Week 1. Introduction - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Week 1. Introduction

Description:

Title: GRS LX 700 Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory Author: Paul Hagstrom Last modified by: Paul Hagstrom Created Date: 1/17/2001 3:53:12 PM – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:296
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 130
Provided by: paulha64
Learn more at: https://www.bu.edu
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Week 1. Introduction


1
GRS LX 700Language Acquisition andLinguistic
Theory
  • Week 1. Introduction

2
Language is really complicated
  • Frasier threw out Martins chair.
  • Frasier threw Martins chair out.
  • Daphne walked out the door.
  • Daphne walked the door out.
  • What did Roz say Niles bought?
  • What did Roz say that Niles bought?
  • Who did Roz say bought an espresso doppio?
  • Who did Roz say that bought an espresso doppio?

3
Language is really complicated
  1. His mother thinks Bill is a genius.
  2. He thinks Bill is a genius, too.
  3. Mary saw her.
  4. Mary saw her duck.
  5. I asked Mary to buy rum.
  6. What did you ask Mary to buy ?
  7. I saw the book about snakes on the table.
  8. What did you see the book about on the table?

4
Yet people know this stuff
  • Adult native speakers uniformly and
    overwhelmingly agree.
  • To know English is to have knowledge of (how to
    determine) which sentences are possible and which
    are impossible in English.
  • How one comes to have this knowledge is going to
    be our primary focus.

5
Consider learning this
  • Frasier threw out Martins chair.
  • Frasier threw Martins chair out.
  • Prepositions can go on either side of the object?

6
Consider learning this
  • Frasier threw out Martins chair.
  • Frasier threw Martins chair out.
  • Prepositions can go on either side of the object?
    Bzzt!
  • Daphne walked out the door.
  • Daphne walked the door out.

7
Consider learning this
  • What did Roz think Niles bought?
  • What did Roz think that Niles bought?
  • Ok, that is optional?

8
Consider learning this
  • What did Roz think Niles bought?
  • What did Roz think that Niles bought?
  • ? Ok, that is optional? Bzzt!
  • Who did Roz say bought an espresso doppio?
  • Who did Roz say that bought an espresso doppio?

9
Consider learning this
  • His mother thinks Bill is a genius.
  • He/his can be Bill even if he precedes Bill?
  • I asked Mary to buy rum.
  • What did you ask Mary to buy ?
  • To make a question, move the wh-word to the
    front, invert auxiliary. Right?
  • I saw the book about snakes on the table.

10
Consider learning this
  • His mother thinks Bill is a genius.
  • He/his can be Bill even if he precedes Bill?
    Bzzt!
  • He thinks John is a genius, too.
  • I asked Mary to buy rum.
  • What did you ask Mary to buy ?
  • To make a question, move the wh-word to the
    front, invert auxiliary. Right? Bzzt!
  • I saw the book about snakes on the table.
  • What did you see the book about on the table?

11
Grammar
  • People eventually end up with a system with which
    they can produce (and rate) sentences a
    grammar.
  • Even if a native speaker of English has never
    heard either of these sentences before, s/he
    knows which one is possible in English and which
    one isnt
  • Eight very adept sea lions played trombones.
  • Eight sea lions very adept trombones played.

12
How do people know this?
  • Every native speaker of English knows these
    things.
  • Nobody who speaks English as a first language was
    explicitly taught (growing up) You cant
    question a subject in a complement embedded with
    that or You cant use a proper name if its
    c-commanded by something coindexed with it.
  • Trying to use any simple kind of general learning
    principle based on (analogy to) the sentences you
    get seems almost sure to lead you astray.

13
Thats the setup
  • Language involves a complex grammar.
  • Adults end up with knowledge of this grammar,
    quite uniformly.
  • Children seem to go through advancing stages of
    language sophistication they are learning, the
    end result being the adult language system.
  • Next question What is the nature of the
    childrens learning?

14
Linguists, great and small
  • As linguists trying to figure out the grammatical
    system of a language, we
  • Look at which sentences are grammatical
  • Look at which sentences are ungrammatical
  • Compare them to describe generalizations about
    what the crucial factors are differentiating the
    grammatical from the ungrammatical.
  • Check the predictions of the hypothesized
    generalization by looking at more complex
    sentences.
  • Are kids just little linguists?

15
Kids are not just little linguists.
  • What did you see the book about on the table?
  • Who did Mary say that bought coffee?
  • Eight very adept sea lions played trombones.
  • Linguists theories built by considering both
    grammatical and ungrammatical sentences (often of
    a fairly complex type).
  • Kids Dont hear ungrammatical sentences, nor
    even all of the grammatical sentences (often of a
    simpler type).

16
So how do they do it?
  • One hypothesis holds suggests that parents
    actually help kids along (though not
    consciously).
  • Its well known that people seem to instinctively
    talk to little kids in kind of a weird way
    exaggerated intonation, simpler words, more
    repetition. Baby talk or as it is sometimes
    known, Motherese.
  • Many have entertained the idea that this simpler,
    more carefully articulated, speech might guide
    kids along the path of language acquisition.

17
Some properties of Motherese
  • Slower speech, longer pauses
  • Higher pitch, greater pitch range
  • Exaggerated intonation and stress
  • More varied loudness
  • Fewer disfluencies
  • More restricted vocabulary
  • More rephrasings
  • More repetitions
  • Shorter, less complex utterances
  • More imperatives and questions
  • Fewer complex (multiclause) sentences

18
Does Motherese drive acquisition?
  • Initially tempting, perhaps, but no.
  • If Motherese were crucial for acquisition, it
    must be available to all language acquirers,
    universally.
  • Several documented cultures dont even speak to
    the kids until they reach linguistic
    sophistication. (Of course, theyre exposed to
    language in the environment, but not directed at
    them in Motherese)

19
Does Motherese drive acquisition?
  • If you give a 4-month old the choice of whether
    to listen to Motherese or to normal
    adult-directed speech, the kid will choose to
    listen to Motherese
  • so it is quite likely that Motherese forms a
    significant part of the PLD for the kid, but it
    cant be necessary for successful language
    acquisition.

20
Simpler isnt really better
  • Linguists look to complex sentences to
    differentiate between predictions of different
    hypotheses about how the grammar works.
  • Generally, prior to considering complex
    sentences, the data underdetermines the grammar
    there are (at least) two systems compatible with
    the data observed so far.
  • If linguists need to look to complex sentences to
    figure out the intricacies of the rules (which
    all adult native speakers seem to end up with),
    kids should need this information too.

21
Positive and negative evidence
  • Kids need to know the grammatical system by the
    time they are adults.
  • Kids hear grammatical sentences(positive
    evidence)
  • Kids are not told which sentences are
    ungrammatical(no negative evidence)
  • Lets consider no negative evidence further

22
Negative evidence
  • Negative evidence (information that a given
    sentence is ungrammatical) could come in various
    conceivable forms.
  • The sentence Bill a cookie ate is not a sentence
    in English, Timmy. No sentence with SOV word
    order is.
  • Upon hearing Bill a cookie ate, an adult might
  • Offer negative reinforcement
  • Not understand
  • Look pained
  • Rephrase the ungrammatical sentence grammatically

23
Kids resist instruction
  • McNeill (1966)
  • Nobody dont like me.
  • No, say nobody likes me.
  • Nobody dont like me.
  • repeats eight times
  • No, now listen carefully say nobody likes me.
  • Oh! Nobody dont likes me.

24
Kids resist instruction
  • Braine (1971)
  • Want other one spoon, daddy.
  • You mean, you want the other spoon.
  • Yes, I want other one spoon, please Daddy.
  • Can you say the other spoon?
  • Otheronespoon
  • Say other
  • Other
  • Spoon
  • Spoon
  • Other spoon
  • Otherspoon. Now give me other one spoon?

25
Kids resist instruction
  • Cazden (1972) (observation attributed to Jean
    Berko Gleason)
  • My teacher holded the baby rabbits and we patted
    them.
  • Did you say your teacher held the baby rabbits?
  • Yes.
  • What did you say she did?
  • She holded the baby rabbits and we patted them.
  • Did you say she held them tightly?
  • No, she holded them loosely.

26
Negative evidence via feedback?
  • Do kids get implicit negative evidence?
  • Do adults understand grammatical sentences and
    not understand ungrammatical ones?
  • Do adults respond positively to grammatical
    sentences and negatively to ungrammatical ones?

27
Approval or comprehension?
  • Brown Hanlon (1970)
  • Adults understood 42 of the grammatical
    sentences.
  • Adults understood 47 of the ungrammatical ones.
  • Adults expressed approval after 45 of
    thegrammatical sentences.
  • Adults expressed approval after 45 of the
    ungrammatical sentences.
  • This doesnt bode well for comprehension or
    approval as a source of negative evidence for
    kids.

28
Kids experience differs
  • Parents respond differently
  • Eve Sarahs parents ask clarification questions
    after ill-formed wh-questions.
  • Adams parents ask clarification after
    well-formed wh-questionsand after past tense
    errors.
  • How can kids figure out what correlates with
    grammaticality in their situation?

29
Kids experience differs
  • Piedmont Carolinas Heath (1983)
  • Trackton adults do not see babies or young
    children as suitable partners for regular
    conversationUnless they wish to issue a
    warning, give a command, provide a
    recommendation, or engage the child in a teasing
    exchange, adults rarely address speech
    specifically to young children.

30
Feedback disappears
  • Adam and Sarah showed almost no reply
    contingencies after age 4
  • But they still made errors after age 4
  • And they still stopped making those errors as
    adults (learning didnt cease).

31
Three possible types of feedback
  • Complete consistent response, indicates
    unambiguously grammatical or ungrammatical.
  • Partial if there is a response, it indicates
    grammatical or ungrammatical
  • Noisy response given to both grammatical and
    ungrammatical sentences, but with
    different/detectible frequency.

32
Statistics (from Marcus 1993)
  • Suppose response R occurs 20 of the time for
    ungrammatical sentences, 12 of the time for
    grammatical sentences.
  • Kid gets response R to utterance U, theres a 63
    chance (20/32) that U is ungrammatical. Guess
    ungrammatical, but 38 chance of being wrong.
  • Kid doesnt get response R, 52 chance (88/168)
    its grammatical. Guess grammatical, but 48
    chance of being wrong.

33
Statistics (from Marcus 1993)
  • Suppose response R occurs 20 of the time for
    ungrammatical sentences, 12 of the time for
    grammatical sentences.
  • Suppose kid got response R to U, and is 63
    confident that U is ungrammaticalok, but nowhere
    near good enough to build a grammar.
  • This is a serious task, a kids going to want to
    be sure. Suppose kid is aiming for 99 confidence
    (adults make at most 1 speech errors of the
    relevant kindpretend this reflects 99
    confidence).

34
Lacking confidence
  • Based on R (20-12 differential), theyd have to
    repeat U 446 times (and compile feedback results)
    to reach a 99 confidence level.
  • Based on various studies on noisy feedback, a
    realistic range might be from 85 times (for a
    35-14 differential) to 679 times (for a
    11.3-6.3 differential).
  • This sounds rather unlike what actually happens.

35
In a way, its moot anyway
  • One of the striking things about child language
    is how few errors they actually make.
  • For negative feedback to work, the kids have to
    make the errors (so that it can get the negative
    response).
  • But they dont make enough relevant kinds of
    errors to determine the complex grammar.

36
Yes-no questions
  • The man is here.
  • Is the man here?
  • Hypothesis 1 Move the first is (or modal,
    auxiliary) to the front.
  • Hypothesis 2 Move the first is after the subject
    noun phrase to the front.
  • The man who is here is eating dinner.

37
Yes-no questions
  • The man who is here is eating dinner.
  • Is the man who here is eating dinner? (H1)
  • Is the man who is here eating dinner? (vH2)
  • No kids ever said (20) to mean (21), which would
    have been necessary to distinguish hypotheses 1
    and 2 Why not?
  • It seems that kids dont even entertain
    Hypothesis 1.
  • And thats fine, because it seems like Hypothesis
    1 is a kind of rule not found in any adult
    language.

38
Abstract principles
  • Principle C Nothing coreferential can c-command
    a proper name.
  • Hei believes Johnis teacher.
  • Hisi teacher believes Johni.
  • Study of adult grammar reveals that c-command is
    the appropriate abstract notion, defined on
    syntactic structures. But how do kids learn about
    c-command? You cant hear c-command.
  • Whats more, study of adult grammar reveals that
    Principle C holds in every language!

39
So, weve got
  • Kids dont make as many mistakes as would be
    needed for hypothesis testing.
  • Kids seem to receive no relevant negative
    evidence while learning language anyway.
  • Kids learn fast.
  • Kids become adults with all the grammatical
    knowledge pertaining thereto (uniform, highly
    complex)
  • Kids come to know abstract principles (like
    Principle C) without access to evidence
    determining them. In many cases, these principles
    are observed in all human languages. Poverty of
    the stimulus

40
Having language being human
  • A linguistic capacity is part of being human.
  • Like having two arms, ten fingers, a vision
    system, humans have a language faculty.
  • Specification of having arms instead of wings,
    etc., is somehow encoded genetically.
  • Structure of the language faculty is
    predetermined, like the structure of the vision
    system is.
  • The language faculty (tightly) constrains what
    kinds of languages a child can learn.
  • Universal Grammar (UG).

41
Universal Grammar
  • UG tightly constrains the learning process.
  • Study of syntax, phonology, etc., is generally
    trying to uncover properties of Language, to
    specify what kind of languages a child can learn,
    to see what kinds of restrictions UG places on
    language.
  • But kids dont just enter the world speaking like
    adultstheres development.
  • And, adults dont all end up speaking the same
    languagethere is learning.

42
Game Plan
  • The goal of the first part of the course is to
    discover what we can about UG, about this
    language acquisition device, through looking at
    first language development.
  • What do kids know and when? How could they come
    to know these things? What have been some of the
    major discoveries concerning development of
    syntax?

43
Coming upL1A
  • Formal learnability
  • Principles and parameters
  • How to set a parameter
  • Triggers, subset principle
  • Syntax at age 2
  • Root clause nonfinite verbs
  • Null subjects
  • Case errors
  • Theories of development
  • Weak and strong continuity
  • Experimental evidence
  • Optional Infinitives/ATOM

Binding theory How early is Principle
B? Passives How early are passives? Maturation Do
some innate grammatical principles
mature? Wh-questions Properties of development,
implications for syntactic theory
44
Coming upL2A
  • L1A ? L2A? How so?
  • Knowledge of language
  • Course of development
  • Parameter settings?
  • Critical period hypothesis
  • Windows of opportunity
  • Access and Transfer
  • What are the effects of the L1 on the L2ers
    knowledge?
  • What role does UG play in L2A?

Experimental results Course of development Sensiti
vity to universal constraints Effects of
instruction Other factors Major models Vainikka
Young-Scholten, Krashen, White, Flynn, Schwartz
Sprouse,
45
Learnability
  • The Principles Parameters model is designed to
    address the learnability problem children face
  • Languages are very complex.
  • Languages differ (something has to be learned).
  • Children get insufficient and variable evidence
    to deduce the uniform rules of grammar they end
    up with.
  • Children have adult-like grammars relatively
    quickly.

46
Principles and Parameters
  • The proposed solution to the apparent paradox is
    to suppose that to a large extent all human
    languages are the same. The grammatical systems
    obey the same principles in all human languages.

English
Japanese
UG
47
Principles and Parameters
  • Languages differ, but only in highly limited
    ways.
  • In the order between the verb and the object.
  • In whether the verb raises to tense

English
Japanese
UG
48
Principles and Parameters
  • This reduces the task for the child immenselyall
    that the kid needs to do is to determine from the
    input which setting each of the parameters needs
    to have for the language in his/her environment.

English
Japanese
UG
49
The standard picture
  • The way this is usually drawn schematically is
    like this. The Primary Linguistic Data (PLD)
    serves as input to a Language Acquisition Device
    (LAD), which makes use of this information to
    produce a grammar of the language being learned.

LAD
PLD
grammar
50
The standard picture
  • This isolates the innately specified language
    faculty into a single component in the picture.
    The LAD contains (a specification for) all of the
    principles and the parameters, and has a
    procedure for going from PLD to parameter
    settings.

LAD
PLD
grammar
51
Modeling human language capacity
  • We may be able to avoid confusion later, though,
    if we differentiate the innately provided system
    into its conceptual components.
  • This is my rendition of a way to think about UG,
    parameters, and LAD.

LAD
UG
PLD
Subjacency
Binding Theory
52
Modeling human language capacity
  • UG provides the parameters and contains the
    grammatical system (including the principles,
    like Subjacency, Binding Theory, etc.) that makes
    use of them.
  • LAD sets the parameters based on the PLD.
    Responsible for getting language to kids.

LAD
UG
PLD
Subjacency
Binding Theory
53
Modeling human language capacity
  • The idea behind this diagram is that UG is
    something like the shape of language knowledge.
  • Knowledge of language can only take a certain,
    innately pre-specified shape.
  • A system with this shape has certain
    properties, among them Binding Theory,
    Subjacency, the Principles.

LAD
UG
PLD
Subjacency
Binding Theory
54
Modeling human language capacity
  • The Parameters are different ways in which stored
    knowledge can conform to the shape of UG.
  • The LAD is a system which analyzes the PLD and
    sets the parameters.

LAD
UG
PLD
Subjacency
Binding Theory
55
Principles and Parameters
  • So two languages which differ with respect to one
    parameter setting might be represented kind of
    like this.
  • This is of course a cartoon view of things, but
    perhaps it might be useful later.

Language A
Language B
56
Principles and Parameters
  • So what are the Principles and Parameters?
  • Good question! and thats what theoretical
    linguistics is all about.
  • Since 1981, many principles and parameters have
    been proposed. As our understanding of language
    grows, new evidence comes to light, and previous
    proposals are discarded in favor of better
    motivated ones. Its hard to keep a current tally
    of the principles we know of because of the
    active nature of the field.

57
Principles and Parameters
  • Some of the (proposed) Parameters that have
    received a fair amount of press are
  • Bounding nodes for Subjacency
  • Binding domain for anaphors and pronouns
  • Verb-object order
  • Overt verb movement (V moves to tense)
  • Allowability of null subject (pro) in tensed
    clauses
  • Well look at each of them in due course

58
Verb-object order
  • The parameter for verb-object order (more
    generally, the head parameter setting out the
    order between X?-theoretic head and complement)
    comes out as
  • Japanese Head-final (X follows complement)
  • English Head-initial (X precedes complement).
  • Figuring out which type the target language is is
    often fairly straightforward. Kids can hear
    evidence for this quite easily. (Not trivial,
    thoughconsider German SOV-V2)

59
Principle A
  1. Sam believes that Harry overestimates
    himself
  2. Sam-wa Harry-ga zibun-o tunet-ta to
    it-taSam-top Harry-nom self-acc pinch-past-that
    say-pastSam said that Harry pinched him(self).

60
Principle A
  • Principle A. A reflexive pronoun must have a
    higher antecedent in its binding domain.
  • Parameter Binding Domain
  • Option (a) domain smallest clause containing
    the reflexive pronoun
  • Option (b) domain utterance containing the
    reflexive pronoun

61
But how can you set this parameter?
  • Every sentence a kid learning English hears is
    consistent with both values of the parameter!
  • If a kid learning English decided to opt for the
    utterance version of the domain parameter,
    nothing would ever tell the kid s/he had made a
    mistake.
  • S/he would end up with non-English intuitions.

62
But how can you set this parameter?
  • A kid learning Japanese can tell right away that
    their domain is the sentence, since theyll hear
    sentences where zibun refers to an antecedent
    outside the clause.

63
But how can you set this parameter?
  • The set of sentences allowed in English is a
    subset of the set of sentences allowed in
    Japanese. If you started assuming the English
    value, you could learn the Japanese value, but
    not vice-versa.Sentences allowed in Japanese
    (domain utterance)Sentences allowed in
    English (domain clause)

64
Subset principle/defaults
  • Leads to The acquisition device selects the most
    restrictive parametric value consistent with
    experience. (Subset principle)
  • That is, for the Principle A domain parameter,
    you (a LAD) start assuming youre learning
    English and switch to Japanese only if presented
    with evidence.

65
What it takes to set a parameter
J
E
  • Binding domain parameter
  • Option (a) Binding domain is clause.
  • Option (b) Binding domain is utterance.
  • English option a, Japanese option b.

66
What it takes to set a parameter
  • Binding domain parameter
  • Kids should start under the assumption that the
    parameter has the English setting.
  • If they hear only English sentences, they will
    stick with that setting.
  • If they hear Japanese sentences, they will have
    evidence to move to the Japanese setting.

J
E
67
What it takes to set a parameter
Very sensible. Now, lets consider another
parameter of variation across languages.
I
E
  • Null subject parameter
  • Option (a) Null subjects are permitted.
  • Option (b) Null subjects are not permitted.
  • Italian option a, English option b.

68
What it takes to set a parameter
  • The Subset principle says that kids should start
    with the English setting and learn Italian if the
    evidence appears.
  • But even English kids are well-known to drop
    subjects early on in acquisition. As if had the
    Italian setting for this parameter.

I
E
69
Moreover
  • English kids hear looks good and seems ok and
    stop that right now. Why dont they end up
    speaking Italian? If they mis-set the parameter,
    how could they ever recover?
  • Italian kids hear subjectless sentenceswhy dont
    they interpret them as imperatives or fragments
    (so as not to have to change the parameter from
    the default)?

70
Triggers
  • It seems like actual occurrence of null subjects
    isnt a very good clue as to whether a subject is
    a null subject language or not.
  • Are there better clues? If a strapping young LAD
    were trying to set the null subject parameter,
    what should it look for?

71
Triggers
  • Turns out Only true subject-drop languages allow
    null subjects in tensed embedded clauses.
  • John knows that must go. (English)
  • Juan sabe que debe ir. (Spanish)Juan knows
    that he must go.
  • Perhaps the LAD knows this and looks for
    exactly this evidence. Null subjects in embedded
    tensed clauses would be a trigger for the
    (positive setting of the) null subject parameter.

72
Triggers
  • A potential problem with the proposed
    subject-drop trigger is that it requires complex
    sentencesyou need to look at an embedded
    sentence to check for the trigger.
  • Such sentences might be too complicated for kids
    to process.
  • Degree-1 learnability Triggers need look no
    lower than 1 level of embedding.
  • Degree-0 learnability Triggers need look only at
    main clauses.

73
Triggers
  • Many who work on learnability haveadopted the
    hypothesis that triggersneed to be degree-0
    learnable.
  • Subjacency. wh a b t where a and b
    are bounding nodes.Bounding node parameter for
    IP
  • Option (a) IP is a bounding node (English).
  • Option (b) IP is not a bounding node (French,
    Italian).

IP and TP are often used inter-changeably
74
Triggers
  • Thus, a kid learning French couldnt choose
    option (b) by hearing this
  • Violà un liste de gens there is a list of
    peopleà qui on na pas encore trouvé quoi
    envoyer t t to whom one has not yet found
    what to send
  • since thats a degree-2 trigger. But

75
Triggers
  • Combien as- IP tu vu NP t de personnes?How-ma
    ny have you seen of peopleHow many people did
    you see?
  • If IP were a bounding node, this should be
    ungrammatical in French, so this can serve as
    (degree-0) evidence for option (b).

76
Triggers
  • Principles are part of UG
  • Parameters are defined by UG
  • Triggers for parameter settings are defined as
    part of the LAD.

77
Navigating grammar spaces
  • Regardless of the technical details, the idea is
    that in the space of possible grammars, there is
    a restricted set that correspond to possible
    human grammars.
  • Kids must in some sense navigate that space until
    they reach the grammar that theyre hearing in
    the input data.

78
Learnability
  • So how do they do it?
  • Where do they start?
  • What kind of evidence do they need?
  • How much evidence do they need?
  • Research on learnability in language acquisition
    has concentrated on these issues.

79
Are we there yet?
  • There are a lot of grammars to choose from, even
    if UG limits them to some finite number.
  • Kids have to try out many different grammars to
    see how well they fit what theyre hearing.
  • We dont want to require that kids remember
    everything theyve ever heard, and sit there and
    test their current grammar against the whole
    corpus of utterancesthat a lot to remember.

80
Are we there yet?
  • We also want the kid, when they get to the right
    grammar, to stay there.
  • Error-driven learning
  • Most theories of learnability rely on a kind of
    error-detection.
  • The kid hears something, its not generable by
    their grammar, so they have to switch their
    hypothesis, to move to a new grammar.

81
Plasticity
  • Yet, particularly as the navigation progresses,
    we want them to be zeroing in on the right
    grammar.
  • Finding an error doesnt mean that you (as a kid)
    should jump to some random other grammar in the
    space.
  • Generally, you want to move to a nearby grammar
    that improves your ability to generate the
    utterance you heardmove in baby steps.

82
Triggers
  • Gibson Wexler (1994) looked at learning word
    order in terms of three parameters (head, spec,
    V2).
  • Their triggering learning algorithm says if you
    hear something you cant produce, try switching
    one parameter and see if it helps. If so, thats
    your new grammar. Otherwise, stick with the old
    grammar and hope youll get a better example.

83
Local maxima
  • A problem they encountered is that there are
    certain places in the grammar space where you end
    up more than one switch away from a grammar that
    will produce what you hear.
  • This is locally as good as it getsnothing next
    to it in the grammar space is betteryet if you
    consider the whole grammar space, there is a
    better fit somewhere else, you just cant get
    there with baby steps.

84
Local maxima
  • This is a point where any move you make is worse,
    so a conservative algorithm will never get you to
    the best place. Something a working learning
    algorithm needs to avoid. (And kids, after all,
    make it).

85
Backing up a few steps
  • Of course, theres a long way to go between being
    plunked down in the world and evaluating whether
    youre hearing null subjects in embedded tensed
    clauses (or, conversely and degree-0 learnably,
    expletives like in its raining).
  • What are the words?
  • Which ones are verbs?

86
lvk\t?ætˆts?\sˆtgosain! ænd\mˆnivæn! si?\brˆd??
87
Learning language is hard.
  • Kids have to find the words. And the referents.
  • Extraction identify grammatical units.
  • Segmentation analyze identified units into
    component parts.
  • We will disregard these important points in order
    to proceed analyzing the development of syntax.

88
Do kids have syntactic categories?
  • Once theyve got the words, have the kids
    categorized them correctly?
  • Do kids categorize the linguistic world in terms
    of the same kinds of categories adults do? (e.g.,
    noun, verb, )
  • Evidence is hard to come by.

89
So, do kids have syntactic categories?
  • Theres not really any clear way to know at the
    earliest (one word) stages.
  • One view is that the null hypothesis (which we
    adopt, lacking evidence to the contrary) should
    be that kids do have adult-like syntactic
    categories.
  • Continuity. Kids end up being adults with adult
    syntactic categories if they initially
    categorize words differently, we need to explain
    how they change their categorization to the adult
    type.

90
MLU
  • Kids linguistic development is often measured in
    terms of Mean Length of Utterance (MLU).
  • Can be measured in various ways (words,
    morphemes)
  • Gives an idea of kids normal utterance length
  • Seems to correlate reasonably well with other
    qualitative changes in kid productions

91
2-year olds
  • Around 2 years old
  • Around MLU 1.75
  • Around 400 words in the vocabulary
  • 1-3 word utterances
  • Word order generally right
  • Grammatical words (the, is) generally missing

92
2 1/2 year olds
  • About 2 1/2 to 3 years
  • About MLU 2.25
  • About 900 words in the vocabulary
  • Some grammatical devices (past tense -ed, verbal
    -ing).
  • Over-regularization errors (He goed in the
    house), indicating theyve grasped the rule of
    past tense formation.
  • Single clause sentences

93
3 and 4 year olds
  • About 3 to 3 1/2 years, MLU about 2.75, about
    1200 words, beginning to use syntactic
    transformations (Is Daddy mad? Where is he
    going?)
  • About 3 1/2 to 4 years, MLU about 3.5, about 1500
    words, multi-clause sentences, still some
    over-regularization

94
4 and 5 year olds
  • 4-5 years, MLU around 4, about 1900 words, using
    more conjunctions and temporal terms (before,
    after), gain some metalinguistic awareness.
  • After 5, MLU stays about the same (no longer
    predictive), sentences get more complex,
    vocabulary increases (more slowly),
    over-regularization decreases

95
Do kids at the one-word stage have/know syntactic
structure?
  • Early attempt to answer the question.
  • Based on comprehensionkids clearly understand
    more than they can produce.
  • de Villiers de Villiers (1973), kids around MLU
    (mean length of utterance) 1 to 1.5 asked to act
    out the truck pushes the car, and got it right
    only about a third of the time.

96
Do kids at the one-word stage have/know syntactic
structure?
  • Hirsh-Pasek Golinkoff (1991), preferential
    looking task. Less burdensome task. Significant
    preference for correct screen (word order role).

Hey,Cookie Monster is tickling Big Bird.
?
?
97
How do we describe multi-word utterances?
  • Syntactically, in the same terms as the adult
    grammar? (continuity)
  • Or discontinuously? (For some reason, people seem
    to think this is simpler)
  • Thematic (agentaction, actiontheme, )
  • Pivot (P1 O, O P2, O O, O)
  • Limited scope formulas (hereX, wantX)

98
Syntactic approach
  • Continuity ?
  • VP VP V PP V PP sit sit P NP
    P NP on chair chair

99
Why 2 words?
  • Maybe they omit words they dont know?
  • Well, but they do omit words they know.
  • A kid whos used hurt before, documented as
    saying baby cheek to mean baby hurt cheek.
  • Pinker (1984) Processing bottleneck
  • A 2-word utterance filter
  • Kids grow out of this constraint.
  • Still, kind of mysterious. Whats easier?

100
Evidence for structure
  • Recall also the Hirsh-Pasek Golinkoff (1991),
    preferential looking task.
  • Structure plays a crucial role in figuring out
    which screen to look at.

Hey,shes kissing the keys.
?
?
101
The second green ball
  • Challenge to assumption that kids have structure?
    Matthei (1982) 39-63 get the second green
    ball.
  • When faced with thisDo they pick the second
    and green ball or the second green ball?
  • Kids did terriblyabout half the time wrong.

102
beware of the task
  • However, why chance? Why not always second and
    green?
  • This tends to suggest kids didnt really get
    the task. In fact, they made the same mistake
    with this array and pick the second ball.
  • So the problem is probably with ordinal numbers
    and manipulating subsets

103
beware of the task
  • Additionally, the kids could see the array the
    whole time, so kids may well have decided on
    which object to pick by the time they heard pick
    the second
  • Hamburger Crain (1984) re-did the experiment,
    hiding the array until the request was
    completekids error rate dropped to 14.

104
Intermediate moral
  • Its not easy to run a successful experimentyou
    have to be sure that what youre testing for
    isnt being obscured by other cognitive
    limitations.
  • Act out The truck pushes the car.
  • Pick the second green ball.

105
One-substitution
  • Anecdotal evidence
  • nice yellow pen, nice one (111)
  • Hamburger Crain (1984) Point to the first
    green ball. Ok. Now, point to the second one.
  • Note Failure wouldnt tell us anything here,
    since one could also legitimately mean ballbut
    if kids take one to mean green ball, thats
    evidence that kids do have the syntactic
    sophistication to replace N? with one.
  • Nevertheless, 42 / 50 kids interpreted it as
    green ball.

106
Some properties of kidspeak
  • Kids language differs from adult language in
    somewhat predictable ways. These can serve as
    clues to kids grammatical knowledge. Up to
    around 3 or so
  • Case errors for nouns
  • Some word order errors
  • Omitted subjects
  • Verbs not (always) fully inflected

107
Word order errors?
  • Languages vary with respect to word order
  • SVO English, French, Mandarin,
  • VSO Tagalog, Irish,
  • SOV Japanese, Korean, Turkish,
  • SOVV2 German,
  • Clahsen (1986) reports that German kids dont
    manage to put the verb in second position until
    the finite/nonfinite distinction is mastered.
  • But at that point the change was immediate
    Sentence-syntactic properties are stored
    separately from words category properties.

108
Word order errors?
  • Surprisingly few95 correct in English,
    DP-internal order (black the dog) may be at
    100.
  • Yet there are a number of things like Doggy sew.
  • It appears that in these cases, it is themeV
    without an expressed agent. When agent is
    expressed, themes are in their place.
  • Sounds like an unaccusative or a passiveperhaps
    they are treating the verb in these cases as
    unaccusatives? An attractive ideabut for the
    fact that young kids are bad at passives and
    unaccusatives.

109
Word order errors
  • Occasionally, postverbal subjects occurbut these
    seem to occur with likely unaccusatives with
    postverbal subjects on occasion going it, come
    car, fall pants. (cf. adult Mandarin , or
    Italian, which would allow that).
  • Alternative approach to Doggy sew might be
    topicalization Doggy, you sewif kids actually
    cant do passives and unaccusatives, then this
    might be the only explanation (short of pure
    performance error).

110
The Bennish optative
  • Anecdote about Ben, from Sadock (1982)
  • SVO normally, but in optative (wish)
    constructions, he uses a weird word order.
  • Intransitives (subject follows verb)
  • Fall down Daddy. Daddy should fall down
  • Eat Benny now. Let Benny eat now.
  • Sit down Maggie, Mommy.Maggie should sit down,
    Mommy.
  • Transitives (subject marked with for)
  • Pick up Benny for Daddy.Daddy should pick Ben
    up.
  • Read a story for Mommy.Mommy should read a
    story.

111
The Bennish optative
  • Hes marking transitive subjects with for, but
    leaving intransitive subjects and objects
    unmarked.
  • In the optative, Ben treats transitive subjects
    differently, and objects and intransitive
    subjects the same way.
  • This pattern is reflected in a type of adult
    language as well. Ergative languages mark
    subjects of transitives differently from both
    objects and intransitive subjects.
  • Accusative languages (like English) mark objects
    differently (I left, I bought cheese, Bill saw
    me).

112
The Bennish optative
  • Perhaps Bens language is ergative in the
    optative mood. (An option for adult languages,
    though clearly not in his parents language)
  • Further evidence
  • Ergative case marker is often homophonous with
    marker for possessive (cf. Inuktitut -up used for
    both), and Ben uses for (his ERG marker) in
    possessive constructions as well.
  • Thats a nose for Maggie Thats Maggies nose.

113
The Bennish optative
  • Further evidence
  • Ergative languages are almost invariably split
    often along semantic lines. Sadock takes the
    optative restriction to be of this type (cf.
    Georgian, nominative-accusative most of the time,
    except in the subjunctive and aorist, where it is
    ergative-absolutive)
  • Bens not really making word order errors,
    exactlyhe just thinks hes speaking Georgian.
    His errors come from among the options.

114
Pre-subject negation
  • Kids will say things like
  • No I see truck
  • Not Fraser read it
  • No lamb have a chair either.
  • Anaphoric no? No, I see the truck.
  • Often distinguishable from context, and they are
    not all anaphoric.

115
Pre-subject negation
  • Déprez Pierce 1993 looked at these, and
    proposed that not Fraser read it comes from a
    failure to raise the subject out of SpecVP to
    SpecIP. That is, here, Fraser is still in its
    VP-internal subject position.
  • Some believe this, some dont, but its a
    well-known analysis.

116
Case errors
  • English pronouns exhibit Case
  • Nom I, he, she, they
  • Acc me, him, her, them
  • Gen my, his, her, their
  • Kids seem to make errors until at least 2.
  • me got bean
  • her do that
  • me eye
  • In general, it is often overgeneralization of Acc.

117
Overuse of accusative
  • Default case Acc in adult English (Schütze 1997)
  • Me too.
  • What, me cheat?! Never!
  • Me, I like pizza.
  • Its me.
  • Who did this? Me.
  • So, overuse of accusative may well be just
    using a default form for nouns which dont have
    case.

118
Default Case
  • Russian (Babyonyshev 1993) Default case appears
    to be Nom.
  • Russian kids make basically no errors in subject
    case.
  • but they overuse Nom in other positions (e.g.,
    Nom instead of Acc on an object).

119
Default Case
  • German (Schütze 1995) Default case also appears
    to be Nom
  • Was? Ich dich betrügen? Nie!What? I cheat on
    you? Never!
  • Der, den habe ich gesehen.He, him I saw.
  • Object case errors are more common than subject
    case errors, and usually involve
    overgeneralization of Nom.

120
Determiners
  • Kids will also often leave out determiners.
  • Hayley draw boat.
  • Turn page.
  • Reading book.
  • Want duck.
  • Wayne in garden
  • Daddy want golf ball.

121
Subject drop
  • Even in languages which dont allow null
    subjects, kids will often leave subjects out.
  • No turn.
  • Ate meat.
  • Touch milk.
  • Dropping the subject is quite commondropping
    other things (e.g., object) is quite rare.

122
Subject vs. object drop
A E S
Subject 57 61 43
Object 8 7 15
123
Root infinitives
  • French
  • Pas manger la poupéenot eatinf the doll
  • Michel dormirMichel sleepinf
  • German
  • Zahne putzenteeth brushinf
  • Thorstn das habenThorsten that haveinf.
  • Dutch
  • Ik ook lezenI also readinf.
  • Another, fairly recently-noticed aspect of kid
    speech is that they will use infinitive verbs
    sometimes when adults would use finite verbs. In
    lots of languages.

124
Root infinitives
  • English kids do this too, it turns out, but this
    wasnt noticed for a long time.
  • It only write on the pad (Eve 20)
  • He bite me (Sarah 29)
  • Horse go (Adam 23)
  • It looks like whats happening is kids are
    leaving off the -s.
  • Taking the crosslinguistic facts into account, we
    now think those are nonfinite forms (i.e. to
    write, to bite, to go).

125
Root infinitives
  • However, children learning some languages seem to
    show very few root infinitives or none at all.
  • Italian, for example.
  • Often these languages with very few root
    infinitives
  • Allow null subjects
  • Have fairly complex agreement morphology

126
Pulling it all together
  • Kids sometimes use nonfinite verbs.
  • Kids sometimes leave out the subject.
  • Kids sometimes use the wrong Case on the subject
    (looks like a default Case).
  • Kids sometimes get the word order wrong
    (specifically, with respect to negation and for
    V2).
  • Kids generally leave out determiners.

127
Kid grammars
  • A major research industry arose trying to explain
    how these properties of child speech come about
    (and how they relate to each other) in terms of
    the grammatical and/or performance abilities of
    children.

128
?
  • ? ?
  • ?
  • ? ?
  • ? ?
  • ?
  • ?

129
References
  • Braine, M. D. S. (1971). On two types of models
    of the internalization of grammars. In D. Slobin
    (ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar A theoretical
    symposium. New York Academic Press.
  • Brown, R. and C. Hanlon (1970). Derivational
    complexity and order of acquisition in child
    speech. In J. R. Hayes (ed.), Cognition and the
    development of language. New York John Wiley
    Sons.
  • Heath, S. B. (1982). Questioning at home and at
    school A comparative study. In G. Spindler
    (ed.), Doing the ethnography of schooling
    Educational anthropology in action. New York
    Holt, Rinehart Winston.
  • Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words Language,
    life and work in communities and classrooms.
    Cambridge University Press.
  • Peters, A. (1983). The units of language
    acquisition. Cambridge University Press.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com