Title: Tara McAllister
1Patterns of Gestural Overlap Account for
Positional Fricative Neutralization in Child
Phonology
- Tara McAllister
- Montclair State University, Montclair, New
Jersey - mcallistert_at_mail.montclair.edu
2Outline
- An interesting data set from phonological
acquisition (positional fricative gliding). - Why these child data are difficult to square with
what we know about adult phonological typology. - Claim A phonetically-based approach to phonology
makes it possible to give a principled account of
child-specific phenomena. - Fricative substitution errors are analyzed as a
phonologized response to a child-specific
articulatory limitation on overlapping vowel and
fricative gestures. - Positional asymmetry emerges as the consequence
of differing degrees of gestural overlap
permitted in syllable-initial versus
syllable-final position (Articulatory Phonology).
3Case study data
- Data were collected from a single case study
subject between the ages of 39 and 43. - Ben is a monolingual English learner with
severe phonological delay/disorder. - Active phonological patterns in addition to the
pattern of interest - cluster reduction
- velar fronting
- liquid gliding
- final devoicing
- debuccalization of coda stops
4Positional neutralization of fricatives (PFN)
- Bens positional fricative neutralization pattern
(39-310) - Syllable-initial fricatives are realized as
glides. - ji see ja? shark
- joi sewing jip sheep
- jiba zebra wu? food
- woda? forgot
5Positional neutralization of fricatives (PFN)
- Bens positional fricative neutralization pattern
(39-310) - Syllable-initial fricatives are realized as
glides. - ji see ja? shark
- joi sewing jip sheep
- jiba zebra wu? food
- woda? forgot
- Syllable-final fricatives preserve faithful
manner (not necessarily place or voicing). - mas mouse j?? fish
- bis beans b??ba? Spongebob
- babajis strawberries was five
6Why is PFN of interest?
- Not unique to Ben.
- Numerous studies have documented children
acquiring fricatives in syllable-final before
syllable-initial contexts. - In babbling (Gildersleeve-Neumann et al. 2000
Oller Eilers, 1982 Redford et al. 1997) - In meaningful speech (Dinnsen, 1996 Edwards,
1996 Farwell, 1976 Ferguson, 1978 Stites,
Demuth, Kirk, 2004 Stoel-Gammon, 1985) - Pattern is not universal, but general consensus
is that fricatives in final position have a
favored status in acquisition (Edwards, 1979).
7Why is PFN of interest?
- PFN is a child-specific pattern that reverses a
strong bias in adult phonological typology. - In fully-developed phonologies, the maximum range
of featural contrasts is realized in
initial/prevocalic position. - Example Manner contrasts in Korean (Ahn, 1998)
- Stop, fricative, affricate manner allowed in
onset position. - All manner contrasts neutralized to stop in coda
position. - PFN belongs to set of child processes of
neutralization in strong position (Dinnsen
Farris-Trimble, 2008 Inkelas Rose, 2003, 2008
McAllister, 2009) - Challenge notion of continuity of child and adult
grammars.
8Neutralization in strong position
- Lets try to model PFN with a general constraint
Fricatives - In a positional faithfulness framework (Beckman,
1997), we need a constraint enhancing
faithfulness to weak/final position. - Ident-manner-weak gtgt Fricatives gtgt Ident-manner
- In a positional markedness framework (Smith,
2000, 2002), we need a constraint limiting
featural contrasts in strong position. - Fricatives gtgt Ident-manner gtgt Fricatives
- If Ident-manner-weak or Fricatives are possible
constraints, we should find examples of adult
phonologies with featural neutralization in
strong position. - Such grammars are in fact unattested.
9Phonetics in child phonology
- The challenge Model childrens positional
neutralization without making incorrect
predictions for the possible range of variation
in adult grammars. - My claim The most principled accounts of
child-specific phonological patterns have adopted
a phonetically-based approach to phonology
(Dinnsen Farris-Trimble, 2008 Inkelas Rose,
2003, 2007 McAllister, 2009 Pater, 1997). - It is uncontroversial that children and adults
experience the physical act of producing/perceivin
g speech in different ways. - Different articulatory anatomy and speech-motor
control - Different perceptual sensitivities
10Phonetically-based phonology
- Since children and adults are subject to
different low-level phonetic pressures, the
phonetically-based model predicts divergence in
their grammars as well. - If a speaker experiences a major change at the
phonetic level (e.g. articulatory maturation),
the grammar can change in response to the new
phonetic pressures. - Accounts for elimination of child-specific
phonological phenomena in the course of typical
maturation. - I will propose a formal phonological model of
Bens PFN pattern with roots in a child-specific
articulatory limitation.
11Child-specific phonetic limitations
- A phonetic difference between children and
adults Children have difficulty moving the
tongue independent of the jaw. - Tongue is motorically complex, with many degrees
of movement freedom (Kent, 1992). - Control of the jaw, a bilaterally hinged joint,
is motorically simple. - In early stages of motor maturation, tongue moves
passively with the active jaw (MacNeilage
Davis, 1990). - Even after some capacity for independent tongue
control is acquired, acoustic measurements reveal
an ongoing preference for jaw-dominated gestures
(Edwards, Fourakis, Beckman, Fox, 1999)
12Child-specific phonetic limitations
- My proposal Preference for jaw-dominated
gestures takes on phonological status. - Move-As-Unit Avoid jaw-independent tongue
gestures. - Move-As-Unit can be analogized to
effort-minimizing constraints in adult grammars. - Lazy Minimize articulatory effort (Kirchner,
2001) - MinimiseEffort (Flemming, 2001)
- Difference is that Move-As-Unit responds to a
type of movement that is effortful for children
but not for adults.
13Why are fricatives dispreferred?
- In adult speech, fricative-containing syllables
involve independent tongue and jaw control. - In a fricative-vowel syllable, the jaw reaches
its target before the tongue tip. (Tongue remains
high to sustain frication while jaw lowers in
anticipation of the upcoming vowel.) - In a vowel-fricative syllable, the tongue tip
reaches its target before the jaw (Mooshammer et
al., 2006). - A speaker who moves tongue and jaw as one unit
cannot achieve this coarticulation. - A typical coarticulated fricative-vowel or
vowel-fricative transition will thus violate
Move-As-Unit. - Stops and glides do not require differentiated
control of tongue and jaw (Kent, 1992).
14Why the positional asymmetry?
- Spectrograms of Bens output reveal an asymmetry
between initial and final fricatives - Syllable-initial fricatives make an immediate
transition into the following vowel. - Syllable-final fricatives tend to be separated
from the vowel by silence and/or aspiration
noise. - Pause separating vowel and coda fricative
indicates that the gestures may not overlap at
all. - No Move-As-Unit violation.
15Why the positional asymmetry?
- This pattern is not unique to Ben
- Target nose produced by a typically developing
child aged 211
16Why the positional asymmetry?
- Target kiss produced by a typically developing
child aged 36 - Non-overlapping vowel-fricative transitions can
be observed in the speech of typically developing
children.
17How general is the phenomenon?
- Measured 237 vowel-fricative and fricative-vowel
transitions elicited from 17 TD children aged
211-57 (mean 47). - Average duration of silence/aspiration noise
separating a vowel and a coda fricative was a
substantial 88.4 ms. - In 58.8 of tokens, this interval was 25 of
total vocalic interval (criterion for
preaspiration adapted from Gordeeva Scobbie,
2010). - This is despite the fact that adult American
English is thought to lack preaspiration of
fricatives (Turk, Nakai, Sugahara, 2006). - There was no significant difference in the
duration of silence/aspiration before a voiced
versus a voiceless fricative. - Between an onset fricative and the following
vowel, the mean duration of non-canonical
frication noise was 20.4 ms. - Only 4.1 met criterion for postaspiration.
18Why the positional asymmetry?
- Conclusion Child speakers produce
fricative-vowel transitions with a greater degree
of overlap than vowel-fricative transitions. - Lesser Move-As-Unit violation in the latter case.
- However, evidence that fricatives and vowels do
not always overlap in final position is
insufficient to account for PFN. - Necessary to explain why a comparable
non-overlapped transition is not available in
syllable-initial position.
19Constraints on gestural timing
- Articulatory Phonology Gestures stand in
characteristic timing relations with respect to
one another (Browman Goldstein, 1986 et seq.). - Characteristic patterns of gestural overlap can
be encoded in Optimality-Theoretic coordination
constraints (Gafos, 2002). - CV-Coord Align(C, C-Center, V, Onset)
- VC-Coord Align(V, Release, C, Target)
- Non-overlapping transitions violate
CV-Coord/VC-Coord - If CV-Coord gtgt VC-Coord, non-overlapping gestures
will be penalized more heavily in initial
relative to final position.
20Constraints on gestural timing
- Experimental evidence of syllable position
effects suggests that CV-Coord gtgt VC-Coord may be
the default. - Degree of overlap between a vowel and a coda
consonant varies with changes in rate or prosody,
but onset-vowel transitions maintain stable
timing across all conditions (Tuller Kelso,
1990, 1991). - Nam et al. (2010) CV and VC transitions have
different coupling modes and consequently
different coupling strength. - CV coupling is in-phase (synchronous), more
stable. - VC coupling is anti-phase (offset by 180), less
stable. - Accounts for developmental and typological
primacy of the CV syllable shape.
21Modeling Bens grammar
- PFN will occur when CV-Coord gtgt Faith gtgt
VC-Coord. - Harmonic Grammar framework turns out to be the
best fit for the data, but here classic OT is
used for simplicity. - Table 1. An initial fricative is replaced with a
glide.
22Modeling Bens grammar
- Table 2. A final fricative preserves faithful
manner.
23More evidence for the gestural account
- Before acquiring faithful fricatives in all
contexts, Ben went through an intermediate stage
(311-42) in which initial fricatives were
realized with an epenthetic glide - sj? saw sjak sock
- sja?t salt sjao? share
- sjo? sew ?jao? shell
- Epenthesis is a truly unexpected repair because
Bens phonology at the time lacked initial
consonant clusters, including obstruent-glide
clusters. - d?k clock bun spoon
- b?t bread d?s?n question
24More evidence for the gestural account
- Articulatory Phonology literature reveals several
cases where apparent epenthetic segments are the
perceptual consequence of non-overlapping
gestural coordination. - Perceived epenthetic schwa in coda clusters in
Moroccan Colloquial Arabic (Gafos, 2002). - Perceived epenthetic schwa in English speakers
attempted non-native clusters (Davidson, 2003). - Vocal tract is briefly open during non-overlapped
transition sound produced is perceived as an
epenthetic segment. - Transition from a vowel to a coda fricative has
come to feature a palatal glide in some
fully-developed phonologies, e.g. luz, light ?
lujs in certain dialects of Brazilian
Portuguese (Albano, 1999 Operstein, 2010).
25More evidence for the gestural account
- If Ident-Consonantal is promoted above CV-Coord,
the optimal candidate will feature a
non-overlapped fricative-vowel transition instead
of glide substitution.
26More evidence for the gestural account
- So why dont we hear a transitional glide in
Bens final vowel-fricative transitions? - Visual inspection of coda fricatives shows
cessation of voicing before onset of frication. - Preaspiration obscures formant transitions that
would create percept of epenthetic glide. - Finding that glottal opening occurs in advance of
the oral constriction for a fricative coda is
entirely consistent with the gestural
coordination analysis pursued here. - Syllable position effects affecting timing of
gestures within a compound segment (e.g. nasal,
voiceless obstruent). - In initial position, both gestures are roughly
synchronous. - In final position, glottal opening gesture tends
to precede the oral constriction (Krakow, 1999).
27Conclusions
- PFN is difficult to model without creating
incorrect predictions for the range of variation
in adult typology. - Roots in childrens articulatory limitations can
account for absence of pattern from adult
grammars. - Positional nature of fricative neutralization
follows from the fact that inter-gestural timing
is more tightly constrained in CV than VC
contexts. - Provides new evidence that patterns of
inter-gestural coordination previously described
in adult speakers are also influential in
developmental phonology.
28References
- Ahn, Sang-cheol (1998). An introduction to Korean
phonology. Seoul Hanshin Publishing. - Albano, Eleonora C. (1999). A gestural solution
for some glide epenthesis problems. In Ohala,
Hasegawa, Ohala, Granville Bailey (1999).
1785-1788. - Beckman, Jill N. (1997). Positional faithfulness,
positional neutralisation, and Shona vowel
harmony. Phonology 14. 1-46. - Browman, Catherine P. Louis Goldstein (1986).
Towards an articulatory phonology. Phonology
Yearbook 3. 219-252. - Browman, Catherine P. Louis Goldstein (1988).
Some notes on syllable structure in Articulatory
Phonology. Phonetica 45. 140-155. - Browman, Catherine P. Louis Goldstein (1990).
Tiers in articulatory phonology, with some
implications for casual speech. In Kingston
Beckman (1990). 341-376. - Davidson, Lisa (2003). The atoms of phonological
representation gestures, coordination and
perceptual features in consonant cluster
phonotactics. PhD dissertation, Johns Hopkins
University. - Dinnsen, Daniel A. (1996). Context effects in the
acquisition of fricatives. In Bernhardt,
Gilbert, Ingram (1996). 136-148. - Dinnsen, Daniel A. Ashley W. Farris-Trimble
(2008). The prominence paradox. In Daniel A.
Dinnsen Judith A. Gierut (eds.) Optimality
Theory, phonological acquisition and disorders.
London Equinox Publishing Ltd. 277-308. - Edwards, Mary Louise (1979). Word position in
fricative acquisition. Papers and Reports in
Child Language Development 16. 67-75. - Edwards, Mary Louise (1996). Word position and
the production of fricatives. In Bernhardt,
Gilbert, Ingram (1996). 149-158. - Edwards, Jan, Marios Fourakis, Mary E. Beckman
Robert A. Fox (1999). Characterizing knowledge
deficits in phonological disorders. Journal of
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42,
169186. - Farwell, Carol B. (1976). Some strategies in the
early production of fricatives. Papers and
Reports on Child Language Development 12. 97-104. - Ferguson, Charles A. (1978). Fricatives in child
language acquisition. In Vladimir Honsa Martha
J. Hardman-de-Bautista (eds.) Papers on
linguistics and child language. The Hague,
Netherlands Mouton. 93-115.
29References
- Flemming, Edward (2001). Scalar and categorical
phenomena in a unified model of phonetics and
phonology. Phonology 18. 7-44. - Gafos, Adamantios (2002). A grammar of gestural
coordination. NLLT 2b0. 269337. - Gildersleeve-Neumann, Christina E., Barbara L.
Davis Peter F. MacNeilage (2000). Contingencies
governing production of for fricatives,
affricates and liquids in babbling. Applied
Psycholinguistics 21. 341-363. - Gordeeva, Olga B., James M. Scobbie (2010).
Preaspiration as a correlate of word-final voice
in Scottish English fricatives. In Susanne
Fuchs, Martine Toda, Marzena Zygis (eds.)
Turbulent Sounds An Interdisciplinary Guide.
Berlin Mouton de Gruyter. 167-207. - Inkelas, Sharon Yvan Rose (2003). Velar
fronting revisited. In Barbara Beachley, Amanda
Brown, Frances Conlin (eds.) Proceedings of the
27th Annual Boston University Conference on
Language Development. Somerville, MA Cascadilla
Press. 334-345. - Inkelas, Sharon Yvan Rose (2008). Positional
neutralization a case study from child language.
Lg 83. 707-736. - Kent, Raymond D. (1992). The biology of
phonological maturation. In Charles A. Ferguson,
Lise Menn, Carol Stoel-Gammon (eds.)
Phonological development models, research,
implications. Timonium, MD York Press. 65-90. - Kirchner, Robert (2001). An effort-based approach
to consonant lenition. New York Routledge. - Krakow, Rena A. (1999). Physiological
organization of syllables a review. JPh 27.
23-54. - MacNeilage, Peter F. Barbara L. Davis (1990).
Acquisition of speech production frames, then
content. In Marc Jeannerod (ed.) Attention and
performance Vol. 13, motor representation and
control. Hillsdale, N.J. Erlbaum. 453-475 - McAllister, Tara (2009). The articulatory basis
of positional asymmetries in phonological
acquisition. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts
Institute of Technology. - Mooshammer, Christine, Philip Hoole Anja
Geumann (2006). Interarticulator cohesion within
coronal consonant production. JASA 120.
1028-1039. - Nam, Hosung, Louis Goldstein Elliot Saltzman
(2010). Self-organization of syllable structure
a coupled oscillator model. In François
Pellegrino, Egidio Marisco Ioana Chitoran
(eds.) Approaches to phonological complexity.
Berlin, New York Mouton de Gruyter. 299-328.
30References
- Oller, D. Kimbrough, Rebecca E. Eilers (1982).
Similarity of babbling in Spanish-leaning and
English-learning babies. Journal of Child
Language 9. 565-577. - Operstein, Natalie (2010). Consonant structure
and prevocalization. Philadelphia John
Benjamins. - Pater, Joe (1997). Minimal violation and
phonological development. Language Acquisition 6.
201-253. - Redford, Melissa A., Peter F. MacNeilage
Barbara L. Davis (1997). Perceptual and motor
influences on final consonant inventories in
babbling. Phonetica 54. 172-186. - Smith, Jennifer L. (2000). Prominence,
augmentation, and neutralization in phonology. In
L. Conathan, J. Good, D. Kavitskaya, A. Wulf,
A. Yu (Eds.), Proceedings of BLS 26 (pp.
247-257). Berkeley, CA Berkeley Linguistics
Society. Published version has formatting
errors corrected version available as Rutgers
Optimality Archive 727 (2005). - Smith, Jennifer L. (2002). Phonological
augmentation in prominent positions. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Massachusetts,
Amherst. - Stites, Jessica, Katherine Demuth, Cecilia Kirk
(2004). Markedness versus frequency effects in
coda acquisition. In Alejna Brugos, Linnea
Micciulla, Christine E. Smith (eds.)
Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University
Conference on Language Development. Somerville,
MA Cascadilla Press. 565-576. - Stoel-Gammon, Carol (1985). Phonetic inventories,
15-24 months a longitudinal study. Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research 18. 505-512. - Tuller, Betty J. A. Scott Kelso (1990). Phase
transitions in speech production and their
perceptual consequences. In Marc Jeannerod (ed.)
Attention and Performance XIII. Hillsdale, NJ
Erlbaum. 429-452. - Tuller, Betty J. A. Scott Kelso (1991). The
production and perception of syllable structure.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34.
501-504. - Turk, Alice, Satsuki Nakai, Mariko Sugahara
(2006). Acoustic segment durations in prosodic
research a practical guide. In Stefan Sudhoff,
Denisa Lenertová, Roland Meyer, Sandra Pappert,
Petra Augurzky, Ina Mleinek, Nicole Richter,
Johannes Schließer (eds.) Methods in empirical
prosody research. Berlin Walter de Gruyter.
127.