PSY 369: Psycholinguistics - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Description:

PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Acquisition III * * * Types of sentences used Past tense Plural Third person singular Present progressive Determiners ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:183
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 45
Provided by: PsychologyD223
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics


1
PSY 369 Psycholinguistics
  • Language Acquisition III

2
Brief outline
  • Continue describing the acquisition of language
    syntax and morphology
  • Some topics in the innateness (nativism vs.
    empiricist) debate
  • What kind of feedback (teaching) do kids get?
  • Is there a critical period for language?

3
Language explosion continues
  • The language explosion is not just the result of
    simple semantic development the child is not
    just adding more words to his/her vocabulary.
  • Child is mastering basic syntactic and
    morphological processes.

4
Language explosion continues
  • Syntax
  • Mean length of utterance (MLU) in morphemes
  • Take 100 utterances and count the number of
    morphemes per utterance

Daddy coming. Hi, car. Daddy car comed. Two car
outside. It getting dark. Allgone outside.
Bye-bye outside.
morphemes 3, 2, 4, 3, 4, 2, 2 -ing and -ed
separate morphemes allgone treated as a single
word
MLU morphemes/utterances 20/7 2.86
5
Language explosion continues
  • Syntax
  • Mean length of utterance (MLU) in morphemes

6
Language explosion continues
  • Proto-syntax (??)
  • Holophrases (around 1-1.5 years)
  • Single-word utterances may be used to express
    more than the meaning usually attributed to that
    single word by adults

dog might refer to the dog is drinking water
  • Typically idiosyncratic, but some
    conventional/common (e.g., indicate the existence
    of an object, request recurrence of object or
    event)
  • Often combined with intonation or gesture
  • Controversial claim May reflect a developing
    sense of syntax, but not yet knowing how to use
    it (e.g., see Bloom, 1973)

7
Language explosion continues
  • Syntax
  • Roger Brown (1973) proposed 5 stages
  • Stage 1 Telegraphic speech (MLU 1.75 around
    24 months)
  • Children begin to combine words into utterances
  • Limited to a small set of semantic relations
    (e.g., nomination, recurrence, attribution,
    possession see table 10.3 for examples)
  • Debate learning semantic relations or syntactic
    (position rules)
  • baby sleep agentaction or Noun Verb
  • Children in telegraphic speech stage are said to
    leave out the little words and inflections
  • e.g. Mummy shoe NOT Mummys shoe
  • Two cat NOT two cats

8
Language explosion continues
  • Syntax
  • Roger Brown (1973) proposed 5 stages
  • More than two words
  • Stages 2 through 5
  • Stage 2 (MLU 2.25)
  • begin to modulate meaning using word order
    (syntax)
  • Modulations for number, time, aspect
  • Gradual acquisition of grammatical morphemes
    (-ing, -s
  • Later stages reflect generally more complex use
    of syntax (e.g., questions, negatives)

9
How do kids learn the syntax?
  • Innateness accounts
  • Semantic bootstrapping
  • Learned accounts
  • Acquired from the linguistic input from the
    environment
  • It is in the stimulus

10
How do kids learn the syntax?
  • Innateness account
  • Pinker (1984, 1989)
  • Semantic bootstrapping

Child has innate knowledge of syntactic
categories and linking rules
Child learns the meanings of some content words
Child constructs some semantic representations
of simple sentences
Child makes guesses about syntactic structure
based on surface form and semantic meaning
11
How do kids learn the syntax?
  • It is in the stimulus accounts (e.g. Bates,
    1979)
  • Speech to children is not impoverished (Snow,
    1977)
  • Children learn grammar by mapping semantic roles
    (agent, action, patient) onto grammatical
    categories (subject, verb, object)
  • In all languages there are multiple potential
    cues indicating semantic/syntactic relations
    (e.g., word order, case marking)
  • Similar words occur in similar linguistic
    contexts
  • Acoustic information (e.g., prosody) may provide
    syntactic cues
  • Children do not need innate knowledge to learn
    grammar

12
Acquiring Morphology
  • Morphology
  • Typically things like inflections and
    prepositions start around MLU of 2.5 (usually in
    2 yr olds)
  • Remember the Wug experiment (Berko-Gleason, 1958)

13
Acquiring Morphology
  • Morphology

This person knows how to rick. She did the same
thing yesterday.
Yesterday she ________.
Typically children say that she ricked.
14
Acquiring Morphology
  • Morphology order of acquisition

Age (yrs) Morpheme Example(s)
2 Present progressive I driving
2 Articles A dog, the doctor
2 Plural Balls
2 Uncontractible Copula He is asleep, am, are
3 Third person singular He wants an apple
3 Full progressive Be ing, I am singing
3 Regular past tense She walked
15
Acquiring Morphology
  • Children sometimes make mistakes.

My teacher holded the baby rabbits.
Yes
She holded the baby rabbits.
No, she holded them loosely.
Did you say your teacher held the baby rabbit?
What did you say she did?
Did you say held them tightly?
16
Acquiring Morphology
  • Children sometimes make mistakes.

My teacher holded the baby rabbits.
  • This is ungrammatical in the adult language
  • Shows that children are not simply imitating
  • In this case, what they produce something that is
    not in their input.

17
Acquiring Morphology
  • Children sometimes make mistakes.

My teacher holded the baby rabbits.
  • Why do they make errors like these?
  • In the case at hand, we have what is called
    overregularization
  • The verb hold has an irregular past tense form,
    held
  • Because this form is used, the regular past
    tense-- that with -ed-- is not found (hold-ed)

18
Acquiring Morphology
  • The case of verb past tense
  • Regular verb forms require no stored knowledge of
    the past tense form (wug test)
  • Past tense is accomplished by applying a past
    tense rule (e.g., add -ed) to the verb stem
  • With irregular verbs something must be memorized
  • Examples
  • Horton heared a Who
  • I finded Renée
  • The alligator goed kerplunk

19
Acquiring Morphology
  • The case of verb past tense
  • Stages in the acquisition of irregular inflections
  • With regular verbs, the default form -ed is used
  • With irregulars, lists associating the verb with
    a particular form of the past tense have to be
    memorized
  • Past tense is -t when attached to leave, keep,
    etc.
  • Is -gt was
  • Dig -gt dug
  • Has -gt had

20
Acquiring Morphology
  • Stages in the acquisition of irregular inflections

time
  • On the face of it, learning these morphological
    quirks follows a peculiar pattern
  • Early correct irregular forms are used
  • Middle incorrect regular forms are used
  • Late correct forms are used again

21
Memory Rules
  • Why do we find this type of pattern?
  • Memory and rules
  • The use of overregularized forms starts at around
    the same that that the child is beginning to
    apply the default -ed rule successfully
  • Early All forms-- whether regular or
    irregular-- are memorized
  • Middle The regular rule is learned, and in some
    cases overapplied
  • Late Irregulars are used based on memory,
    regulars use the rule (the idea is that if the
    word can provide its own past tense from memory,
    then the past tense rule is blocked)

22
Memory Rules
  • Why do we find this type of pattern?
  • Memory and rules
  • Other accounts
  • Maratsos (2000) frequency explanation
  • It is possible to predict which verbs will be
    subject to overregularization
  • The more often an irregular form occurs in the
    input, the less likely the child is to use it as
    an overregularization
  • This is evidence that some part of
    overregularization occurs because of memory
    failures
  • Something about irregulars is unpredictable,
    hence has to be memorized

23
What kind of teaching do kids get?
  • If language is learned (and not innate), how do
    kids do it?
  • What kind of feedback do they get?
  • Claim Positive evidence is not sufficient for
    learning a language.

24
What kind of teaching do kids get?
  • Are the kids even aware of mistakes?
  • The children are apparently aware of the fact
    that their forms are strange
  • Parent Wheres Mommy?
  • Child Mommy goed to the store
  • Parent Mommy goed to the store?
  • Child NO! Daddy, I say it that way, not you

25
Positive and negative evidence
  • What kind of feedback is available for learning?
  • Positive evidence Kids hear grammatical
    sentences
  • Negative evidence information that a given
    sentence is ungrammatical
  • Kids are not told which sentences are
    ungrammatical(no negative evidence)
  • Lets consider no negative evidence further

26
What kind of teaching do kids get?
  • How much Positive Evidence is there?
  • Estimated 5000 7000 utterances a day
  • Between ¼ and 1/3 are questions
  • Over 20 are not full adult sentences
    (typically Noun or prepositional phrases)
  • Only about 15 have typical English SVO form
  • Roughly 45 of all maternal utterances began with
    one of 17 words (e.g., what, that, it,
    you)
  • Cameron-Faulkner, et al (2003)
  • So what kids do hear may be somewhat limited.

27
Negative evidence
  • Negative evidence could come in various
    conceivable forms.
  • The sentence Bill a cookie ate is not a sentence
    in English, Timmy. No sentence with SOV word
    order is.
  • Upon hearing Bill a cookie ate, an adult might
  • Not understand
  • Look pained
  • Rephrase the ungrammatical sentence grammatically

28
Kids resist instruction
  • McNeill (1966)
  • Child Nobody dont like me.
  • Adult No, say nobody likes me.
  • Child Nobody dont like me.
  • repeats eight times
  • Adult No, now listen carefully say nobody
    likes me.
  • Child Oh! Nobody dont likes me.

29
Kids resist instruction
  • Cazden (1972) (observation attributed to Jean
    Berko Gleason)
  • Child My teacher holded the baby rabbits and we
    patted them.
  • Adult Did you say your teacher held the baby
    rabbits?
  • Child Yes.
  • Adult What did you say she did?
  • Child She holded the baby rabbits and we patted
    them.
  • Adult Did you say she held them tightly?
  • Child No, she holded them loosely.
  • So there doesnt seem to be a lot of explicit
    negative evidence, and what there is the kids
    often resist

30
Negative evidence via feedback?
  • Do kids get implicit negative evidence?
  • Do adults understand grammatical sentences and
    not understand ungrammatical ones?
  • Do adults respond positively to grammatical
    sentences and negatively to ungrammatical ones?

31
Negative evidence via feedback?
  • Brown Hanlon (1970)
  • Case study of Adam - looked at things that were
    said to him by adults, and what he said to them
  • Adults understood 42 of the grammatical
    sentences.
  • Adults understood 47 of the ungrammatical ones.
  • Adults expressed approval after 45 of
    thegrammatical sentences.
  • Adults expressed approval after 45 of the
    ungrammatical sentences.
  • Suggests that there isnt a lot of good negative
    evidence.

32
In a way, its moot anyway
  • One of the striking things about child language
    is how few errors they actually make.
  • For negative feedback to work, the kids have to
    make the errors (so that it can get the negative
    response).
  • But they dont make enough relevant kinds of
    errors to determine the complex grammar.
  • Pinker, Marcus and others, conclude that much of
    this stuff must be innate.
  • But this isnt the only view. There is an
    ongoing debate about whether there are rules, or
    whether these patterns of behavior can be learned
    based on the language evidence that is available
    to the kids

33
Critical (sensitive) periods
34
Critical (sensitive) periods
  • Certain behavior is developed more quickly within
    a critical period than outside of it. This
    period is biologically determined.
  • Examples
  • Imprinting in ducks (Lorenz, Hess, 1973)
  • Ducklings will follow the first moving thing they
    see
  • Only happens if they see something moving within
    the first few hours (after 32 hours it wont
    happen) of hatching
  • Binocular cells in humans
  • Cells in visual system that respond only to input
    from both eyes.
  • If these cells dont get input from both eyes
    within first year of life, they dont develop

35
Critical (sensitive) periods
  • Certain behavior is developed more quickly within
    a critical period than outside of it. This
    period is biologically determined.
  • Some environmental input is necessary for normal
    development, but biology determines when the
    organism is responsive to that input.
  • That when is the critical period

36
Critical period for language
  • Lenneberg (1967) proposed that there is a
    critical period for human language
  • It assumes that language acquisition must occur
    before the end of the critical period
  • Estimates range from 5 years up to onset of
    puberty

37
Evidence for critical period for language
  • Feral Children
  • Children raised in the wild or with reduced
    exposure to human language
  • What is the effect of this lack of exposure on
    language acquisition?
  • Two classic cases
  • Victor, the Wild Boy of Aveyron
  • Genie

38
Victor, The Wild Boy of Aveyron
  • Found in 1800 near the outskirts of Aveyron,
    France
  • Estimated to be about 7-years-old
  • Considered by some to be the first documented
    case of autism
  • Neither spoke or responded to speech
  • Taken to and studied by Dr. Jean-Marc-Gaspard
    Itard, and educator of deaf-mute and retarded
    children
  • Never learned to speak and his receptive language
    ability was limited to a few simple commands.
  • Described by Itard as an almost normal boy who
    could not speak

39
Genie
  • Found in Arcadia, California in 1970, was not
    exposed to human language until age 13.5.
  • Raised in isolation a situation of extreme abuse
  • Genie could barely walk and could not talk when
    found
  • Dr. Susan Curtiss made great efforts to teach her
    language, and she did learn how to talk, but her
    grammar never fully developed.
  • Only capable of producing telegraphic utterances
    (e.g. Mike paint or Applesauce buy store)
  • Used few closed-class morphemes and function
    words
  • Speech sounded like that of a 2-year-old

40
Genie
  • By age of 17 (after 4 years of extensive
    training)
  • Vocabulary of a 5 year old
  • Poor syntax (telegraphic speech mostly)
  • Examples
  • Mama wash hair in sink
  • At school scratch face
  • I want Curtiss play piano
  • Like go ride yellow school bus
  • Father take piece wood. Hit. Cry.

41
What Do These Cases Tell Us?
  • Suggestive of the position that there is a
    critical period for first language learning
  • If child is not exposed to language during early
    childhood (prior to the age of 6 or 7), then the
    ability to learn syntax will be impaired while
    other abilities are less strongly affected
  • Not uncontroversial Victor and Genie and
    children like them were deprived in many ways
    other than not being exposed to language
  • Genie stopped talking after age 30 and was
    institutionalized shortly afterward (Rymer, 1993)

42
Effects of the Critical Period
  • Learning a language
  • Under c. 7 years perfect command of the language
    possible
  • Ages c. 8- c.15 Perfect command less possible
    progressively
  • Age 15- Imperfect command possible
  • In some special cases, we are given a window on
    the nature of the critical period

43
Effects of the Critical Period
  • Learning a new language
  • What if we already know one language, but want to
    learn another?

44
Effects of the Critical Period
  • Johnson and Newport (1989)
  • Native Chinese/Korean speakers moving to US
  • Task Listen to sentences and judge whether
    grammatically correct
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com