Title: Does Teacher Feedback Make a Difference in Second Language Learning?
1Does Teacher FeedbackMake a Differencein Second
Language Learning?
- Dr. Roy Lyster
- McGill University
- Montreal, Canada
2Teaching in the 80s
- Krashen said that feedback was useless, and
harmful, and would cause anxiety (more recently,
see Krashen, 1994 Truscott, 1996). - (I did not want anxious students, so I provided
very little feedback.) - Researchers said that errors would diminish over
time. - (I wanted to be patient, so I provided very
little feedback.) - My students had studied their L2 for 8 years
- (How long would it take for errors to work
themselves out?)
3Serge and the Negotiation of Form
- I observed a teacher named Serge who provided
feedback without causing any any observable
anxiety (Lyster, 1994). - There was good-natured humour and the
communication flow did NOT stop. - Serge negotiated form with his students
- Serge How is formal correspondence different
in English and French? - Student 1 The thing at the bottom
- Serge The thing at the bottom?!
- Student 1 The final salutation
4Feedback Terminology
- Error correction
- Negative feedback
- Corrective feedback
- Interactional feedback
- Negotiation of meaning
- Negotiation of form
5Rationale for FeedbackTransfer-appropriate
Learning
- The context in which learning occurs should
resemble the context in which the learning will
be put to use (Segalowitz, 2000) - Language features learned in isolated grammar
lessons may be remembered in similar contexts
(e.g., during a grammar test), but hard to
retrieve in the context of communicative
interaction. - Language features noticed during communicative
interaction may be more easily retrieved in
communicative contexts.
6Teachable Moments Focus on Form
- Providing feedback in the heat of the moment
when a learner really has something to say,
rather than waiting till later - How can teachers do this? How can they focus on
form during meaningful interaction? - By providing different types of interactional
feedback recasts or prompts.
7Recasts
- In a recast, the teacher implicitly reformulates
the students utterance, minus the error. - Example 1
- Student Before someone will takes it.
- Teacher Before someone takes it.
- Example 2
- Student Or an boat.
- Teacher Yes, thats true that it could be a
boat, but there theyre giving addresses.
8Frequency of Recasts
- Recasts are the most frequent type of feedback in
a wide range of classroom settings - elementary immersion classrooms
- (Lyster Ranta, 1997 Mori, 2002)
- university-level foreign language classrooms
- (Doughty, 1994 Roberts, 1995)
- high school EFL classrooms
- (Tsang, 2004)
- adult ESL classrooms
- (Ellis, Basurkmen, Loewen, 2001 Panova
Lyster, 2002)
9Theoretical Value of Recasts
- Based on claims that children frequently repeat
their parents recasts during L1 acquisition,
recasts have been promoted as an effective type
of feedback - Some researchers hypothesize that recasts help
learners to notice the gap between interlanguage
forms and target forms, thus serving as negative
evidence - Doughty (2001)
- Long (1996)
- Long Robinson (1998)
10Practical advantages
- Recasts provide supportive scaffolding that helps
learners participate in lessons when the target
forms in question are beyond their current
abilities. - Recasts are ideal for facilitating the delivery
of complex subject matter (Lyster, 2002).
11Disadvantages of Recasts
- Recasts do not lead to any self- or peer-repair
when there is repair, the student can only repeat
the teachers reformulation - In L2 classrooms, many recasts can be ambiguous
and therefore do not help learners to notice
their mistakes (Lyster, 1998).
12Ambiguity of Recasts
- Recasts Compete with Non-Corrective Repetition
- Recast
- T6 What smells so good? Allen?
- St Sap maple.
- T6 Maple sap. Thats good.
- Non-corrective repetition
- T6 What do we call the baby of a hen? Nicole?
- St Chicks.
- T6 Chicks. Thats good.
13Ambiguity of Recasts
- Recasts Compete with Signs of Approval
- Example 1
- T5 What are orders?..Yes?
- St Its, just like uhh you say us, do this,
do that - T5 Exactly, its when someone tells us Do that,
go there, eat that. - Example 2
- T6 A hole in which a rabbit lives, Patrick?
- St A din.
- T6 A den, thats good.
14Perlette and the Water Cycle
- T5 Whats a stream again? Yes?
- StA Its like a small lake.
- T5 A small lake we said?
- StA Its an little river.
- T5 Thats it. Its a little river, O.K.?
Because a lake is a, a place where theres water
but its a ... - Sts Like a circle.
- T5 And so she finds herself near a forest. What
do they do in the forest? Will? - StB They cut down trees.
- T5 They cut down trees.
15Perlette and the Water Cycle
- T5 What do they do to transport the wood?
- StC Um, you put the wood in the water and the
um, how do you say emporter? - Sts Carries.
- T5 Carries, good.
- StC Carries tree to an place and another
person who puts the wood. - T5 Thats it. So, they put the wood in the
river so it gets transported from one place
to another.
16Perlette and the Water Cycle
- T5 And when hes talking to Perlette, what
happens to the fish? - St Hes going to drink her.
- T5 Hes going to drink Perlette? No, hes not
going to drink Perlette. - St Uhm, the fish is friend of her.
- T5 Yes, thats it, theyre friends and they talk
together. Then suddenly what happens? Yes? - StD A person fishing took.
- T5 Exactly. Right, theres a hook with a little
worm on it and so the fish turns around....
17Perlette and the Water Cycle
- T5 Why does she want to warm up do you think?
Yes? - StA Because she has too cold to go into all
the ? - T5 Because she is too cold, O.K. Yes?
- StB She has too frightened.
- T5 Because she is frightened, yes.
18Experimental Studies of Recasts
- Some experimental studies have shown that recasts
are more effective than no feedback - in laboratory settings
- Long et al. (1998)
- Mackey Philp (1998)
- in a classroom setting
- Doughty Varela (1998) showed that corrective
recasting was more effective than no feedback - St I think that the worm will go under the soil.
- T I think that the worm will go under the soil?
I thought that the worm would go under the
soil.
19Prompts Negotiation of form
- Clarification request
- The teacher pretends that the message has not
been understood and that a repetition or a
reformulation is required - Pardon me?
- I dont understand
- Repetition
- The teacher repeats the students erroneous
utterance, adjusting the intonation to highlight
the error - He goed?
20Prompts
- Metalinguistic clues
- The teacher provides comments or questions
related to the accuracy of the students
utterance, without explicitly providing the
correct form - Do we say goed in English?
- No, thats not it.
- Elicitation
- The teacher directly elicits correct forms from
students by asking questions such as - How do we say that in English?
- He what?
21Prompts
- Self-repair
- Prompts lead to student-generated repair because,
unlike recasts, they withhold correct forms and
providing clues instead, pushing students to
retrieve correct forms on their own (i.e., peer-
or self-repair). - Frequency
- Prompts accounted for 38 of all feedback in
French immersion classrooms (Lyster Ranta,
1997) and 26 in Japanese immersion classrooms
(Mori, 2002).
22Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
- T3 The porcupine? Sara?
- St Its the pines on its back, its ...
- T3 The pines. Do we say pines?
- StD The upines.
- T3 The...?
- StD The quills.
- T3 The quills. Very good. The quills.
23Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
- T3 And so the skunk, what does it do? Karen
- St Uhm...it does...Well theres a stream of
perfume that doesnt smell very good... - T3 A stream of perfume, well call that a ...?
- Sts Liquid.
- T3 Liquid. A liquid . . .?
- StD Smelly.
- T3 A smelly liquid. We also call that ..
24Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
- T3 The hare. Joseph could you tell us what its
means of defense are? - St It runs fast and it hops.
- T3 It runs fast.
- StD It jump.
- T3 It jump?
- Sts It jumps.
- T3 It jumps, from the verb. . . ?
- Sts To jump.
- T3 To jump. It jumps about. Right, it jumps.
Next, Joseph?
25Porcupines, Skunks, Hares, Giraffes
- T3 Bigger than you would be what?
- St The giraffe? masc.
- T3 The giraffe? masc.
- St The giraffe. fem.
- T3 The giraffe.fem. But is the giraffe an
animal from Canada?
26Effectiveness of Prompts
- Prompts can improve control over
already-internalized forms by providing
opportunities for - pushed output, hypothesized by Swain (1985,
1988) to move interlanguage development forward, - practice that helps learners in the transition of
declarative to procedural knowledge (de Bot,
1996 Lyster Ranta, 1997).
27Effectiveness of Prompts
- L2 learners benefit more from being pushed to
retrieve target language forms than from merely
hearing the forms in the input - because the retrieval and subsequent production
require a deep level of processing that
stimulates connections in memory (de Bot, 1996). - Studies comparing recasts with prompts in
classroom settings have shown that prompts are
more effective than recasts - Havranek Cesnik (2001)
- Ammar (2003)
- Lyster (2004)
28Ammar (2003)
- Third-person possessive determiners in English
(his and her) were targeted in three 6th-grade
intensive ESL classrooms over a four-week period. - One class received recasts, another received
prompts, and the third received no feedback. - The group receiving prompts significantly
outperformed the recast group on written and oral
post-tests - Prompts were particularly effective for
lower-proficiency learners, whereas
higher-proficiency learners benefited similarly
from both recasts and prompts.
29Lyster (2004)
- Grammatical gender in French was targeted by
three 5th-grade immersion teachers in different
ways that permitted comparisons of three oral
feedback options prompts, recasts, and no
feedback. - A comparison group received no form-focused
instruction nor any pre-planned feedback on
grammatical gender. - The analysis of eight proficiency measures
administered over time showed that the group
receiving prompts distinguished itself by being
the only group to significantly outperform the
comparison group on all eight measures.
30When to use recasts
- Depending on the interactional context, learners
are likely to notice the corrective quality of
many recasts, especially when - the recasts have been shortened and/or provided
with added stress to highlight the error - the target forms are beyond the students current
abilities.
31When to use prompts
- Learners benefit from being pushed to produce
modified output by means of prompting, especially
when - recasts might be perceived ambiguously as
approving students use of non-target forms - students have reached a developmental plateau in
their use of the non-target forms (i.e.,
fossilized forms) and need to automatize target
forms.
32Conclusion
- Continued recasting of what students already know
is likely ineffective for ensuring continued
development of L2 accuracy. - Continued prompting of learners to draw on what
they have not yet acquired will be equally
ineffective. - Effective L2 teachers need to orchestrate the use
of both recasts and prompts, without abandoning
one at the expense of the other.
33References
- Ammar, A. (2003). Corrective feedback and L2
learning Elicitation and recasts. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, McGill University,
Montreal. - Braidi, S. (2002). Reexamining the role of
recasts in native-speaker/nonnative-speaker
interactions. Language Learning, 52, 1-42. - de Bot, K. (1996). The psycholinguistics of the
output hypothesis. Language Learning, 46,
529-555. - Doughty, C. (2001). Cognitive underpinnings of
focus on form. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition
and second language instruction (pp. 206-257).
New York Cambridge University Press. - Doughty, C., Varela, E. (1998). Communicative
focus on form. In C. Doughty J. Williams
(Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second
language acquisition (pp. 129-154). New York
Cambridge University Press. - Havranek, G., Cesnik, H. (2001). Factors
affecting the success of corrective feedback.
EUROSLA Yearbook, 1, 99-122. - Krashen, S. (1994). The input hypothesis and its
rivals. In N. Ellis (ed), Implicit and Explicit
learning of Languages. London Academic Press,
pp. 45-77. - Leeman, J. (2003). Recasts and second language
development Beyond negative evidence. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 25, 37-63. - Long, M. (1996). The role of the linguistic
environment in second language acquisition. In W.
C. Ritchie T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of
language acquisition Vol. 2. Second language
acquisition (pp. 413-468). San Diego, CA
Academic Press. - Long, M., Inagaki, S., Ortega, L. (1998). The
role of implicit negative evidence in SLA Models
and recasts in Japanese and Spanish. Modern
Language Journal, 82, 357-371.
34References
- Long, M., Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on Form
Theory, research, and practice. In C. Doughty
J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom
second language acquisition (pp.15-41). New York
Cambridge University Press. - Lyster, R. (1994b). La négociation de la forme
stratégie analytique en classe dimmersion. The
Canadian Modern Language Review, 50, 446-465 - Lyster, R. (1998). Recasts, repetition, and
ambiguity in L2 classroom discourse. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 20, 55-85. - Lyster, R. (2002). Negotiation in immersion
teacher-student interaction. International
Journal of Educational Research, 37, 237-253 - Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of
prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26
399-432. - Lyster, R., Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective
feedback and learner uptake Negotiation of form
in communicative classrooms. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 19, 37-66. - Mackey, A., Philp, J. (1998). Conversational
interaction and second language development
Recasts, responses, and red herrings? Modern
Language Journal, 82, 338-356. - Oliver, R., Mackey, A. (2003). Interactional
context and feedback in child ESL classrooms. The
Modern Language Journal, 87, 519-533 - Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence Some
roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible
output in its development. In S. Gass C. Madden
(Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp.
235-253). Rowley, MA Newbury House. - Swain, M. (1988). Manipulating and complementing
content teaching to maximize second language
learning. TESL Canada Journal, 6, 68-83.