to 24 month-olds
- drop-out rates depending on age
- best for 6 to 9-month-olds (15 to 20)
12Dos and dont dos
- make sure that the experimenter is blind with
respect to the condition to which a given trial
belongs - make sure that the parents do not hear the
acoustic stimuli - make sure that the experiment is run in a quite,
smoothly illuminated room - make sure that the child does not carry toys or
nipples into the experiment - make sure that the child is healthy when coming
to the experiment
13Dos and dont dos
- tell the parents that they should chancel their
date if they have the feeling that their child
does not feel well - if the child and the mother are seated in the
experimental booth start the experiment
immediately - make sure that the types of stimuli you are using
are presented from both sides for an equal number
of times - make sure that the number of stimulus
presentations from both sides is balances
14Dos and dont dos
- make sure to use different randomizations because
there are very strong order effects in the
results - use warming-up trials at the beginning of the
experiment (two to four) to make the child
familiar to the procedure
15Dos and dont dos
- dont play around with the child too much before
starting the experiment - there should not be many people around the child
before the experiment starts - dont put too many trials into one single
experimental session (around 16 seems to be
perfect)
16Possible Outcomes Familiarity vs. Novelty-Effects
- Familiarity-Effect
- preference for the familiar stimulus
- e.g. grammatical over ungrammatical stimulus
- e.g. familiarized words over new words
- Novelty-Effect
- preference for the new stimulus
- e.g. new words over familiarized words
- e.g. new contexts over old contexts
17Possible Outcomes Familiarity vs. Novelty-Effects
- Familiarity Effects (examples)
- word detection Jusczyk Aslin, 1992 Jusczyk et
al. 1999 - grammaticality Santelmann Juzscyzk, 1990
- phonotactics Friederici Wessels, 1993 Jusczyk
et al. 1993) - Novelty-Effects (examples)
- word detection Saffran et al. 1996 Echols et
al. 1997) - grammaticality Höhle et al. (in press)
18Modelling the direction of the preference (Ames
Hunter, 1988)
- initially infants show no preference for one of
the stimuli - at first a preference for the familiar stimulus
arises - during the experiment this preference changes to
the novel stimulus - whether a familiarity or a novelty effect is
observed in an experiment depends on how long the
children stay in these phases
19An experiment on rhythm discrimination
- 4-month-olds
- familiarization with trochaic bisyllabic BA-ga
for 30s - during test phase presentation of trials
consisting only of trochaic or of iambic
bisyllabics
20(No Transcript)
21An experiment on rhythm discrimination
22An experiment on rhythm discrimination
23Modelling the direction of the preference (Ames
Hunter, 1988)
- there are several factors influencing how quick
these phases are passed - age of the children with growing age a novelty
effect becomes more probable - variability of the stimuli the more variable the
stimuli the higher the probability for a
familiarity effect - complexity of the stimuli the more complex the
stimuli the higher the probability for a
familiarity effect
24Evidence for the model
- word detection
- Juscyzk and coworkers real speech stimuli in
different contexts familiarized for 45 s
familiarity effect - Saffran et al. sequences of simple CV-syllables
repeated for 2 minutes during familiarization
novelty effect
25Using other dependent variables with this paradigm
- Headturn latency (Orientation Latency Procedure
Gout, Christophe Dupoux, 2002) - each trial consisted of familiarization stimulus
(presented centrally) - in the test phase two different kind of stimuli
were presented laterally either from the same
category as the familiarization stimulus (control
trials) or from another category (test trials) - dependent variable latency of headturn
initiation from beginning of the presentation of
the acoustic stimulus, headturn was reinforced by
a flashing light - result longer latencies for control than for
test trials
26Using other dependent variables with this paradigm
- headturn direction (Hirsh-Pasek et al. 1986)
- fixed presentation side for different types of
stimuli - child is familiarized to the presentation side at
the beginning of the experiment - at the beginning of a trial lights on both sides
flash - stimulus corresponding to that side is started
when the child turns her head to the side - child controls which kind of stimulus is
presented by headturn - results no systematic preference for one of the
types of stimuli
27Summary
- Pros
- different kind of domains can be studied
- wide age range
- relatively low drop-out rates
- high inter-rater reliability (90 - 98)
- Cons
- restricted number of trials restricted number
of experimental conditions (normally two) gtgt
between subjects designs - no clear prediction of direction for a given
experiment possible
28(No Transcript)