GUIDELINES FOR VALID CAUSAL RELATONS: EXTERNAL VALIDITY - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 29
About This Presentation
Title:

GUIDELINES FOR VALID CAUSAL RELATONS: EXTERNAL VALIDITY

Description:

How disentangle possible multiple meanings of practice-defined ... Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research (Rand McNally). Kerlinger, F. 1985. ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:104
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 30
Provided by: shie151
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: GUIDELINES FOR VALID CAUSAL RELATONS: EXTERNAL VALIDITY


1
GUIDELINES FOR VALID CAUSAL RELATONS EXTERNAL
VALIDITY
  • Mike Shields
  • Michigan State University

2
OUTLINE
  • Concepts and variables
  • Linearity
  • Causal-model form
  • Directionality
  • Level of analysis
  • MA as IV or DV
  • Cross-level models
  • Luft and Shields (AOS 2003)

3
1. CONCEPTS AND VARIABLES
  • 2 types of partially shared meanings
  • Some variables are from practice (e.g., ABC, BSC)
    and some are social-science theory constructs
  • Formalization, informativeness, role ambiguity
  • How disentangle possible multiple meanings of
    practice-defined variables like ABC and BSC?
  • Do practice-defined and social-science constructs
    have the same meaning?
  • How, if at all, are ABC and BSC related to
    aggregation, informativeness, timeliness,
    uncertainty, visibility of power relations?

4
GUIDELINES FOR DEFINING VARIABLES
  • If a practice-defined variable is used, then
    clearly define its underlying theoretical
    properties not only those that are of
    particular interest in the current study, but
    also other properties that the practice-defined
    variable is likely to possess.
  • If a practice-defined variable can represent
    multiple theoretical properties, then gather
    evidence that identifies their separate causes
    and effects.
  • If the theoretical property of interest belongs
    to only a definable subset of instances of the
    practice-defined variable (e.g., only some ABC
    systems), then state this limitation explicitly.

5
CONTINUED
  • Different theories define their variables with
    differing degrees of broadness
  • Research question and theory determine
    appropriate breath of definition How specific
    are concepts and theory?
  • Uncertainty? Task and environmental uncertainty?
    Competitor, regulatory or consumer uncertainty?
  • Too broad includes noise in cause-effect relation
  • More likely effects other than predicted effects
    will be detected and wrongly interpreted
  • Too narrow captures only part of cause-effect
    relation
  • Less likely the expected effect will be detected
  • Guideline
  • 4. A variable definition should not include
    content irrelevant to the research question and
    theory employed or exclude relevant content.

6
2. LINEARITY
  • Theories predict curvilinear relations but few
    studies predict them
  • Linearize by range of evidence (typical, not
    extreme cases) and transforming data
  • Need extremes to test boundary conditions
  • Practice wants to know not just that more/less MA
    is better, but how much is optimal
  • Guidelines
  • 5. If theory predicts nonlinearities in the
    relation examined, then consider the value of
    capturing nonlinearities in the study.
  • 6. If a linear model is used for the sake of
    simplicity, then be explicit about the resulting
    limitations.

7
3. CAUSAL-MODEL FORM
  • Consider the participative budgeting
    satisfaction relation
  • Studies predict and find that the relation is
    additive, intervening-variable,
    independent-variable interaction, and
    moderator-variable interaction
  • Because some of these causal-model forms are in
    conflict, not all of them can be valid
    representations of this relation
  • Some of these causal-model forms are in conflict
    and some are compatible

8
ADDITIVE, INTERACTION AND INTERVENING MODELS
  • Does it matter which model is used?
  • Are these models in conflict or are they
    compatible?

9
ADDITIVE VS INTERVENING VARIABLE
  • Additive model that predicts X1 ? Y is not in
    conflict with an intervening variable model that
    predicts X1 ? X2 ? Y
  • Intervening variable model provides supplementary
    information about the X1-Y relation can can help
    explain week X1-Y results
  • Additive model that predicts X1, X2 ? Y is in
    conflict with an intervening variable model that
    predicts X1 ? X2 ? Y
  • If the intervening variable model is valid, then
    using an additive model can show that neither X
    affects Y because of multicollinearity between X1
    and X2
  • If the additive model is valid, then using an
    intervening variable model with no direct X1-Y
    path can show no effect of X1 on Y because there
    is not effect of X1 on X2 even though the X1-Y
    relation is strong

10
ADDITIVE VS INTERACTON
  • If a relation is interactive the effect of X1
    on Y is conditional on X2 and vice versa but an
    additive model is used (X1, X2 ? Y)
  • If X2 is held constant, then the conclusion about
    X1 ? Y is valid only at the level of X2
  • If X2 is omitted or included as an additive
    variable, then the detected effect of X1 on Y is
    a weighted average of the different X1 effects
    that occur at different levels of X2
  • If a relation is an ordinal interaction, then
    using an additive model affects the estimated
    magnitude of the effect of X on Y
  • If a relation is a disordinal interaction, then
    using an additive model affects the estimated
    magnitude and possibly the sign of the X-Y
    relation

11
INTERVENING VARIABLE VS INTERACTION
  • Consider a setting in which X1 ? X2 and X1 by X2
    ? Y can be represented by 2 models
  • Intervening variable X1 ? X2 ? Y
  • Interaction X1-Y depends on X2 and X2-Y depends
    on X1
  • Testing both models is problematic
  • If the X1-X2 relation is strong, then there will
    be insufficient variation in the sample to
    provide a powerful test of the interaction

12
INTERACTING INDEPENDENT- AND MODERATOR-VARIABLE
MODELS
  • An IV has a causal influence on the DV
  • A moderator variable influences the IV-DV
    relation, but is not associated with either the
    IV or DV
  • Important practice and theory implications
    whether an interaction involves IVs only or IVs
    and MVs

13
GUIDELINES
  • If the causal model proposed is additive, then
    indicate both the reasons for assuming there are
    no important intervening-variable or interaction
    relations and the consequences of omitting these
    relations if they exist.
  • If the causal model proposed is conditional, then
    indicate the type of conditionality (intervening
    versus interacting).
  • For interaction models, indicate whether the
    interaction is ordinal or disordinal.
  • For interaction models, indicate whether the
    interaction involves independent variables only
    or independent and moderator variables.

14
4. DIRECTIONALITY
  • Does budget goal difficulty influence performance
    or vice versa?
  • Cross-sectional studies (Map A)
  • budget goal difficulty ? performance
  • A study with 2 points in time (Map B)
  • past performance ? current budget goal difficulty
  • A study of ratchet systems with 3 time points
    (Map F)
  • performance ? budget goal difficulty ?
    performance
  • Answers to these questions depend on answers to 2
    questions about time length
  • What is the time frame of the study over how
    long a time period and at what intervals within
    that period should evidence be collected?
  • What is the causal interval of the relation how
    long does it take for a change in the IV to cause
    a change in the DV?

15
CONTINUED
  • For bidirectional relations, common for research
    to assume unidirectionality
  • When do so, usually treat slower changing
    variable as exogenous (IV) because its response
    to changes in the other variables is too slow to
    be captured within the studys time frame
  • Consider MA and national culture
  • NC ? MA has a shorted causal interval than MA ?
    NC
  • Therefore, most studies treat NC as the IV and MA
    as the DV
  • If changes in NC have had time to cause changes
    in MA but changes in MA have not had time to
    cause changes in NC, then a unidirectional model
    of NC ? MA is valid

16
TYPES OF BIDIRECTIONALITY
  • Bidirectional model is required if the interest
    is on the slower effect (variable with the longer
    causal interval) or if both effects have similar
    causal intervals
  • Cyclical model is valid if the causal interval of
    the relation and time frame are matched such that
    causality goes in one direction for one time
    period and the other direction for the next time
    period
  • Reciprocal model is valid if the causal
    influences are simultaneous or causal intervals
    are shorter than the intervals at which evidence
    is collected so that influences in both
    directions are captured

17
GUIDELINES FOR DIRECTIONALITY
  • If unidirectional causality is assumed, then
    indicate the reasons for excluding
    bidirectionality.
  • Align the time frame of the study (length and
    frequency of evidence collection) and the causal
    interval (the time required for the cause
    examined in the study to have an effect).

18
5. LEVEL OF ANALYSIS
  • What is the level of analysis?
  • Is participative budgeting at the individual,
    subunit, or organization level of analysis? Map
    A vs. B?
  • For single-level studies, need to align
  • Level of theory What is being explained?
  • Level of variable measurement What is the source
    of the evidence?
  • Level of data analysis What is treated as an
    independent datum for analysis purposes?

19
MULTIPLE LEVELS OF ANALYSIS
  • Effects on performance from different levels can
    be additive
  • Industry market structure ? industry component of
    managers performance
  • Organizational strategy ? organizational
    component of managers performance
  • Subunit budget ? subunit component of managers
    performance
  • Individual skill ? individual component of
    managers performance
  • Additive effects is not cross-level model because
    a variable defined at one level cannot
    systematically affect a variable defined at
    another level but can add noise to measurements
    of variables at other levels
  • Use hierarchical or nested models to partial out
    the additive effects at different levels either
    to remove noise if some levels are not of
    interest or to identify the multiple-level
    effects separately

20
CROSS-LEVEL MODELS
  • Must be interactive causal models with an
    interacting IV or MV at the level of the DV
  • Organizational MA can affect individual
    performance only if individuals are different on
    some characteristic (knowledge, preferences) that
    causes them to respond differently to
    organization MA
  • Cross-level models can be top-down (as most are
    due to exogenizing the slower changing variable,
    which tends to be a variable at a higher level)
    and/or bottom-up

21
GUIDELINES FOR MULTI-LEVEL MODELS
  • Indicate whether the variable of interest varies
    across individuals, organizational subunits,
    organizations, or beyond-organization entities
    like markets and societies.
  • Align the level of theory (what is being
    explained), level of variable measurement (source
    of evidence), and level of data analysis (unit of
    data).
  • If the theoretical variables at multiple levels
    affect the observed measures, then separate the
    effects from multiple levels.
  • If cross-level effects are proposed, then use an
    interaction causal-model form, with at least one
    interacting independent or moderator variable at
    the level of the dependent variable.
  • If the variation of interest in a variable is
    variation in its value relative to a subset of
    other values in the sample, then use an
    individual-within-group-level model.

22
6. MA AS IV OR DVLINKING CAUSES AND EFFECTS
  • Some studies explain only causes of MA and others
    explain only effects of MA
  • Questions arise about whether the explanations of
    cause are consistent with explanations of effect
  • If the assumptions about performance effects used
    to explain causes are correct, then causal-model
    forms should be consistent across studies of MAs
    causes and effects

23
STUDIES OF CAUSES AND EFFECTS ARE SOMETIMES IN
CONFLICT
  • A study shows that competition has an additive
    effect on MA
  • It assumes MA is used to increase performance as
    any type of competition increases
  • conflicts with
  • Another study in which an interaction model
    predicts that the performance effects of MA are
    conditional on the type of competition (price or
    quantity)
  • 3 possible explanations for such conflicts
  • Across-study differences in the meaning of
    similar variables
  • Across-study differences in the level of
    analysis Causes and effects vary with level
  • Assumptions about the performance effects of MA
    that explain its causes are not correct (e.g., MA
    is performance beneficial for only some kinds of
    competition)
  • This explanation is controversial because
    economics-based research excludes it because
    economics assumes organizations will only use
    practices that are optimal, thus there is no
    performance effect of MA

24
CAUSES, EFFECTS AND EQUILIBRIUM
  • Economics assumes equilibrium
  • Explanations of causes of MA are explanations of
    why MA is an equilibrium solution to the economic
    problem of performance maximization
  • If MA is an equilibrium solution, then it is
    possible to provide evidence for explanations of
    its causes but not its performance effects
  • If all organizations use the MA that is optimal
    for them, then they would all have the same
    performance, controlling for the effects of other
    variables, thus there is no variance in
    performance due to MA

25
CONTINUED
  • Psychology and sociology do not assume
    equilibrium
  • Assume that causes and effects of MA can differ
  • Assumes that MA can be used for reasons other
    than performance maximization, or if performance
    maximization is the goal then variables like
    judgments and decisions can cause errors that do
    not result in MA maximizing performance
  • Contingency theory assumes that organizations use
    the MA that is best for them, but that the
    process of adapting to a new equilibrium can take
    a long time
  • During the adjustment process, it is possible to
    test for performance effects as different
    organizations are at different stages of
    converging on a new equilibrium

26
CONTINUED
  • Critical issue in determining whether MA (and
    performance) is an IV or DV is whether an
    equilibrium condition exists or there is
    convergence on an equilibrium
  • Research indicates that the dynamics of many
    complex systems result in non-equilibrium
    behavior
  • cyclical or chaotic behavior
  • Equilibrium behavior is less likely as the length
    of the causal interval increases, time lags
    increase, and relations are curvilinear and
    indirect
  • Causal intervals are important in choosing the
    valid causal model form and whether MA should be
    an IV or DV

27
7. CROSS-LEVEL MODELS
  • Individuals decisions are affected by
    organizational MA
  • Design of organizational MA requires individuals
    to make decisions about the design

28
MA AS IV AND DVCONSTRUCTING ONE MAP OF MA
PRACTICE
  • Unlikely that a study will include this model,
    but studies should be conducted that allow their
    theory-consistent evidence to construct maps that
    contain this model of MA as IV and DV
  • How do we combine 275 studies into 1 map?
  • Consider our 3 questions and 17 guidelines
  • What variables are researched?
  • What is the shape and direction of explanatory
    links?
  • What is the level of analysis?

29
BIBLIOGRAPHY
  • GENERAL
  • Campbell, D. and J. Stanley. 1963. Experimental
    and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research (Rand
    McNally).
  • Kerlinger, F. 1985. Foundations of Behavioral
    Research (Harcourt Brace).
  • Nunnally, J. and I. Bernstein. 1994. Psychometric
    Theory (McGraw Hill).
  • Rosenthal, R. and R. Rosnow.1969. Artifact in
    Behavioral Research (Academic Press).
  • Runkel, P. and J. McGrath. 1972. Research on
    Human Behavior A Systematic Guide to Method
    (Holt, Rhinehart and Winston).
  • ACCOUNTING
  • Birnberg, J., M. Shields, and M. Young. 1990.
    "The Case for Multiple Methods in Empirical
    Management Accounting Research," Journal of
    Management Accounting Research, pp. 33-66.
  • Libby, R. R. Bloomfield, and M. Nelson. 2002,
    "Experimental Research in Financial Accounting,"
    Accounting, Organizations and Society, pp.
    775-810.
  • Luft, J. and M. Shields. 2003. "Mapping
    Management Accounting Graphics and Guidelines
    for Theory-Consistent Empirical Research,"
    Accounting, Organizations and Society, pp.
    169-249.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com