Title: Colour Language 2: Explaining Typology
1Colour Language 2Explaining Typology
Mike Dowman Language and Cognition 5 October, 2005
2Todays Lecture
- Kay and McDaniel Direct neurophysiological
explanation - Terry Regier et al Predicting denotations from
foci - Yendrikhovskij Colours in the environment
- Evolutionary and Acquisitional Explanations
- Me An evolutionary model
3Kay and McDaniel (1978)
- Red, yellow, green and blue colour categories
could be derived directly from the outputs of
opponent process cells
hue
hue
4Opponent Processes
Union of blue and green blue-green
Intersection of red and yellow orange
- Composite categories can be derived using fuzzy
unions - Purple, pink, brown and grey can be derived as
fuzzy using fuzzy intersections
5Problems
- Colour term denotations vary across languages.
- Denotations and foci arent in the same places as
opponent process cells predict. - Doesnt explain why some types of colour term are
unattested (e.g. blue-red composites,
yellow-green derived terms (lime)).
6Regier et al (2005)
- Is knowing the location of the prototypes in the
colour space enough to predict the full
denotations of colour words? - Investigated using a computer model.
- Used CIELab colour space which attempts to
accurately capture conceptual distances between
colours
7Details of Computer Model
- Colour categories are represented as points in
the colour space each at a unique hue - Plus a parameter that controls for category size
- Size parameter was fit to naming data to get best
result - Each colour is classified based on the distance
to each focus, and the size of the categories
based on each focus
8Results Berinmo
Berinmo naming data
Model predictions fit to data
Categories centred at red, yellow, green, black
and white universal foci
- Explains naming in terms of foci
- But doesnt explain which foci each language uses
- Doesnt show that non-attested colour term
systems cant be represented
9Yendrikhovskij (2001)
- Can the colours in the environment explain
typological patterns in colour naming?
N.B. Photo from Tony Belpaeme, not Yendrikhovskij
10Distribution of Colours
Full range of colours
Those in natural images
- Colours in natural images mapped to CIE colour
space - Then clustered (those closest to each other were
grouped together) - Number of clusters was varied
11Yendrikhovskijs Results
- 11 Clusters
- 10 are close to centres of English colour terms
- A yellow-green cluster replaces purple
- 7 Clusters
- black, white, red, green, yellow, blue, brown
- 3 Clusters
- ? black, white, red
- Distribution of colours in the environment
together with the properties of the sensorial
system predict attested colour term systems
quite well
12Acquisitional and Evolutionary Explanations
13Learnable and Evolvable Languages
E
L
F
Occurring languages
All of the languages which actually exist in the
world will fall within the intersection of the
learnable languages, (L), and those languages
which are preferred as a result of evolutionary
pressures, (F) (Kirby, 1999).
14Expression-Induction Models
- Models simulate the transmission of language
between agents (artificial people) - Each agent can learn a language based on
utterances spoken by another agent - In turn they can speak and so create data from
which another agent can learn
L0
L1
L2
15Evolving Colour Categories Dowman (2003, 2004)
- Can we explain colour term typology in terms of
cultural evolution? - This was the original thesis of Berlin Kay
(1969). - Small biases in the way we learn or perceive
colour categories could create evolutionary
pressures that, over several generations, result
in only a limited range of languages emerging. - Tony Belpaeme (2002) and Me both have
expression-induction models of colour term
evolution
16Hypothesis
- Typological patterns observed in colour term
naming are due to irregularities in the
conceptual colour space. - In particular the irregular spacing of the
unique hues - and their added salience
17Agents Conceptual Colour Space
The whole colour space is 40 units in size
18Learning by Bayesian Inference
- Statistical inference allows the most likely
denotation for colour terms to be estimated based
on some example colours - Has no predisposition to believe any type of
colour term is more likely than any other - Can cope with errors in the data
- Each colour word is learned individually
19Learning Colour Word Denotations from Examples
low probability hypothesis
high probability hypothesis
medium probability hypothesis
20Urdu
21Agent Communication
Agent 3
Agent 8
Agent 3 thinks Mehi is the best label for colour
27
Mehi 27 remembered by agent 8
Says Mehi
Nol 11, 14 Wor 3, 12 Mehi 33
Both agents can see colour 27
22A speaker is chosen.
Evolutionary Model
A hearer is chosen.
A colour is chosen.
Yes (P0.001)
The Speaker makes up a new word to label the
colour.
Decide whether speaker will be creative.
No (P0.999)
The speaker says the word which they think is
most likely to be a correct label for the colour
based on all the examples that they have observed
so far.
The hearer hears the word, and remembers the
corresponding colour. This example will be used
to determine the word to choose, when it is the
hearers turn to be the speaker.
23Evolutionary Simulations
- Average lifespan (number of colour examples
remembered) set at - 18, 20, 22, 24, 25, 27, 30, 35, 40, 50, 60, 70,
80, 90, 100, 110 or 120 - 25 simulation runs in each condition
- Languages spoken at end analysed
- Only agents over half average lifespan included
- Only terms for which at least 4 examples had been
remembered were considered
24Analyzing the Results
- Speakers didnt have identical languages
- Criteria needed to classify language spoken in
each simulation - For each agent, terms classified as red, yellow,
green, blue, purple, orange, lime, turquoise or a
composite (e.g. blue-green) - Terms must be known by most adults
- Classification favoured by the most agents chosen
25Example One Emergent Language
Denotations of Basic Color Terms for all Adults
in a Community
Each row is one agent Each column is a hue Boxes
mark unique hues
26Typological Results
Percentage of Color Terms of each type in the
Simulations and the World Color Survey
27Derived Terms
- 80 purple terms
- 20 orange terms
- 0 turquoise terms
- 4 lime terms
28Divergence from Trajectories
- 1 Blue-Red term
- 1 Red-Yellow-Green term
- 3 Green-Blue-Red terms
- Most emergent systems fitted trajectories
- 340 languages fitted trajectories
- 9 contained unattested color terms
- 35 had no consistent name for a unique hue
- 37 had an extra term
29Does Increased Salience of Unique Hues Matter?
30Unique Hues Create More Regular Colour Term
Systems
- 644 purple terms
- 374 orange terms
- 118 lime terms
- 16 turquoise terms
- Only 87 of 415 emergent systems fits trajectories
31How Reliable is WCS Data?
- Would a model that more closely replicated the
WCS data be a better model? - Field linguists tend to suggest that colours are
much more messy than Kay et al suggest - WCS is only a sample not a gold standard
- Is data massaged to fit theories?
32Summary
- Typological patterns in colour term systems
cross-linguistically can be explained in terms of
uneven conceptual spacing of the unique hues. - The typological patterns are emergent properties
of the cultural evolution of colour term systems
over time. - The evolutionary approach readily accommodates
exceptional languages. - Environmental and/or cultural pressures probably
also influence emergent colour term systems.
33References
- Belpaeme, Tony (2002). Factors influencing the
origins of color categories. PhD Thesis,
Artificial Intelligence Lab, Vrije Universiteit
Brussel. - Berlin, B. Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms.
Berkeley University of California Press. - Dowman, M. (2003). Explaining Color Term Typology
as the Product of Cultural Evolution using a
Bayesian Multi-agent Model. In R. Alterman and D.
Kirsh (Eds.) Proceedings of the 25th Annual
Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah,
N.J. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. - Dowman, M. (2004). Colour Terms, Syntax and
Bayes Modelling Acquisition and Evolution. Ph.D.
Thesis, University of Sydney. - Hurford, J. R. (1987). Language and Number The
Emergence of a Cognitive System. New York, NY
Basil Blackwell. - Kirby, S. (1999). Function Selection and
Innateness The Emergence of Language Universals.
Oxford Oxford University Press.
34- Kay, P. McDaniel, K. (1978). The Linguistic
Significance of the Meanings of Basic Color
Terms. Language, 54 (3) 610-646. - Regier, T. Kay, P. and Cook, R. S. (2005).
Universal Foci and Varying Boundaries in
Linguistic Color Categories. In B. G. Bara, L.
Barsalou and M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings
of the XXVII Annual Conference of the Cognitive
Science Society. Mahwah, New Jersey Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. - Yendrikhovskij, S. N. (2001). Computing Color
Categories from Statistics of Natural Images,
Journal of Imaging Science and Technology, 45(5).
35Discussion Questions for Tomorrow
- Is colour term typology best explained in terms
of neurophysiology, the environment, cultural
practices, or some other factor? - What evidence is there for innate biases
concerning colour terms? - Is colour term evolution really as predictable as
Berlin and Kays implicational hierarchy
suggests? - Is it really possible to separate basic from
non-basic colour terms objectively? (Think about
English and any other languages you know.) - Is colour term typology best explained
ontogenetically or diachronically?