Title: The NatureNurture Debate
1The Nature-Nurture Debate
Approaches to Psychology Lecture 26, The
Nature-Nurture Debate
2Lecture contents
- Group differences in IQ.
- Population variation in IQ Heritability and the
environment. - What can and cannot be inferred from heritablity
indices. - Culture and intelligence testing.
- Modifying IQ scores.
- My tentative conclusions.
3The U.S. Immigration Act of 1924
- Rigid quotas to restrict the influx of
biologically weaker stock from southern and
eastern Europe. - The proof? Members of these groups had lower Army
Intelligence Scores than obtained by Northern
European-Americans. - These scores turned out to correlate with length
of time in the States, improving with English
language proficiency and AUS cultural knowledge. - After years of residency, European are of origin
had no discernible effect. - A seemingly hereditary difference was revealed as
due to environmental factors related to genetic
ones.
4Group differences
- Numerous studies have found that average IQ for
U.S. blacks is 85-90, i.e., 10 to 15 points lower
than for U.S. whites. - The dispute revolves around why this is so and
whether much can be done about it. - The difference in average racial IQs decreases
proportionate to the extent that environments are
relevantly matched (e.g., on SES dimensions). - Also, average IQ for U.S. blacks adopted into
white middle-class households is 110 (25 points
higher than average blacks, 10 points higher than
average whites). - Thus, differences in group means seem at least
largely to be due to different environments. - The rest of the lecture concerns variation in
individual scores within given populations.
5A distinction and an interaction
- Genotypic traits are genetic potentialities.
- Phenotypic traits are manifest characteristics.
- All phenotypic traits are often claimed to be the
result of an interaction between the genotype and
the environment - There can be no organism without a genotype, and
this genotype cannot express itself independently
of the environment (Gleitman et al., 1999, p.
655) - Organisms are dependent on the genotype, but are
all traits so dependent once the organism exists?
6Heritability indices
- a heritability index shows the proportional
contribution of genetic or heriditary factors to
the total variance of a particular trait in a
given population under specific conditions - Anastasi Urbina (1997, p. 297)
- E.g., A heritability index of .70 means that 70
of the variance is attributable to hereditary
factors (with the remainder being attributable to
the environment).
7How heritability indices are calculated
- Primarily, by examining resemblances among
genetically related people. E.g., - Twin studies
- 1) Compare monozygotic and dizygotic twins.
- 2) Compare siblings (especially monozygotic twins
reared together and apart. - - Adoption studies
- Compare adopted children with both biological and
adopted relatives.
8Correlations between IQ scores for family members
9Difficulties with heritability indices
- Unassessed contributions of environmental
factors. - Monozygotic (identical) twins may share more
similar environments than do dyzygotic
(fraternal) twins. - Distal environments can be similar and close ones
rather different. - There may be genetic influences on environments.
- Issues of generalisability from twins to
non-twins. - E.g., Twins more likely to have prenatal trauma
leading to severe mental retardation (and
inclusion of such people raises heritability
indices).
10Inheriting differences
- Heritability coefficients are estimates of genes
on individual differences within a population.
They are not estimates of the relative importance
of genes and the environment. - Westen (1998, p. 348)
- They indicate determinants of variances
(differences) not means (similarities).
11Inheriting differences from differences
- Heritability estimates are highly dependent on
the sample. - Westen (1998, p. 348)
- Heritability indices are frequently obtained from
relatively homogeneous environments but
relatively genetically diverse people. - Including a random sample of people from various
environments would typically lower heritability
indices.
12Inheriting differences from specific
gene-situation interactions
- Any change in either hereditary or environmental
conditions would be liable to alter the
heritability index. - Anastasi Urbina (1997, p. 298)
- E.g., making either the gene pool or the
environment more or less homogeneous in ways that
affect the characteristic of interest (e.g.,
intelligence).
13Average inheritance effects
- Almost irrespective of the size of a
heritability index in the population, a
particular individuals characteristics may be
almost entirely determined by their genes, their
environment, or any combination of the two. - E.g., intelligence is largely hereditary, but any
particular instance of extraordinarily high or
low (or even average) intelligence may result
from extraordinary genetic conditions,
extraordinary environmental conditions, or both.
14Indicating actualities, not potentials
- Even if the heritability index of a trait in a
given population is 100, it does not follow that
the contribution of environment to that trait is
unimportant. - Anastasi Urbina (1997, p. 298)
- It simply follows that the environment is having
no differential effect across individuals in that
population.
15Cultural bias in IQ tests
- Linguistic familiarity.
- Vocabulary, dialect.
- Content familiarity.
- Information, concepts, tasks
- Medium familiarity.
- Paper and pencil, interview
- Test motivation.
- Every test tends to favour persons from the
culture in which it was developed - Anastasi Urbina (1997, p. 342)
16Modifying IQ scores
- IQ is not fixed it is amenable to modification.
- Educationally impoverished environments depress
IQ. - Interventions (and other factors) that richen
such environments increase IQ. - Worldwide improvements in IQ scores over time 3
IQ points every 10 years (The Flynn effect).
17My (tentative) conclusions
- Substantially overlapping variation in IQ scores
across groups makes differential treatment of
members of those groups an example of prejudice. - Whatever the difference in the means and the
reasons for such difference. - Evidence for genetic differences in average IQ
across groups is poor. - (I have encountered no relevant evidence about
possible current differences in IQ heritability
across groups.) - Evidence for environmental differences in average
IQ across groups is strong. - Evidence for (current) genetic differences in IQ
scores within groups is strong. - Evidence for the ability to influence IQ scores
environmentally is strong. - All the above holds for intelligence as well as
for IQ, only more so. - Overall conclusion From social science and
societal perspectives, it makes sense to look for
both genetic and environmental determinants of
intellectual and other important functioning. - There is no compelling evidence that any such
functioning is genetically determined. - As a consequence of this, it is better to
recognize inhibitors and enhancers, and then
learn how to potentially overcome the former and
exploit the latter.
18Points to ponder
- Environmental improvements can increase average
IQ scores. Would you predict that they also
affect heritability indices for IQ with the
relevant populations. If not, why not and, if so,
why (and how)? - How convincing is Gleitmans argument (p. 662)
that studies such as Quay (1971) demonstrate that
cultural bias in IQ tests is evidently not
responsible for average racial difference in IQ
scores? Hint content (esp. information) bias.