Reducing accidents and promoting safety behaviours - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 58
About This Presentation
Title:

Reducing accidents and promoting safety behaviours

Description:

... messages were that buckling up for every ride is a good thing for everyone to do ... UNDERSTANDING MEDICAL INSTRUCTIONS. We live in a world full of icons ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:228
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 59
Provided by: stu9
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Reducing accidents and promoting safety behaviours


1
Reducing accidents and promoting safety
behaviours
  • Health Psychology

2
Road Safety
  • Where young children are concerned, it is usually
    adults who have to take responsibility for
    promoting safe behaviours and preventing
    accidents. It is therefore adults who are
    targeted in health promotion and accident
    prevention campaigns rather than children. There
    are some messages that can be conveyed to
    children such as road safety - for example, the
    Green Cross Code in the 1970s. More messages are
    being taught in schools through Personal, Social
    and Health Education (PSE).

3
Wortel et al (1994)
  • Wortel et al (1994) describe four safety
    behaviours that parents can engage in that
    prevent accidents among pre-school children
  • 1.      Educating the child about risks
  • 2.      Supervision of the child
  • 3.      Making sure that the child's
    environment is safe
  • 4.      Giving first aid when an accident has
    happened.

4
Commentary
  • It is difficult to make a child understand the
    nature of risk. It is almost impossible to ensure
    constant supervision, and also does not allow the
    child to explore the environment and learn from
    its mistakes. Making the environment safe is the
    best choice.

5
Langley and Silva (1982)
  • Langley and Silva (1982) found that only 39 of
    parents whose child had had an accident in the
    pre-school period changed their behaviour to
    prevent further accidents. Most of the parents
    who did not change their behaviour did not feel
    that it was possible to prevent the accident.

6
Commentary
  • The problem with an approach that focuses on the
    role of the parent is that it lays blame on these
    parents, instead of recognizing the need for a
    safe environment to be provided for everyone.

7
Commentary
  • For example, if we recognize that children who
    grow up in deprived homes are more likely to have
    accidents than those who do not, then we often
    lay the blame for that statistic on negligent
    parents, rather than looking at the environment
    in which these parents are forced to bring up
    their children - in high-rise flats or on housing
    estates near main roads, for example.

8
Laws
  • The promotion of safe behaviours can be more
    effective if laws are passed.

9
learning theory approach
  • Oborne (1982) (cited in Pitts, 1996) uses a
    learning theory approach to understanding safety.
    He argues that often safety routines and
    practices take a lot of time, and that these
    behaviours are less likely to be reinforced than
    behaviours that are often quicker and easier,
    although more risky.

10
Pitts (1996)
  • Pitts (1996) lists the following accident
    prevention actions as the most important
  •         To eliminate the hazards from the
    workplace
  •         To remove the individual from exposure
  •         To isolate the hazard
  •         Workers can be issued with personal
    protection - such as protective clothing.
  • The emphasis in this model is that the
    management, rather than the individual should
    take the action.

11
The use of cycle helmets
  • In Maryland (USA), the use of cycle helmets was
    compared in three counties
  • one in which a law had been passed in 1990 making
    it mandatory for everyone under the age of 16 to
    wear an approved helmet
  • one in which publicity about proposed legislation
    was widespread,
  • and one in which there were no laws or publicity.

12
The use of cycle helmets
  • Using self-report measures, the increase in
    helmet use rose from 11.4 to 37.5, 8.4 to
    12.6 and 6.7 to 11.1 respectively.

13
The use of cycle helmets
14
The use of cycle helmets
  • Observations of the use of cycle helmets in the
    three counties found slightly different
    increases from 4 to 47, 8 to 19, and in the
    county with no laws or publicity, there was a
    decrease during the period of survey.

15
The use of cycle helmets
  • In one state in Australia, after the wearing of
    cycle helmets was made compulsory there was an
    immediate increase in helmet use from 31 in
    March 1990 to 75 a year later. The number of
    cyclists killed from head injuries decreased by
    48 in the first year, and by 70 in the second
    year.

16
motor vehicle accidents
  • The death rates for motor vehicle accidents
    increase dramatically during adolescence, as
    depicted in the graph, and males between 15 and 9
    years of age are about 2½ times more likely to
    die in traffic mishaps than females in the same
    age range (Matarazzo, 1984).

17
motor vehicle accidents
18
Safe-driving programs
  • Because of the high rates of traffic fatalities
    in adolescence, special safe-driving programs
    have been directed toward teenagers. One approach
    has involved providing driver training in high
    schools, and early quasi-experimental research
    showed that students who take driver education
    courses subsequently have fewer accidents than
    those who do not.

19
Safe-driving programs
  • But later studies revealed that the course itself
    was not the cause of this relationship for some
    reason, students who elect to take driver
    education simply drive less than those who do not
    (Robertson, 1986). Similarly, driver education
    for adults-for example, as a condition for
    employment or in response to traffic
    violations---also seems to have little effect on
    accidents.

20
Perception and reaction
  • Other ways to reduce traffic accidents have been
    more effective than driver training. One approach
    capitalizes on research findings regarding
    drivers' perceptual and reaction abilities, with
    the goal of reducing their errors and enhancing
    their reaction time. Public health researcher
    Leon Robertson has described two examples

21
Perception and reaction
  • (1)   An extra brake light mounted in the centre
    of the vehicle above the trunk resulted in a 50
    reduction in rear-end collisions when the front
    vehicle was braking, compared to randomly
    assigned control cars in the same fleets.

22
Perception and reaction
  • (2)   Stripes across a road at an exponentially
    decreasing distance creates the illusion of
    acceleration when crossing at a constant speed .
    . . Installation of such stripes at high-speed
    approaches to roundabouts in England resulted in
    an average 66 reduction in crashes at such
    sites. (1986, pp. 22-23)

23
Perception and reaction
  • Another approach that is quite effective involves
    raising the legal driving age (Robertson, 1986).

24
SLEEPY DRIVERS
  • As mentioned above, there is a problem in the UK
    with sleep-related vehicle accidents (SRVAs).
    There has been extensive research into this issue
    (Reyner and Horn, 1998) which shows that the
    methods suggested to prevent this by motoring
    organisations, such as opening the window or
    turning up the radio, only have small and
    short-term benefits (about 15 minutes). The best
    advice is to take a break and maybe have a nap.

25
SLEEPY DRIVERS
  • It has been found that naps of between 4 and 20
    minutes can have a positive effect on performance
    and reduce sleepiness. In fact, 15-minute naps
    taken every 6 hours during a period of 35 hours
    of no sleep have been found to be effective in
    maintaining a good level of performance.

26
SLEEPY DRIVERS
  • The common technique of having some coffee is
    also a good one, and laboratory tests have shown
    that low doses of caffeine (100200 mg, or about
    two cups of coffee) improve alertness in sleepy
    people. The answer is fairly clear. To reduce
    road SRVAs we need to encourage drivers to stop
    driving when sleepy, and to take a nap or drink
    some coffee (for a review see Horne and Reyner,
    1999).

27
MOBILE PHONES
  • There is concern about the use of mobile phones
    by drivers. A review of research by RoSPA
    (R0SPA, 2001 b) about the effects of using mobile
    phones on driving found that when the driver is
    using a hand-held or hands-free phone they (a)
    vary their road speed and (b) wander in their
    lane. The driver appears to lose touch with
    driving conditions and become distracted. They
    concluded that using a mobile phone when driving
    increases the risk of having an accident.

28
MOBILE PHONES
  • Interestingly, not all research paints such a
    negative picture of the phone user. For example
    Alm (1998) tested the idea that the more
    demanding the driving task, the greater would be
    the effect on using a mobile phone. The study did
    not support the hypothesis and showed, in fact,
    that drivers under pressure of a demanding road
    will reduce the level of difficulty by, for
    example slowing down, when they are using a
    mobile phone.

29
MOBILE PHONES
  • This suggests that we are able to successfully
    multi-task and adjust our behaviour to match the
    actions we are required to do.

30
MOBILE PHONES
  • The health promotion strategy to reduce accidents
    in drivers who are using mobile phones is carried
    out though driver education, through legislation
    (drivers must be in proper control of their
    vehicles at all times and holding a mobile phone
    whilst driving is now banned in the UK), and
    through employer education (so that they do not
    require their drivers to be available on the
    phone at all times).

31
Protective equipment
  • Injuries and deaths can also he prevented if
    drivers and passengers will use protective
    equipment, such as seat belts in cars and helmets
    when riding motorcycles (Latimer Lave, 1987
    Robertson. 1986 Waller, 1987). But after seat
    belts were installed as standard equipment in
    cars, few people opted to use then. As a result,
    researchers began to try a wide variety of
    methods to promote the use of protective
    equipment in cars.

32
Protective equipment
  • Some of these studies were conducted to improve
    car safety for children by providing instruction
    and information to parents through hospitals and
    paediatricians. These programs have had mixed
    success (Cataldo et al, 1986 Christophersen,
    1984, 1989).

33
Protective equipment
  • Some programs to increase seat belt use have been
    directed at the child, rather than the parent.
    One study presented a 2-week passenger safety
    curriculum to children in several preschools,
    using a theme character called Bucklebear'
    (Chang, Dillman, Leonard, English, 1985). Two
    of the curriculum's main messages were that
    buckling up for every ride is a good thing for
    everyone to do and that the best seat in the car
    is the back seat.

34
Protective equipment
35
Protective equipment
  • Some of the parents also took part in activities
    to promote seat belt, use. The children in
    several other preschools served as a control
    group who were matched to the experimental
    subjects for their prior seat belt use. Follow-up
    observations in the preschool parking lots 3
    weeks after the program was completed revealed
    that over 44 of the "Bucklebear' children and
    only about 22 of the control children were using
    seat belts.

36
ACCIDENT REDUCTION AT WORK
  • Health promotion can be used at work to reduce
    accidents. The most frequently cited methods for
    reducing accidents at work are stress reduction
    programmes. For example, Kunz (1987) describes
    how a stress intervention programme reduced
    medical costs and accident claims in a hospital.
    The programme more than paid for itself with the
    savings from reduction in accidents.

37
ACCIDENT REDUCTION AT WORK
  • Stress reduction programmes have also been shown
    to reduce absenteeism (Murphy and Sorenson,
    1988).
  • Another way of reducing accidents is through
    incentive programmes. Fox et al. (1987) looked at
    the effects of a token economy programme at open
    cast pits.

38
ACCIDENT REDUCTION AT WORK
  • Employees earned stamps for working without time
    lost for injuries, for being in work groups in
    which none of the workers had lost time through
    injury, for not being involved in equipment
    damaging accidents, for making safety
    suggestions, and for behaviour that prevented
    injury or accident. They lost stamps for
    equipment damage, injuries to their work group
    and failure to report accidents and injuries.

39
ACCIDENT REDUCTION AT WORK
  • The token economy produced a dramatic reduction
    in days lost through injury and reduced the costs
    of accidents and injuries. These improvements
    were maintained over a number of years.
  • A relatively simple intervention to reduce
    fatigue and accidents in logging workers involved
    encouraging them to take regular fluids.

40
ACCIDENT REDUCTION AT WORK
  • Sports science has shown that the use of regular
    fluid intake is one way to reduce the sense of
    strain in a task and delay the onset of physical
    and mental fatigue. A study of loggers in New
    Zealand (Paterson et al., 1998) looked at the
    normal performance of the loggers and compared it
    with performance when they were taking a sports
    drink every 15 minutes.

41
ACCIDENT REDUCTION AT WORK
  • In the normal condition, the loggers lost on
    average about 1 per cent of their body weight
    during the working day, but in the fluid
    condition they maintained or increased their body
    weight. Also in the fluid condition, the heart
    rate was lower, and the loggers reported feeling
    fresher, stronger, more alert and more vigorous.
    Reducing fatigue and strain can reduce errors so
    it is a useful intervention to keep a worker
    properly hydrated.

42
MEDIA CAMPAIGNS
  • Public information films on television often tell
    us to do very sensible things like dip our
    headlights or fit smoke alarms. They might well
    affect our attitudes to these procedures and
    products but do they affect our behaviour? In the
    field of accidents it is possible to estimate
    changes in behaviour by comparing accident rates
    before and after an advertising campaign.

43
MEDIA CAMPAIGNS
  • This discrepancy between attitude (what we think)
    and behaviour (what we do) is illustrated in a
    report by Cowpe (1989). This report looked at the
    effectiveness of a series of advertisements about
    the dangers of chip pan fires. Before the
    advertisements, people were asked about this
    hazard and most of them claimed that they always
    adopted safe practices.

44
MEDIA CAMPAIGNS
  • However, the statistics from fire brigades about
    the frequency of chip pan fires and the
    descriptions by people of what they should do
    suggested that their behaviour was not as safe as
    they thought. A television advertising campaign
    was developed and broadcast showing dramatic
    images of exactly how these fires develop, and
    how people should deal with them.

45
MEDIA CAMPAIGNS
  • The adverts ended with a simple statement, such
    as Of course, if you dont overfill your chip
    pan in the first place, you wont have to do any
    of this.

46
MEDIA CAMPAIGNS
  • By comparing fire brigade statistics for the
    areas which received the advertisements, and
    those for the areas which did not, the
    advertisers found that the advertisements had
    produced a 25 per cent reduction in the number of
    chip pan fires in some areas, with a 12 per cent
    reduction overall. Surveys taken after the series
    of advertisements showed that people had more
    accurate knowledge about what they should do in
    the event of a chip pan fire than before.

47
PREVENTING SLIPS, TRIPS AND FALLS
  • Slips, trips and falls make up around a third of
    injuries leading to absence from work (HSE,
    1999). Older people are especially susceptible to
    health-damaging falls, with approximately 30 per
    cent of people over 65 who live in the community
    falling each year and about 50 per cent of the
    over 80s (D0H, 2000).

48
PREVENTING SLIPS, TRIPS AND FALLS
  • The consequences of falling can be
  •  
  • physical injury such as fractures
  • psychological impacts such as increased fear
    of falling
  • reduced mobility
  • needing to be cared for in an institution
  • death.

49
PREVENTING SLIPS, TRIPS AND FALLS
  • There have been many programmes aimed at reducing
    damaging falls in older people. Studies that have
    targeted high-risk groups and offered programmes
    of exercise aimed at increasing mobility and
    strength have been relatively ineffective in
    reducing the number of falls.

50
PREVENTING SLIPS, TRIPS AND FALLS
  • Programmes which have the greatest success
    combine a number of interventions such as a
    review of the medication the older person is
    taking, a safety review of their house and taking
    moderate exercise (for a review see D0H, 2000).
    For people at particular risk, there have been
    some interventions using hip protection so that
    falls are cushioned and less damaging. The
    problem with such interventions is that the
    compliance rate for wearing the devices is
    relatively low.

51
PREVENTING SLIPS, TRIPS AND FALLS
52
UNDERSTANDING MEDICAL INSTRUCTIONS
  • We live in a world full of icons and signs.
    Diagrams of stick people with crosses through
    them appear all over our everyday environment.
    What is the stick figure doing? Does everybody
    understand the same message from these signs?
    Research into signs can help us adjust them so
    that more people can understand what is required
    and make fewer errors in medication.

53
UNDERSTANDING MEDICAL INSTRUCTIONS
  • For example, a study by Dowse and Ehlers (1998)
    on the different perceptions of signs by black
    and white people in South Africa, was able to
    devise signs that could be better recognised by
    the black community. Literacy in the black
    community is still very low in South Africa
    (estimated 45 per cent illiterate and 25 per cent
    semi-literate), so the use of icons and pictures
    is important in medical instructions.

54
UNDERSTANDING MEDICAL INSTRUCTIONS
  • A set of international symbols was published in
    1991 in the United States Pharmacopoeia, but the
    researchers believed that many of these symbols
    would be poorly understood by black South
    Africans.

55
UNDERSTANDING MEDICAL INSTRUCTIONS
  • Following interviews with black students they
    devised some Africanised versions of the symbols.
    When they tested them with members of their
    target group (black South Africans with low
    levels of literacy), they found that the
    Africanised symbols were either equally well or
    better recognised than the US symbols.

56
UNDERSTANDING MEDICAL INSTRUCTIONS
57
Homework
  • a) Outline one study that shows how accidents can
    be reduced. (6)
  • b) Discuss the difficulties in reducing accidents
    and promoting safety behaviours. (10)

58
The End
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com