Motor Cognition - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Motor Cognition

Description:

Motor Cognition Overview Many people believe processes used to plan and enact a movement can be used in problem solving and reasoning Moreover, the processes involved ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:173
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 76
Provided by: Baycre8
Category:
Tags: fmri | cognition | motor | week

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Motor Cognition


1
Motor Cognition
  • Overview
  • Many people believe processes used to plan and
    enact a movement can be used in problem solving
    and reasoning
  • Moreover, the processes involved in perceiving
    action are also involved in movement
  • This lecture will introduce key ideas involved in
    motor cognition and memory for action needed
  • in planning, and producing our own actions
    anticipating, perceiving, and interpreting the
    actions of others
  • In remembering actions

2
Motor Cognition
  • Terminology
  • Movement is a voluntary displacement of a body
    part in space
  • Action a series of movements performed to
    achieve a goal

3
Motor Cognition
  • Perception-action cycle
  • Refers to the transformation of perceived
    patterns (often visually) into coordinated
    patterns of movement
  • Examples. Returning a tennis ball, picking up a
    mug of coffee, walking on uneven terrain
  • According to this perspective much of human
    behavior involves the interconnection of
    perception and movement
  • The mediating link between perception and action
    is that a shared representation that is, the
    coding of perception and action is shared in the
    brain

4
Motor Cognition
  • Motor processing in the brain
  • Neuroanatomy
  • Area M1, the primary cortex. Neurons in this
    region control fine motor movements, and send
    fibers out to muscles themselves
  • Premotor area (PM) sets up program for motor
    sequences, and send input into M1
  • Supplementary motor area (SMA) sets up and
    executes motor plans

5
Motor Cognition
  • Motor processing in the brain
  • Neuroanatomy
  • Think of these 3 areas as a hierarchy. M1 is at
    the bottom of the hierarchy, and it enables
    specific movements PM higher up and it enables
    sets of movements at top SMA represents
    overarching plans of action

6
Primary motor cortex (M1)
Posterior parietal cortex
Supplementary motor cortex (SMA)
Premotor cortex
7
Motor Cognition
  • Some evidence for roles of three areas
  • Mushiake (1991)
  • Recorded single-cell activity in monkeys in M1,
    PM, and SMA
  • 2 conditions of interest were IT (internally
    triggered) and VT (visually triggered)
  • In both conditions monkeys were required to touch
    3 pads on a panel in the IT condition monkeys
    needed to remember sequence and then touch the
    panels in the remembered order in the VT
    condition monkeys touched panels as they were
    illuminated

8
Motor Cognition
  • Some evidence for roles of three areas
  • M1 cells were active in the premovement and
    movement periods consistent with the hypothesis
    that M1 is involved in movement
  • More SMA cells were active in the IT than the VT
    condition, consistent with the idea that SMA is
    important during planning since planning is more
    important in IT than VT condition
  • More neurons were active in the VT than the IT
    condition during the premovement and movement
    periods

9
Motor Cognition
  • Some evidence for roles of three areas
  • Conclusions
  • Movement planning and production involves a
    number of neuropsychological processes with
    different functions. These processes occur in
    different brain regions, and often occur
    simultaneously
  • Planning and then producing a response involves
    different neural processes than responding to
    environmental cues

10
Motor Cognition
  • Summary
  • We have reviewed the role of just 3 brain regions
    involved in motor cognition
  • Other regions are also involved with their
    involvement depending on the precise nature of
    the task in fact its been said that it takes
    the whole brain to make a cup of coffee

11
Motor Cognition
  • Shared representation
  • A considerable amount of data suggests that we
    can efficiently represent the actions made by
    other people (e.g., through viewing)
  • It has been hypothesized this occurs (and
    brain-activation studies support this) because
    the representation of perceived action and
    produced action is shared
  • These representations enable us to interpret the
    actions of others, respond appropriately, and
    efficiently learn how to physically produce
    actions that were viewed

12
Motor Cognition
  • Mirror Neurons
  • Mirror neuronsrefers to neurons that fire when
    organism (monkey) performs a specific grasping
    movement, and when that same grasping movement is
    observed being performed by experimenter or monkey

13
Motor Cognition
  • Mirror Neurons
  • Human evidence
  • A variety of techniques have been used to provide
    evidence for motor neurons in humans
  • A transcranial magnetic stimulation study showed
    increased excitability in the motor system during
    observation of actions performed by another
    person, but only for the brain regions involved
    in the muscles used by the other person

14
Motor Cognition
  • Apraxia
  • Definition
  • An impaired ability to generate skilled actions
    that cannot be attributed to basic sensory or
    motor disturbance
  • Generally conceptualized to reflect a disruption
    of a distributed praxis network
  • Research has often focused on transitive actions
    -- actions that involve manipulation of a tool or
    object
  • Examples use of a hammer, spatula,
  • Apraxia frequently observed after neurological
    damage
  • praxis network was thought to be left lateralized

15
Motor Cognition
Diagram of praxis network
16
Component Model ApproachRoy and Square, 1994
Roy, 1996
Sensory/Perceptual System
Visual/Gestural Information
Auditory/Verbal Information
Visual Tool/Object Information
Pantomime
Conceptual System
Knowledge of Action
Knowledge of Tool/Object Function
Production System
Response Selection
Image Generation
Delayed Imitation
Working Memory
Concurrent Imitation
Response Organization/Control
17
Component Model ApproachRoy and Square, 1994
Roy, 1996
Sensory/Perceptual System
Visual/Gestural Information
Auditory/Verbal Information
Visual Tool/Object Information
Main Responsibilities Analyzing visual gestural
information Identifying key features of tools
and objects for use
18
Component Model ApproachRoy and Square, 1994
Roy, 1996
Sensory/Perceptual System
Visual/Gestural Information
Auditory/Verbal Information
Visual Tool/Object Information
Conceptual System
Knowledge of Action
Knowledge of Tool/Object Function
Main Responsibilities Understanding tools,
objects, and gestures different types of
conceptual knowledge are dissociable
19
Motor Cognition
  • Conceptual knowledge
  • It appears that different types of conceptual
    knowledge associated with action are dissociable
    from each other as demonstrated in the next slides

20
Motor Cognition
  • Conceptual knowledge
  • Function knowledge vs manipulation knowledge
  • Buxbaum and Saffran (2002) investigated function
    and manipulation knowledge in apraxic and
    non-apraxic patients with LHD (aside, px were
    apraxic to gestural tests including tests of
    pantomime)
  • Function knowledge which two items are most
    similar in function (e.g., stapler, cellophane
    tape, pen)
  • Manipulation knowledge which two items are most
    similar in manner of manipulation (e.g., piano,
    typewriter, stove)

21
Motor Cognition
  • Conceptual knowledge
  • Results apraxic patients were more impaired in
    manipulation test, but than function test
  • Kellenbach et al. (2003) used PET to investigate
    brain activation associated with function and
    manipulation judgments
  • Results showed that when participants made
    conceptual judgments about function, there was
    activation of a left network consisting of the
    ventral premotor cortex and the posterior middle
    temporal gyrus
  • When participants made manipulation judgment an
    additional region, and additional region, the
    intraparietal sulcus, was activated

22
Motor Cognition
  • Conceptual knowledge
  • The intraparietal sulcus plays an important role
    in skilled object use (Heilman, 1993)

23
Motor Cognition
  • Conceptual knowledge
  • Visual-gestural knowledge
  • Beauchamp (2002) in neuroimaging study showed
    that bilateral regions of the middle temporal
    cortex were activated when tool motion (i.e.,
    gestural motion) was viewed in comparison to
    human motion (i.e., person jogging on the spot)

24
Motor Cognition
  • Conceptual knowledge
  • Beauchamp (2003) in neuroimaging study showed
    that middle temporal gyrus responded more
    strongly to tool motion videos and point-light
    displays of tool motion

25
Motor Cognition
  • Point-light displays (aside)
  • Animals and humans move in ways that are
    distinctive and different from the way in which
    nonhumans move. These patterns of movement are
    called biological motion
  • Johansson (1973) developed the point-light
    display technique to investigate movement.
  • Small light sources attached wrists, knees,
    ankles, shoulders, and heads of actors who
    performed various movements (e.g., walking,
    running, dancing)

26
Motor Cognition
  • Conceptual knowledge
  • Park and Roy (in prep) showed that patients with
    LHD, but not RHD were strongly impaired on
    function and manipulation tests but that patients
    with LHD and RHD were impaired on tests of
    visual-gestural knowledge

27
Motor Cognition
  • Conclusions
  • These studies suggest that different types of
    conceptual knowledge associated with action are
    dissociable from each other
  • Three types of knowledge have been studied in
    depth. These are knowledge of
  • Function
  • Manipulation
  • Visual-gestural knowledge of tool motion

28
Motor Cognition
  • Imitation in this model can be accomplished in
    two different ways
  • 1. directly from perception to action and
  • 2. indirectly through long-term memory
  • Evidence to support this comes from studies which
    have shown
  • The general observation that meaningless actions
    can be imitated accurately in cognitively
    unimpaired individuals
  • Findings of dissociation between imitation and
    pantomime (e.g., Goldenberg Hagmann, 1997
    Ochipa et al., 1994) stronger lateralization to
    pantomime than to imitation
  • (interpret on basis of model)

29
Motor Cognition
  • What is acquired when we view purposeful action
  • It would appear that we derive the goal of the
    action
  • (e.g., see a person reaching hand across table,
    grasping a mug of coffee, and moving is toward
    lips would be described as drinking a cup of
    coffee. In other words viewed actions tend to be
    described in terms of the goal of the action

30
Motor Cognition
  • Imitation
  • Imitation -- ability to understand the intent of
    a viewed action and then to reproduce it
  • This needs to be distinguished from mimicry,
    which is reproduction of a behavior without
    understanding (e.g., a parrot mimics human
    speech)
  • Meltzoff and Moore (1977) showed that newborn
    infants can imitate viewed action (sticking out
    tongue opening mouth etc.)
  • By 6 months of age infants can imitate actions on
    objects (e.g., shaking a rattle)

31
Motor Cognition
  • Imitation
  • As infants grow older deferred imitation
    abilities increase (i.e., memory for imitated
    action)
  • data show that infants as young as 18 months
    appear to represent intentions of actions not
    just the action itself
  • E.g., children who watched an actor try to pull
    apart a dumbbell but failed, were more likely to
    try and pull apart the dumbbell than if they
    watched a mechanical device attempt to pull apart
    a dumbbell

32
Motor Cognition
  • Components of imitation
  • Decety et al. (1997) in neuroimaging studies
    compared brain activation of subjects as they
    viewed meaningless actions either intending to
    recognize or to imitate the viewed action
  • Additional brain regions activated when intending
    imitate meaningless actions supplementary motor
    area (SMA), the middle frontal gyrus, the
    premotor cortex, the anterior cingulate, and the
    superior and inferior parietal cortices in both
    hemispheres.

33
Motor Cognition
  • Conclusion intentions (top-down) processes of
    participant influenced observation of action.
    Regions activated during observation also are the
    ones involved in action generation.
  • Observing an action with the intention to perform
    that action involves regions similar to those
    used to generate the action
  • when intending to recognize an action activated
    regions were the memory encoding structures (the
    parahippocampal gyrus)

34
Motor Cognition
  • When actions are viewed separating intention
    from means a neuroimaging perspective
  • Several people have proposed that actions are
    often understood in terms of the intentions
    (goals) they achieve and the means used
    (movements) to achieve these goals (e.g., Heider)
  • Chaminade (2002) investigated using neuroimaging
    the neural regions associated with goals and
    means

35
Motor Cognition
  • When actions are viewed separating intention
    from means a neuroimaging perspective
  • Participants saw an actor make Lego
    constructions participants viewed the goal alone
    (hand moving away from block in specified
    location) the means alone (the hand grasping and
    moving the block) or the entire action. All
    participants imitated action just observed

36
Motor Cognition
  • When actions are viewed separating intention
    from means a neuroimaging perspective
  • Findings when participants imitated action or
    means, the medial prefrontal cortex was
    activated this region appears to play a critical
    role in inferring other peoples intentions
  • When participants imitated goal the left premotor
    cortex activated
  • Conclusion
  • In normally functioning adults imitating a
    gesture activates neural regions associated with
    the intentions underlying the action

37
Motor Cognition
  • Mental simulation
  • Since imagery and perception activate similar
    brain regions it seems reasonable to hypothesize
    that one way to reason is to simulate (or
    imagine) the consequence of performing a planned
    action

38
Motor Cognition
  • Simulation theories of action understanding
  • It has been proposed that actions of others are
    understood by putting yourself in their place
    (either by observation or imagination)
  • This permits you to derive their intentions and
    generate an action plan (but how can you do this
    since intentions and actions are internal and
    unobservable?)

39
Motor Cognition
  • Mental simulation
  • It has been shown that practicing with mental
    imagery can help participants in their
    performance of the task
  • It has been shown that mental imagery practice
    has positive effects on complex motor skill
    learning (e.g., putting a golf ball)
  • Yue Cole (1992) showed compared finger strength
    of two groups group 1 performed repeated
    isometric exercises group 2 received motor
    imagery instruction and imagined making finger
    movements without actually making them
  • Both groups had increased finger strength group
    1 by 30 and group 2 by 22
  • Conclusion possible to increase strength without
    actually repeated muscle activation

40
Motor Cognition
  • Mental simulation
  • It has also been shown that viewing an action can
    facilitate enactment of that action
  • Priming refers to the facilitation of processing
    by previous performance of a task
  • Motor priming refers to the facilitative effect
    that watching a movement or action has on making
    a similar motor response. Motor priming has been
    observed in a variety of experimental situations

41
Motor Cognition
  • Mental simulation
  • fMRI studies have shown that the neural
    difference between motor imagery and motor
    performance is not a matter of what, but how
    much
  • i.e., similar brain regions are activated, but
    the level of activation is significantly lower
    in one study imagery activation was 30 of that
    found in actual execution (Roth, 1996)

42
Motor Cognition
  • Mental simulation
  • Ruby Decety (2001) Nature Neuroscience
    investigated the question of agency
  • Backgroundif viewing an action activates regions
    involved in performing an action, how do people
    distinguish actions they perform vs those they
    observe (i.e., attribution of action to self or
    another agent)
  • Previous studies have shown that the first-person
    perspective (imagining oneself) is associated
    with activation of inferior parietal, premotor,
    and SMA on left side

43
Motor Cognition
  • Mental simulation
  • This study asked what areas are activated when we
    imagine not ourselves performing an action, but
    another person performing that action
  • Method
  • Participants were scanned while they simulated
    everyday actions (e.g., winding a watch) with
    right hand (all Ps were right handed)
  • Ps instructed to imagine themselves perform the
    action or to imagine another person performing
    the action
  • Perspectives initiated by presenting a photo or
    from a spoken sentence describing the action

44
Motor Cognition
  • Conclusions
  • First person perspective versus imaging another
    person acting was associated with activation of
    common neural resources
  • Consistent with notion that a common code is used
    to perceive, imagine, and produce actions
  • However, specific regions are activated when
    imagining oneself performing an action versus
    another person. These regions may be used to
    determine agency

45
Motor Cognition
  • Mental simulation
  • Results
  • Both self perspective and other perspective
    activated common regions supplementary motor
    area (SMA), premotor cortex, precuneus (an area
    located in parietal lobe), and occipital-temporal
    lobe
  • However, when compared to the first-person
    perspective, the third-person perspective
    selectively activated the frontopolar cortex, the
    precuneus, and the right inferior portion of the
    parietal lobe
  • See figure in next slide

46
Ruby Decety (2001)
47
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for action
  • Subject-performed task (SPT) paradigm requires
    participant(P) to perform actions according to
    verbal instructions given by experimenter (e.g.,
    roll the ball, fold the paper, lift the pen) at
    study
  • At test Ps memory for these actions is tested
  • Control condition P hears instructions but does
    not perform actions
  • Resultmemory for enacted action phrases is
    superior to that for events encoded without
    enactment

Presentation relies on Nilsson (2000) In Craik
and Tulving Oxford Handbook of memory
48
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for action--theories
  • Non-strategic encoding theory of Cohen
  • This theory proposed that enacted actions are
    encoded nonstrategically unlike verbal and other
    types of events

49
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for action--theories
  • Multimodal theory of Backman and Nilsson (1984,
    1985)
  • Enactment during encoding automatically leads to
    multimodal processing, which produces a rich
    encoding of information (multimodal because there
    is auditory, visual, and haptic input) in SPT
    condition
  • subsequently proposed that physical (perceptual)
    properties were encoded nonstrategically, whereas
    verbal components were encoded strategically.
  • SPTs contained verbal and physical properties
    whereas VTs contained verbal component only
    (Backman et al. 1986)

50
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for action--theories
  • (Backman et al. 1989) proposed that the physical
    component of the dual code is encoded
    incidentally and retrieved implicitly, whereas
    verbal component is encoded intentionally and
    retrieved explicitly

51
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for action--theories
  • Engelkamp and Zimmer (1984, 1985 etc.) proposed
    that encoding SPTs is governed by separate motor,
    visual, and verbal programs that produce separate
    modality-specific representations
  • Motor encoding is more efficient than the other
    types of encoding and this results in the
    enactment effect

52
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for action--theories
  • Motor coding improves item-specific encoding,
    whereas visual and verbal processing result in
    relational encoding between the items
  • Two types of relational encoding 1. integration
    of actions within a list 2. integration of noun
    and verb in a command
  • Hypothesized that type 1 integration is
    independent of enactment, and type 2 integration
    is hindered by enactment

53
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for actiondata in support of Cohen
  • 1. no levels of processing effect
  • 2. no primacy effect
  • 3. no generation effect
  • 4. no rate of processing effect
  • 5. no difference in memory performance for
    children of different ages, for mentally retarded
    and controls or for elderly.
  • These results all support notion that enactment
    (in contrast to verbal encoding) does not require
    strategic processing at encoding as hypothesized
    by Cohen

54
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for actiondata in support of multimodal
    coding theory
  • These data are supported by studies that have
    used a divided attention as Ps encode SPTs at
    study (e.g., bounce the ball). At test memory for
    perceptual (color of object) and conceptual
    aspects (recall SPT) was tested (from Backman,
    Nilsson, Herlitz, Nyberg, Stigsdotter, 1991)
  • Results shown in next slide

55
Motor Cognition Memory
56
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Memory for actiondata in support of multimodal
    coding theory
  • This next experiment very similar to previous
    one
  • Ps encode SPTs at study (e.g., bounce the ball)
    under full and divided attention.
  • At test memory for perceptual (weight of object)
    and conceptual aspects (recall SPT) was tested
    (from Backman, Nilsson, Herlitz, Nyberg,
    Stigsdotter, 1991)
  • Results shown in next slide

57
Motor Cognition Memory
58
Motor Cognition Memory
  • Conclusions
  • recall of perceptual components is less strongly
    affected by DA than recall of the verbal
    instruction
  • Supports hypothesis that verbal component
    requires strategic processing and physical
    (perceptual) component is more automatic or
    non-strategic
  • Potential problem with experiment is that color
    recall is a form of cued recall, whereas verbal
    recall is a form of free recall. (This problem
    was addressed in Backman et. al. 1993).

59
Whats a tool?
a manipulable object that is used to transform
an actors motor output into a predictable
mechanical action for the purpose of attaining a
specific goal (Frey, 2007)
  • Simple vs. complex tools
  • Simple tools amplify the movement of the upper
    limbs (e.g., using a stick to extend reach)
  • Complex tools provide a mechanical advantage and
    convert hand movements into qualitatively
    different actions (e.g., using scissors to cut
    paper)

60
What do I need to know to use this tool?
colour of the tool
function of the tool
manner of grasping the tool
identity of the recipient
how the tool physically is used
colour of the recipient
learned motor skill
61
Memory Systems
Relies on frontal striatal network
Gradual acquisition of skills
Implicit retrieval
Resistant to interference and decay
Relies on medial temporal structures
Semantic (i.e., facts) and episodic (i.e., recollection of events) memory
Conscious retrieval
Sensitive to interference and decay
knowing how
knowing what
62
Memory for Tools
63
Overview of Roy Park (2010)
  • Investigated memory systems involved in the
    acquisition of different types of complex tool
    knowledge in a single study
  • Examined extent to which an amnesic individual
    could acquire knowledge and skills related to
    novel complex tools

Why study amnesia?
- Individuals with amnesia are impaired in
acquiring new declarative knowledge, but have
intact procedural learning
Ideal population to study dissociation between
declarative and procedural aspects of tool
knowledge!
64
Method
Participants
  • D.A.
  • - 58 year old man with 17 years of
    education
  • - Diagnosed with retrograde and
    anterograde amnesia after
  • contracting herpes encephalitis in 1993

Neuroanatomical Profile Cognitive Functioning
Damaged / Impaired Medial temporal lobe structures (bilaterally), right anterior temporal lobe Delayed memory
Spared / Unimpaired Dorsal frontal, superior and inferior parietal, and posterior cingulate regions Immediate memory, visual naming, fluency, digit span, and executive functioning
  • 6 healthy age and education-matched controls (3
    males, 3 females)

65
Method
Materials
  • 15 novel unimanual complex tools constructed
    using
  • KNEX
  • Tools were designed to act on a recipient (e.g.,
    plastic wheel) to perform a specific function
    (e.g., move wheel down a path)
  • Tool function, manner of grasping, or manner of
    use cannot be inferred based on physical
    appearance

66
Example of Novel Complex Tool
67
Procedure
  • Each session (S1, S2, S3) had 3 phases
  • 1) Pre-test
  • 2) Training
  • 3) Post-test

68
Procedure
1) Pre-test - Recall test (e.g. tool
function, tool colour) - Recognition
test - Grasp-to-command -
Use-to-command
2) Training Phase - 2 blocks (10 target
tools x 2) - Video demonstration
followed by practice - Limit of 90
seconds to complete one errorless trial
- Experimenter provided feedback
3) Post-test (same format as Pre-test)
69
Procedure
  • Task order remained the same across sessions
    except....

D.A.s S3 Post-test
  • Recall test
  • Recognition test
  • Grasp-to-command
  • Use-to-command
  • Use-to-command Recipient cued (RC) trial
  • Grasp-to-command
  • Use-to-command
  • Use-to-command Recipient cued (RC) trial

Changes made to bring D. A.s performance off
the floor
70
Hypotheses
1) D.A. would demonstrate unimpaired motor skill
acquisition associated with novel complex
tools (i.e., becoming faster in using the
tools across training trials)
2) D.A. would be impaired in his ability to
recall the properties (functional and
perceptual) of the novel tools
3) D.A. would be impaired on tasks that required
him to consciously demonstrate the
appropriate grasp and trained use of the
novel complex tools.
4) There would be no effect of the 3-week delay
on measures of procedural memory in either
D.A. and controls, but that there would be
an effect of the 3-week delay on measures
of declarative memory in the controls
71
Training
  • No differences between D.A. and controls in any
    training trial
  • Completion time decreased by approximately 3.4
    seconds per trial in controls and 6.3 seconds per
    trial in D.A.
  • No effect of the 3-week delay found in either
    D.A. or controls

72
Recall
(3 days) (3 weeks)
(3 days) (3 weeks)
  • For functional associative recall, D.A.s
    performance was worse than controls in all trials
    except in S3 Post
  • For perceptual recall, D.A.s performance was
    worse than controls in all trials except in S3
    Pre and S3 Post
  • Performance for controls is significantly worse
    after the 3-week delay for both categories

73
Grasp-to-command
(3 days) (3 weeks)
  • D.A.s grasp-to-command accuracy was worse than
    controls only in S3 Post
  • Grasp-to-command accuracy in controls was worse
    after the 3-week delay

74
Use-to-command
  • D.A.s completion time was worse than controls
    at S2 Post and S3 Pre, and his accuracy was worse
    than controls at all trials
  • D.A.s performance on both measures in the RC
    trial was better with the target tools than the
    lure tools
  • Controls were slower and less accurate after the
    3-week delay

75
Conclusion
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com