Tara McAllister - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 30
About This Presentation
Title:

Tara McAllister

Description:

Patterns of Gestural Overlap Account for Positional Fricative Neutralization in Child Phonology. ... This is despite the fact that adult American English is thought ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:117
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 31
Provided by: montc159
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Tara McAllister


1
Patterns of Gestural Overlap Account for
Positional Fricative Neutralization in Child
Phonology
  • Tara McAllister
  • Montclair State University, Montclair, New
    Jersey
  • mcallistert_at_mail.montclair.edu

2
Outline
  • An interesting data set from phonological
    acquisition (positional fricative gliding).
  • Why these child data are difficult to square with
    what we know about adult phonological typology.
  • Claim A phonetically-based approach to phonology
    makes it possible to give a principled account of
    child-specific phenomena.
  • Fricative substitution errors are analyzed as a
    phonologized response to a child-specific
    articulatory limitation on overlapping vowel and
    fricative gestures.
  • Positional asymmetry emerges as the consequence
    of differing degrees of gestural overlap
    permitted in syllable-initial versus
    syllable-final position (Articulatory Phonology).

3
Case study data
  • Data were collected from a single case study
    subject between the ages of 39 and 43.
  • Ben is a monolingual English learner with
    severe phonological delay/disorder.
  • Active phonological patterns in addition to the
    pattern of interest
  • cluster reduction
  • velar fronting
  • liquid gliding
  • final devoicing
  • debuccalization of coda stops

4
Positional neutralization of fricatives (PFN)
  • Bens positional fricative neutralization pattern
    (39-310)
  • Syllable-initial fricatives are realized as
    glides.
  • ji see ja? shark
  • joi sewing jip sheep
  • jiba zebra wu? food
  • woda? forgot  

5
Positional neutralization of fricatives (PFN)
  • Bens positional fricative neutralization pattern
    (39-310)
  • Syllable-initial fricatives are realized as
    glides.
  • ji see ja? shark
  • joi sewing jip sheep
  • jiba zebra wu? food
  • woda? forgot  
  • Syllable-final fricatives preserve faithful
    manner (not necessarily place or voicing).
  • mas mouse j?? fish
  • bis beans b??ba? Spongebob
  • babajis strawberries was five

6
Why is PFN of interest?
  • Not unique to Ben.
  • Numerous studies have documented children
    acquiring fricatives in syllable-final before
    syllable-initial contexts.
  • In babbling (Gildersleeve-Neumann et al. 2000
    Oller Eilers, 1982 Redford et al. 1997)
  • In meaningful speech (Dinnsen, 1996 Edwards,
    1996 Farwell, 1976 Ferguson, 1978 Stites,
    Demuth, Kirk, 2004 Stoel-Gammon, 1985)
  • Pattern is not universal, but general consensus
    is that fricatives in final position have a
    favored status in acquisition (Edwards, 1979).

7
Why is PFN of interest?
  • PFN is a child-specific pattern that reverses a
    strong bias in adult phonological typology.
  • In fully-developed phonologies, the maximum range
    of featural contrasts is realized in
    initial/prevocalic position.
  • Example Manner contrasts in Korean (Ahn, 1998)
  • Stop, fricative, affricate manner allowed in
    onset position.
  • All manner contrasts neutralized to stop in coda
    position.
  • PFN belongs to set of child processes of
    neutralization in strong position (Dinnsen
    Farris-Trimble, 2008 Inkelas Rose, 2003, 2008
    McAllister, 2009)
  • Challenge notion of continuity of child and adult
    grammars.

8
Neutralization in strong position
  • Lets try to model PFN with a general constraint
    Fricatives
  • In a positional faithfulness framework (Beckman,
    1997), we need a constraint enhancing
    faithfulness to weak/final position.
  • Ident-manner-weak gtgt Fricatives gtgt Ident-manner
  • In a positional markedness framework (Smith,
    2000, 2002), we need a constraint limiting
    featural contrasts in strong position.
  • Fricatives gtgt Ident-manner gtgt Fricatives
  • If Ident-manner-weak or Fricatives are possible
    constraints, we should find examples of adult
    phonologies with featural neutralization in
    strong position.
  • Such grammars are in fact unattested.

9
Phonetics in child phonology
  • The challenge Model childrens positional
    neutralization without making incorrect
    predictions for the possible range of variation
    in adult grammars.
  • My claim The most principled accounts of
    child-specific phonological patterns have adopted
    a phonetically-based approach to phonology
    (Dinnsen Farris-Trimble, 2008 Inkelas Rose,
    2003, 2007 McAllister, 2009 Pater, 1997).
  • It is uncontroversial that children and adults
    experience the physical act of producing/perceivin
    g speech in different ways.
  • Different articulatory anatomy and speech-motor
    control
  • Different perceptual sensitivities

10
Phonetically-based phonology
  • Since children and adults are subject to
    different low-level phonetic pressures, the
    phonetically-based model predicts divergence in
    their grammars as well.
  • If a speaker experiences a major change at the
    phonetic level (e.g. articulatory maturation),
    the grammar can change in response to the new
    phonetic pressures.
  • Accounts for elimination of child-specific
    phonological phenomena in the course of typical
    maturation.
  • I will propose a formal phonological model of
    Bens PFN pattern with roots in a child-specific
    articulatory limitation.

11
Child-specific phonetic limitations
  • A phonetic difference between children and
    adults Children have difficulty moving the
    tongue independent of the jaw.
  • Tongue is motorically complex, with many degrees
    of movement freedom (Kent, 1992).
  • Control of the jaw, a bilaterally hinged joint,
    is motorically simple.
  • In early stages of motor maturation, tongue moves
    passively with the active jaw (MacNeilage
    Davis, 1990).
  • Even after some capacity for independent tongue
    control is acquired, acoustic measurements reveal
    an ongoing preference for jaw-dominated gestures
    (Edwards, Fourakis, Beckman, Fox, 1999)

12
Child-specific phonetic limitations
  • My proposal Preference for jaw-dominated
    gestures takes on phonological status.
  • Move-As-Unit Avoid jaw-independent tongue
    gestures.
  • Move-As-Unit can be analogized to
    effort-minimizing constraints in adult grammars.
  • Lazy Minimize articulatory effort (Kirchner,
    2001)
  • MinimiseEffort (Flemming, 2001)
  • Difference is that Move-As-Unit responds to a
    type of movement that is effortful for children
    but not for adults.

13
Why are fricatives dispreferred?
  • In adult speech, fricative-containing syllables
    involve independent tongue and jaw control.
  • In a fricative-vowel syllable, the jaw reaches
    its target before the tongue tip. (Tongue remains
    high to sustain frication while jaw lowers in
    anticipation of the upcoming vowel.)
  • In a vowel-fricative syllable, the tongue tip
    reaches its target before the jaw (Mooshammer et
    al., 2006).
  • A speaker who moves tongue and jaw as one unit
    cannot achieve this coarticulation.
  • A typical coarticulated fricative-vowel or
    vowel-fricative transition will thus violate
    Move-As-Unit.
  • Stops and glides do not require differentiated
    control of tongue and jaw (Kent, 1992).

14
Why the positional asymmetry?
  • Spectrograms of Bens output reveal an asymmetry
    between initial and final fricatives
  • Syllable-initial fricatives make an immediate
    transition into the following vowel.
  • Syllable-final fricatives tend to be separated
    from the vowel by silence and/or aspiration
    noise.
  • Pause separating vowel and coda fricative
    indicates that the gestures may not overlap at
    all.
  • No Move-As-Unit violation.

15
Why the positional asymmetry?
  • This pattern is not unique to Ben
  • Target nose produced by a typically developing
    child aged 211

16
Why the positional asymmetry?
  • Target kiss produced by a typically developing
    child aged 36
  • Non-overlapping vowel-fricative transitions can
    be observed in the speech of typically developing
    children.

17
How general is the phenomenon?
  • Measured 237 vowel-fricative and fricative-vowel
    transitions elicited from 17 TD children aged
    211-57 (mean 47).
  • Average duration of silence/aspiration noise
    separating a vowel and a coda fricative was a
    substantial 88.4 ms.
  • In 58.8 of tokens, this interval was 25 of
    total vocalic interval (criterion for
    preaspiration adapted from Gordeeva Scobbie,
    2010).
  • This is despite the fact that adult American
    English is thought to lack preaspiration of
    fricatives (Turk, Nakai, Sugahara, 2006).
  • There was no significant difference in the
    duration of silence/aspiration before a voiced
    versus a voiceless fricative.
  • Between an onset fricative and the following
    vowel, the mean duration of non-canonical
    frication noise was 20.4 ms.
  • Only 4.1 met criterion for postaspiration.

18
Why the positional asymmetry?
  • Conclusion Child speakers produce
    fricative-vowel transitions with a greater degree
    of overlap than vowel-fricative transitions.
  • Lesser Move-As-Unit violation in the latter case.
  • However, evidence that fricatives and vowels do
    not always overlap in final position is
    insufficient to account for PFN.
  • Necessary to explain why a comparable
    non-overlapped transition is not available in
    syllable-initial position.

19
Constraints on gestural timing
  • Articulatory Phonology Gestures stand in
    characteristic timing relations with respect to
    one another (Browman Goldstein, 1986 et seq.).
  • Characteristic patterns of gestural overlap can
    be encoded in Optimality-Theoretic coordination
    constraints (Gafos, 2002).
  • CV-Coord Align(C, C-Center, V, Onset)
  • VC-Coord Align(V, Release, C, Target)
  • Non-overlapping transitions violate
    CV-Coord/VC-Coord
  • If CV-Coord gtgt VC-Coord, non-overlapping gestures
    will be penalized more heavily in initial
    relative to final position.

20
Constraints on gestural timing
  • Experimental evidence of syllable position
    effects suggests that CV-Coord gtgt VC-Coord may be
    the default.
  • Degree of overlap between a vowel and a coda
    consonant varies with changes in rate or prosody,
    but onset-vowel transitions maintain stable
    timing across all conditions (Tuller Kelso,
    1990, 1991).
  • Nam et al. (2010) CV and VC transitions have
    different coupling modes and consequently
    different coupling strength.
  • CV coupling is in-phase (synchronous), more
    stable.
  • VC coupling is anti-phase (offset by 180), less
    stable.
  • Accounts for developmental and typological
    primacy of the CV syllable shape.

21
Modeling Bens grammar
  • PFN will occur when CV-Coord gtgt Faith gtgt
    VC-Coord.
  • Harmonic Grammar framework turns out to be the
    best fit for the data, but here classic OT is
    used for simplicity.
  • Table 1. An initial fricative is replaced with a
    glide.

22
Modeling Bens grammar
  • Table 2. A final fricative preserves faithful
    manner.

23
More evidence for the gestural account
  • Before acquiring faithful fricatives in all
    contexts, Ben went through an intermediate stage
    (311-42) in which initial fricatives were
    realized with an epenthetic glide
  • sj? saw sjak sock
  • sja?t salt sjao? share
  • sjo? sew ?jao? shell
  • Epenthesis is a truly unexpected repair because
    Bens phonology at the time lacked initial
    consonant clusters, including obstruent-glide
    clusters.
  • d?k clock bun spoon
  • b?t bread d?s?n question

24
More evidence for the gestural account
  • Articulatory Phonology literature reveals several
    cases where apparent epenthetic segments are the
    perceptual consequence of non-overlapping
    gestural coordination.
  • Perceived epenthetic schwa in coda clusters in
    Moroccan Colloquial Arabic (Gafos, 2002).
  • Perceived epenthetic schwa in English speakers
    attempted non-native clusters (Davidson, 2003).
  • Vocal tract is briefly open during non-overlapped
    transition sound produced is perceived as an
    epenthetic segment.
  • Transition from a vowel to a coda fricative has
    come to feature a palatal glide in some
    fully-developed phonologies, e.g. luz, light ?
    lujs in certain dialects of Brazilian
    Portuguese (Albano, 1999 Operstein, 2010).

25
More evidence for the gestural account
  • If Ident-Consonantal is promoted above CV-Coord,
    the optimal candidate will feature a
    non-overlapped fricative-vowel transition instead
    of glide substitution.

26
More evidence for the gestural account
  • So why dont we hear a transitional glide in
    Bens final vowel-fricative transitions?
  • Visual inspection of coda fricatives shows
    cessation of voicing before onset of frication.
  • Preaspiration obscures formant transitions that
    would create percept of epenthetic glide.
  • Finding that glottal opening occurs in advance of
    the oral constriction for a fricative coda is
    entirely consistent with the gestural
    coordination analysis pursued here.
  • Syllable position effects affecting timing of
    gestures within a compound segment (e.g. nasal,
    voiceless obstruent).
  • In initial position, both gestures are roughly
    synchronous.
  • In final position, glottal opening gesture tends
    to precede the oral constriction (Krakow, 1999).

27
Conclusions
  • PFN is difficult to model without creating
    incorrect predictions for the range of variation
    in adult typology.
  • Roots in childrens articulatory limitations can
    account for absence of pattern from adult
    grammars.
  • Positional nature of fricative neutralization
    follows from the fact that inter-gestural timing
    is more tightly constrained in CV than VC
    contexts.
  • Provides new evidence that patterns of
    inter-gestural coordination previously described
    in adult speakers are also influential in
    developmental phonology.

28
References
  • Ahn, Sang-cheol (1998). An introduction to Korean
    phonology. Seoul Hanshin Publishing.
  • Albano, Eleonora C. (1999). A gestural solution
    for some glide epenthesis problems. In Ohala,
    Hasegawa, Ohala, Granville Bailey (1999).
    1785-1788.
  • Beckman, Jill N. (1997). Positional faithfulness,
    positional neutralisation, and Shona vowel
    harmony. Phonology 14. 1-46.
  • Browman, Catherine P. Louis Goldstein (1986).
    Towards an articulatory phonology. Phonology
    Yearbook 3. 219-252.
  • Browman, Catherine P. Louis Goldstein (1988).
    Some notes on syllable structure in Articulatory
    Phonology. Phonetica 45. 140-155.
  • Browman, Catherine P. Louis Goldstein (1990).
    Tiers in articulatory phonology, with some
    implications for casual speech. In Kingston
    Beckman (1990). 341-376.
  • Davidson, Lisa (2003). The atoms of phonological
    representation gestures, coordination and
    perceptual features in consonant cluster
    phonotactics. PhD dissertation, Johns Hopkins
    University.
  • Dinnsen, Daniel A. (1996). Context effects in the
    acquisition of fricatives. In Bernhardt,
    Gilbert, Ingram (1996). 136-148.
  • Dinnsen, Daniel A. Ashley W. Farris-Trimble
    (2008). The prominence paradox. In Daniel A.
    Dinnsen Judith A. Gierut (eds.) Optimality
    Theory, phonological acquisition and disorders.
    London Equinox Publishing Ltd. 277-308.
  • Edwards, Mary Louise (1979). Word position in
    fricative acquisition. Papers and Reports in
    Child Language Development 16. 67-75.
  • Edwards, Mary Louise (1996). Word position and
    the production of fricatives. In Bernhardt,
    Gilbert, Ingram (1996). 149-158.
  • Edwards, Jan, Marios Fourakis, Mary E. Beckman
    Robert A. Fox (1999). Characterizing knowledge
    deficits in phonological disorders. Journal of
    Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42,
    169186.
  • Farwell, Carol B. (1976). Some strategies in the
    early production of fricatives. Papers and
    Reports on Child Language Development 12. 97-104.
  • Ferguson, Charles A. (1978). Fricatives in child
    language acquisition. In Vladimir Honsa Martha
    J. Hardman-de-Bautista (eds.) Papers on
    linguistics and child language. The Hague,
    Netherlands Mouton. 93-115.

29
References
  • Flemming, Edward (2001). Scalar and categorical
    phenomena in a unified model of phonetics and
    phonology. Phonology 18. 7-44.
  • Gafos, Adamantios (2002). A grammar of gestural
    coordination. NLLT 2b0. 269337.
  • Gildersleeve-Neumann, Christina E., Barbara L.
    Davis Peter F. MacNeilage (2000). Contingencies
    governing production of for fricatives,
    affricates and liquids in babbling. Applied
    Psycholinguistics 21. 341-363.
  • Gordeeva, Olga B., James M. Scobbie (2010).
    Preaspiration as a correlate of word-final voice
    in Scottish English fricatives. In Susanne
    Fuchs, Martine Toda, Marzena Zygis (eds.)
    Turbulent Sounds An Interdisciplinary Guide.
    Berlin Mouton de Gruyter. 167-207.
  • Inkelas, Sharon Yvan Rose (2003). Velar
    fronting revisited. In Barbara Beachley, Amanda
    Brown, Frances Conlin (eds.) Proceedings of the
    27th Annual Boston University Conference on
    Language Development. Somerville, MA Cascadilla
    Press. 334-345.
  • Inkelas, Sharon Yvan Rose (2008). Positional
    neutralization a case study from child language.
    Lg 83. 707-736.
  • Kent, Raymond D. (1992). The biology of
    phonological maturation. In Charles A. Ferguson,
    Lise Menn, Carol Stoel-Gammon (eds.)
    Phonological development models, research,
    implications. Timonium, MD York Press. 65-90.
  • Kirchner, Robert (2001). An effort-based approach
    to consonant lenition. New York Routledge.
  • Krakow, Rena A. (1999). Physiological
    organization of syllables a review. JPh 27.
    23-54.
  • MacNeilage, Peter F. Barbara L. Davis (1990).
    Acquisition of speech production frames, then
    content. In Marc Jeannerod (ed.) Attention and
    performance Vol. 13, motor representation and
    control. Hillsdale, N.J. Erlbaum. 453-475
  • McAllister, Tara (2009). The articulatory basis
    of positional asymmetries in phonological
    acquisition. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts
    Institute of Technology.
  • Mooshammer, Christine, Philip Hoole Anja
    Geumann (2006). Interarticulator cohesion within
    coronal consonant production. JASA 120.
    1028-1039.
  • Nam, Hosung, Louis Goldstein Elliot Saltzman
    (2010). Self-organization of syllable structure
    a coupled oscillator model. In François
    Pellegrino, Egidio Marisco Ioana Chitoran
    (eds.) Approaches to phonological complexity.
    Berlin, New York Mouton de Gruyter. 299-328.

30
References
  • Oller, D. Kimbrough, Rebecca E. Eilers (1982).
    Similarity of babbling in Spanish-leaning and
    English-learning babies. Journal of Child
    Language 9. 565-577.
  • Operstein, Natalie (2010). Consonant structure
    and prevocalization. Philadelphia John
    Benjamins.
  • Pater, Joe (1997). Minimal violation and
    phonological development. Language Acquisition 6.
    201-253.
  • Redford, Melissa A., Peter F. MacNeilage
    Barbara L. Davis (1997). Perceptual and motor
    influences on final consonant inventories in
    babbling. Phonetica 54. 172-186.
  • Smith, Jennifer L. (2000). Prominence,
    augmentation, and neutralization in phonology. In
    L. Conathan, J. Good, D. Kavitskaya, A. Wulf,
    A. Yu (Eds.), Proceedings of BLS 26 (pp.
    247-257). Berkeley, CA Berkeley Linguistics
    Society. Published version has formatting
    errors corrected version available as Rutgers
    Optimality Archive 727 (2005).
  • Smith, Jennifer L. (2002). Phonological
    augmentation in prominent positions. Doctoral
    dissertation, University of Massachusetts,
    Amherst.
  • Stites, Jessica, Katherine Demuth, Cecilia Kirk
    (2004). Markedness versus frequency effects in
    coda acquisition. In Alejna Brugos, Linnea
    Micciulla, Christine E. Smith (eds.)
    Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University
    Conference on Language Development. Somerville,
    MA  Cascadilla Press. 565-576.
  • Stoel-Gammon, Carol (1985). Phonetic inventories,
    15-24 months a longitudinal study. Journal of
    Speech and Hearing Research 18. 505-512.
  • Tuller, Betty J. A. Scott Kelso (1990). Phase
    transitions in speech production and their
    perceptual consequences. In Marc Jeannerod (ed.)
    Attention and Performance XIII. Hillsdale, NJ
    Erlbaum. 429-452.
  • Tuller, Betty J. A. Scott Kelso (1991). The
    production and perception of syllable structure.
    Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34.
    501-504.
  • Turk, Alice, Satsuki Nakai, Mariko Sugahara
    (2006). Acoustic segment durations in prosodic
    research a practical guide. In Stefan Sudhoff,
    Denisa Lenertová, Roland Meyer, Sandra Pappert,
    Petra Augurzky, Ina Mleinek, Nicole Richter,
    Johannes Schließer (eds.) Methods in empirical
    prosody research. Berlin Walter de Gruyter.
    127.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com