Title: Sentence-Level Effects on Alaskan Athabaskan Tone
1Sentence-Level Effects on Alaskan Athabaskan Tone
- Siri G. Tuttle, University of Alaska
- Symposium in Honor of Gösta Bruce
- University of Lund, Sweden
- January 2007
2Athabaskan Languages
- The Athabaskan family is the largest indigenous
language family in North America. It includes
languages spoken in Alaska, western Canada, on
the Oregon and California coasts, and in Arizona
and New Mexico.
3(No Transcript)
4(No Transcript)
5Another View
6Athabaskan tonogenesis
- While many of the languages have lexical tonal
systems from the same proto-Athabaskan source
(loss of syllable-final glottalic consonants
Leer 1979, Krauss 1978, 2005), these vary in
reflex some languages have high tone, some low,
and some no tone at all. This is true within
Alaska as within the whole language family.
7(No Transcript)
8Tone map in color
9Athabaskan languages in Alaska
10Tonal distribution in Alaska
11Typology of Athabaskan prosodies
- ? Tone languages may or may not have lexical
tone from proto-Athabaskan laryngealization - Tonal reflex some languages have high tone,
some low, from the same historical source - ? Saturation languages may or may not exhibit
tonal saturation many systems monovalent
12Non-tonal (Ahtna)
13Low-toned (Lower Tanana)
14High tone from historical source
15Low tone from historical source
16Saturated
- Navajo and Apache, with low tone from historic
glottalization, have developed lexical high and
mid tones on nearly all other prominent
syllables (those with light vowels fall in with
the low-toned), leaving few unmarked these
languages can be considered well-saturated. - Tanacross (high tone from historical source) is
also well-saturated, with both high and low tones
specified (Holton 2005).
17Saturated San Carlos Apache
18-Saturated
- In some of the eleven Alaskan Athabaskan
languages, the majority of syllables are
non-tonal, with only those reflecting
proto-Athabaskan glottalization being marked,
and the rest unmarked, to use terminology
employed by Krauss and Leer. These languages can
best be described as having sparse tonal systems.
19Unsaturated Minto, Lower Tanana
20High tone spread in Tanacross
21Minto spreading, with antitone
22More Minto Spreading
23No rightward spreading - Minto
24Sentence type question marker
25Sentence type Minto negative
26Edge effects final lowering
27Summary
- Athabaskan languages in Alaska may have high, low
or no tone from historic laryngealization. - Tone may be sparse or saturated.
- Tone sandhi is found in both high and low toned
languages - Intonation interacts with tone in all languages
surveyed. - Saturation may affect the relationship with
intonation.
28Masi ChwxThank you for listening
29Selected references
- Krauss, Michael E. (2005). Athabaskan Tone. in
Hargus, etc. pp. 55-136. - Krauss, Michael E. (1978, 2005) Athabaskan
Tone. In Hargus, Sharon and Keren Rice,
Editors, Athabaskan Prosody. Amsterdam John
Benjamins. Pp. 51-136. - Krauss, Michael E. (1982) Notes on Salcha
Athabaskan. Ms., Fairbanks Alaska Native
Language Center. - Krauss, Michael E. (1982) Report on field session
with Ellen Frank, March 17, 1996, and tentative
conclusions about Minto tone. Ms., Fairbanks
Alaska Native Language Center. - Jeff (1979) Proto-Athabaskan verb stem variation.
Part 1 phonology. Alaska Native Language Center
Research Papers No. 3. Fairbanks Alaska Native
Language Center. - Tuttle, Siri G. (1998a) Metrical and tonal
structures in Tanana Athabaskan. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Washington. - Tuttle, Siri G. (1998b) Acoustic realizations of
laryngeal contrasts in Minto Athabaskan
obstruents. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America 1033086 (Abstract). - Tuttle, Siri G. (2003) Archival Phonetics Stress
and Tone in Tanana Athabaskan. Anthropological
Linguistics 453, pp. 316-336. - Tuttle, Siri G. (2005) Coronal ejectives in Ahtna
Athabaskan. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America 117, 2489 (2005) - Tuttle, Siri G. and Ellen Kaisse (1996) Tone
loss in Minto (Lower Tanana) Athabaskan. CLS 32.