Title: Eurosla 11, Paderborn, 26-29 September
1Eurosla 11, Paderborn, 26-29 September
Re-evaluating Theoretical and Methodological
Aspects of Focus on Form Research
- Danijela Trenkic Mike Sharwood Smith
- Heriot-Watt University, Edinburgh
2Background Focus on Form research
- the principle aim
- to investigate whether focusing learners
attention to formal aspects of language in
communicative context can (in some cases) promote
SLA. - theoretical underpinnings
- the noticing hypothesis (Schmidt 1990) only what
has been noticed will be learned - goals of research
- pedagogical
- theoretical
3Typical design of FonF studies
- Classroom studies
- typically, two or more classrooms of FL/SL
learners at the same (or similar) proficiency
level are pretested, and then exposed to a
different instructional treatment over a period
of time.
4Their improvement on a post-test is then compared
Control Group no FonF treatments
Post-test
Pre-test
Experimental Group receives some sort of FonF
treatment
5Typical findings and generalisations
- Learners who receive FonF treatment perform
better on the post-test - FonF - an effective means of promoting noticing
- noticing the key factor of SLA
- FonF should be included in language teaching
6Problems with FonF research design, findings and
generalisations
- Theoretical problems
- how do you define noticing?
- Schmidt (1990) noticing/understanding
distinction - the distinction is scalar, rather than
categorical - what is it that a learner has to notice?
- how do you measure noticing?
7Methodological problems
- A whole plethora of uncontrollable variables
involved - impossible to interpret the results confidently
(you are not sure why something worked, and even
more importantly, why something did not work) - impossible to compare studies
- impossible to draw generalisations
8Major variables in FonF studies
- The choice of forms
- learners proficiency levels
- the size and nature of groups
- the length of treatment
- the number of post-tests
- the choice of testing materials
- the level of explicitness
9The choice of forms
- Not all linguistic features are equally amenable
to focus on form (cf. Williams and Evans 1998) - the results obtained on one form are no guarantee
that the same treatments would work equally well
for another form
10Learners proficiency levels
- What work for learners at a certain level of
proficiency may not work for learners at another
level
11The size and nature of groups
- Groups not usually big enough for reliable
statistical analysis and conclusions ( the
intergroup differences in proficiency,
motivation, etc. can be considerable). - since the sample sizes were fairly small and the
data were not normally distributed (Doughty and
Varela 1998129) - BUT the effects of FonF treatment are clearly
interpretable from our results (ibid.) - This sample is too small to provide convincing
quantitative evidence (Williams and Evans
1998151) - BUT the results point to the fact that focus
on form is indeed useful and should be integrated
into communicative curricula. (ibid.)
12The length of treatment
- A few days/several weeks/whole semesters
- Effects for instruction of any kind may be, and
probably almost are, gradual and cumulative
rather than instantaneous and categorical (Long
and Robinson 199840) - results are affected by the length of treatments
13The number of post-tests
- There is a reverse side to the gradual
accumulation of the effect of instruction - FonF groups often improve their performance on
the immediate post-test, but on (sufficiently)
delayed post-tests, this improvement decreases,
or even disappears altogether. - White (1991) - found positive effects for a FonF
instruction on the 5 week post-test, but not on
the post-test administered a year later. - The last post-test rarely exceeds 5 weeks
- BUT instruction that appropriately incorporates
form-focused treatments into communication-oriente
d language teaching can have lasting positive
effect on L2 acquisition (Muranoi 2000661)
14The choice of testing materials
- Testing materials come in a variety of forms
- oral and written reports on science experiments
(Doughty and Varela 1998), short constrained
narratives based on several pictures (Williams
and Evans 1998, Muranoi 2000), more or less
constrained sentence-completion tasks (ibid.),
different varieties of grammaticality judgement
tasks (ibid.), description of a short film-scene
(Muranoi 2000), etc. - different testing materials yield somewhat
different results ? when comparing the results,
no guarantee that like is being compared with
like - the central problem what are the testing
materials testing?
15The level of explicitness of FonF treatment
- it has not been clear exactly what it means to
draw a learners attention to form or how this is
to be accomplished. (Williams and Evans
1998139) - Accomplished in many ways from the most implicit
ones (e.g. the flood of positive evidence), to
the most explicit ones (e.g. stating a rule)
16The level of explicitness
- Original proposal FonF should occur incidentally
and be fairly implicit, so as not to distract
learners from their communicative goal (cf. Long
1991). - a quintessential element of the theoretical
construct of focus on form is its dual
requirement that the focus must occur in
conjunction with - but must not interrupt -
communicative interaction. (Doughty and Varela
1998114) - more explicit procedures may cause stress and
anxiety, and so preclude fluency. This is because
they do not add attention to form to a primarily
communicative task but rather depart from an
already communicative goal in order to discuss a
linguistic features (ibid.)
17The levels of explicitness
- The general trend emerging from FonF studies
employing a whole range of FonF techniques seems
to be that the more explicit the treatment, the
more marked the gain on the post-test.
18Some examples
- Williams and Evans (1998) considered English
participial adjectives - found that ESL learners who received a flood of
positive evidence, plus explicit instruction,
plus feedback, significantly outperformed the
group which only received a flood of positive
evidence, which in turn, outperformed but not
significantly, the control group which did not
undergo any FonF treatment. - Muranoi (2000) considered English articles
- found that Japanese EFL learners show much better
results in using E articles after receiving an
implicit interaction enhancement treatment, but
even better when the treatment is supported by
explicit formal instruction.
19Result a split in the theory of FonF research
- Long and Doughty - still advocate exclusively
implicit techniques - Lightbown - argues for a role for grammatical
instruction that is separate from communicative
activities, and is yet integrated in the lesson
as a whole. (1998194) - DeKeyser - advocates explicit instruction at
first, and believes that declarative knowledge
acquired through explicit FonF instruction can
eventually become fully automated (199847).
Communicative interaction is vital for the
process of proceduralising declarative knowledge.
20Failing on the pedagogical aim
- Due to a great number of uncontrollable variables
in research, all teachers can be told is yes,
the results show that focusing your students
attention to form may work, but you have to work
out what will work for YOUR students. - Not exactly helpful or revealing
21Failing on the theoretical aim
- Not much has been revealed about the process of
SLA. - Since it is not properly defined what noticing is
or how it is to be measured ? the hypothesis is
not falsifiable
22Failing on the theoretical aim
- Further, findings from FonF research show that
- the more explicit the instruction, the more
marked the effect on a post-test - effects are not preserved in spontaneous
production (cf. the choice of testing materials
above), or on sufficiently delayed post-tests - overgeneralised uses of the treated form are a
regular by-product of FonF research. - These are characteristics of meta-linguistic
learning/knowledge!
23Implications of these findings
- FonF treatments, irrespective of their level of
explicitness, actually manipulate meta-linguistic
knowledge, that is knowledge about language,
rather than knowledge of language (cf. Truscott
1998). - Similarly, meta-linguistic knowledge is what FonF
research testing materials test. - The only safe conclusion noticing does promote
learning, but of meta-linguistic type. It does
not seem to promote learning OF language (and we
believe there are good theoretical reasons why it
doesnt)
24The future of FonF research?
- More research of the same type?
- OR defining a viable model of SLA with a clear
set of testable predictions? - We would advocate the second choice.
- The most theoretically grounded and
methodologically worked-out line of research
within the FonF framework so far, has been that
of DeKeyser (1998), in Andersons ACT framework.
The idea is to see whether declarative knowledge,
acquired through FonF instruction, can be
proceduralised by engaging in target behaviour
while temporarily leaning on declarative
crutches (199849)
25The future of FonF classroom practice
- We believe that there is a place for FonF
instruction and feedback in language classroom,
despite the fact that it produces meta-linguistic
knowledge. - Learners may have very limited exposure to the TL
(especially FL learners) they may learn in huge
groups, a few hours a week, and be taught by a
non-native speaker of that language ? there may
not be enough input to develop knowledge of
language. - Learners have practical goals - e.g. to pass the
exam, get a job, etc., and these goals can be
achieved by developing meta-linguistic knowledge
(it is actually more than likely that
meta-linguistic knowledge is what is going to be
tested by the testing materials). - There is a possibility that it can ultimately
influence knowledge of language - a question to
be theoretically and empirically addressed by
future FonF research.