Title: Tamara Nicol Medina
1Using Extra-Linguistic Cues to Identify Good Word
Learning Instances
- Tamara Nicol Medina
- John Trueswell
- Lila Gleitman
- University Of Pennsylvania
- Jesse Snedeker
- Harvard University
- Society for Research in Child Development
- April 2, 2009, Denver, CO
2(No Transcript)
3Just look at the world!
- Observe physical and temporal contingencies
between words and objects. - (At least, for physically observable objects.)
- Experimental evidence supports ease of mapping
- Fast mapping (e.g., Carey, 1978 Mervis
Bertrand, 1994 Behrend et al., 2001 Jaswal
Markman, 2001) - Cross-situational word learning (e.g., Yu
Smith, 2007 Smith Yu, 2008 Vouloumanos, 2008
Xu Tenenbaum, 2007)
4(No Transcript)
5Its Not that Easy! (Augustine, Locke, Quine,
Gleitman, Fodor, Siskind, etc.)
- Reference problem
- Book? Cat? Shoes? Chair? Cheerios? Cup?
Rug? Pants? Head? Hand? - Frame problem
- Dog or Puppy? Hand or Finger? Red or Ball?
- Naturalistic learning conditions
- Medina, Trueswell, Snedeker, Gleitman. (2008).
When the shoe fits Cross-situational word
learning in realistic learning environments.
BUCLD.
6How do learners narrow down the possibilities?
- Linguistic context (Landau Gleitman, 1985
Gleitman, 1990 Gillette, Gleitman, Gleitman,
Lederer, 1999) - Learning biases
- Whole object constraint (Markman, 1989)
- Mutual exclusivity (Markman Wachtel, 1988
Markman, Wasow, Hansen, 2003) - Social-attentional cues (e.g., Baldwin 1991,
1993 Tomasello Akhtar, 1995 Bloom, 2002
Behne, Carpenter, Tomasello, 2005)
7Social-Attentional Cues
- Nonverbal cues can reduce the range of possible
interpretations. - Direction of speaker eye-gaze (Baldwin, 1991,
1993 Trueswell Gleitman, 2003 Nappa, Wessel,
McEldoon, Gleitman, Trueswell, 2009) - Joint attention Occurs naturally when parent
and child are focused on the same thing at the
same time (Baldwin, 1991 Bruner, 1978) - 70 of mothers utterances (Collis, 1977
Harris, Jones, Grant, 1983 Tomasello Todd,
1983) - Positively associated with early vocabulary
acquisition (Tomasello Todd, 1983 Harris,
Jones, Brookes, Grant, 1986 Tomasello, Mannle,
Kruger, 1986 Akhtar, Dunham, Dunham, 1991)
8Quality of Learning Instances(Baldwin, 1991)
- But what about the lack of perfect contingency
between word and referent?
9Follow-In vs Discrepant Labeling
Look! A dax!
10Quality of Learning Instances(Baldwin, 1991)
- But what about the lack of perfect contingency
between word and referent? - Follow-in labeling eye gaze, voice direction,
and body posture oriented at object child is
currently focused on - 16-19 mo-olds mapped correctly
- Discrepant labeling eye gaze, voice direction,
and body posture directed at a hidden (but
previously seen) object, while infant is focused
on another object - Infants did not map the word to the
focused-object.
11Social-attentional cues in interaction(Frank,
Goodman, Tenenbaum, In Press)
- Rollins corpus (CHILDES) mom and baby (6 mo)
- Social-attentional cues
- Infant Hands, Mouth (infant only), Touch,
Looking (direction of eye gaze) - Caregiver Hands, Touch, Looking (direction of
eye gaze) - Cross-situational word-learning model
successfully discovered the mappings between
words and objects.
12Social-attentional cues in interaction(Frank,
Goodman, Tenenbaum, In Press)
- Rollins corpus (CHILDES) mom and baby (6 mo)
- Social-attentional cues
- Infant Hands, Mouth (infant only), Touch,
Looking (direction of eye gaze) - Caregiver Hands, Touch, Looking (direction of
eye gaze) - Cross-situational word-learning model
successfully discovered the mappings between
words and objects.
13Social-attentional cues in interaction(Frank,
Goodman, Tenenbaum, In Press)
- Rollins corpus (CHILDES) mom and baby (6 mo)
- Social-attentional cues
- Infant Hands, Mouth (infant only), Touch,
Looking (direction of eye gaze) - Caregiver Hands, Touch, Looking (direction of
eye gaze) - Cross-situational word-learning model
successfully discovered the mappings between
words and objects. - Joint attention? Follow-in?
- What would interaction look like if child were
initiating actions?
14Our Goals
- Look at a representative sample of parent-child
interactions. - Explore the conditions under which word meaning
is transparent (or not) from extra-linguistic
cues alone - Presence (or absence) of cues
- Timing and coordination of cues
- Joint attention?
- Follow-in?
15Selection of Stimuli
- Large video corpus of parent-child interactions
in natural settings (home, outdoors, etc.) - Snedeker, J. (2001). Interactions between
infants (12-15 months) and their parents in four
settings. Unpublished corpus.
16Selection of Stimuli
- Word learning norming study
- Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., Gleitman, L., Joshi, A.,
Snedeker, J. (In progress). Machine
implementation of a verb learning algorithm. - Adapation of Human Simulation Paradigm (Gillette,
Gleitman, Gleitman, Lederer, 1999 Snedeker and
Gleitman, 1999) - Randomly selected six instances of highly
frequent content words. - Each instance was edited into a 40-second
vignette. - Sound turned off.
- Visual context only cue to word meaning, placing
viewers in the situation of the early word
learner. - Utterance of target word (at 30 sec) indicated by
a BEEP. - Guess the mystery word in each vignette.
17(No Transcript)
18(silence)
(silence)
ltBEEPgt
30 sec
(silence)
10 sec
Drawings courtesy of Emily Trueswell
19Selection of Stimuli
- Two types of vignettes
- High Informative vignettes guessed by gt50 of
participants - Low Informative vignettes guessed by lt33 of
participants
20Selection of Stimuli
- Two types of vignettes
- High Informative vignettes guessed by gt50 of
participants - Rare (only 7 of vignettes).
- All basic level objects.
- Low Informative vignettes guessed by lt33 of
participants - Stimuli for Current Study
- 8 nouns bag, ball, book, horse, necklace, nose,
phone, shoe - One HI vignette
- One LI vignette
21Pilot Study Children
- N 12 (ages 31 to 54)
- Modified for fun!
- Shorter vignettes with funny noises
- What do you think the parent said?
- Celebratory animation
22Pilot Study Children
?2 3.84
23Extra Linguistic Cue Coding
- Is the target object visible in the scene (on
screen)? - Is the child moving or reaching towards the
object? - Is the child handling the object?
- Is the child looking at the object?
- Is the child looking at the parent?
- Is the parent moving or reaching towards the
object? - Is the parent handling the object?
- Is the parent looking at the object?
- Is the parent looking at the child?
24Presence of Target Object
Error bars reflect Standard Error of the Mean.
25Cue Occurrence at Word Onset
?2 7.27
?2 4.00
?2 4.27
26Joint Attention at Word Onset
Child Looking at and/or Handling Object AND
Parent Looking at Object
?2 4.00
27What is the timing of cues?
- Follow-in?
- Parent refers to object under childs focus of
attention. - First onset of cues relative to word onset.
28First Onset of Cues
Error bars reflect Standard Error of the Mean.
Child Looking at Object t(1,12)1.56, p0.14
Child Moving/Reaching Toward Object t(1,12)2.05,
p0.06
Child Handling Object t(1,12)2.96, p0.01
Parent Handling Object t(1,8)1.09, p0.31
29First Onset of Cues
Error bars reflect Standard Error of the Mean.
Parent Looking at Child t(1,13)0.54, p0.59
Parent Looking at Object t(1,12)0.08, p0.93
Child Looking at Parent t(1,6)0.02, p0.98
Parent Moving/Reaching Toward Object t(1,7)0.15,
p0.89
30Differentiating HI and LI Vignettes
- High Informative
- Follow-In Utterance of target word immediately
after first onset of childs shift in focus
towards object. - Joint Attention Co-occurring high rates of
childs attention to object and parents
attention to child and object. - Low Informative
- Delayed follow-in.
- Low joint attention.
31Implications
- Basic Level Object Terms provide a scaffold for
further learning. - Word order, syntax, abstract lexical items, etc.
- Vindication of Bruner/Baldwins social conditions
for word learning found in natural parent-child
interactions. - Word learning is successful when cues align.