Title: Historical Linguistics: Questions of reconstruction and relatedness
1Historical Linguistics Questions of
reconstruction and relatedness
- Ian Roberts
- Downing College
- igr20_at_cam.ac.uk
2The Indo-European family tree
3The Indo-European Language Family
4(No Transcript)
5(More) Correspondences
- English mouse, father, three, fish
- German Maus, Vater, drei, Fisch
- Latin mus, pater, tres, piscis
- Kannada ili, appa, muru, minu
6The Comparative Method
- If a similarity between forms in two languages is
observed this can in principle be attributed to - Necessity (BUT linguistic signs are arbitrary,
and cf. Kannada etc etc) - Chance (this is always the most boring account of
anything, but cf. English dog, Mbambaram dog) - Borrowing (e.g. Japanese kompyutaa)
- A historical connection common origin
- So we can conclude that English and German are
quite closely related, and that Latin is more
distantly related to both, while Kannada is
unrelated to either.
7Grimms Law (or the First Germanic Consonant
Shift) (oder die erste Lautverschiebung)
8Verners Law (the second Germanic consonant
shift die zweite Lautverschiebung)
- PIE bhrater- p?ter-
- Gothic broþar fadar
- German Bruder Vater
- brother father
- Why the different medial consonants?
- Verner voiceless intervocalic stops become
voiced when the preceding vowel is unaccented. - Sanskrit, Greek show father originally had an
unstressed first syllable. - (cf. http//mr-verb.blogspot.com/2009/10/verners-l
aw-movie.html)
9The Neogrammarian Thesis
- Sound laws are exceptionless!! (Osthoff
Brugmann 1878) - Ausnahmslosigkeit!
- Hence phonological reconstruction can be relied
on. - But what about syntax?
10What we know about PIE I Phonology
Proto-Indo-European consonant segments Proto-Indo-European consonant segments Proto-Indo-European consonant segments Proto-Indo-European consonant segments Proto-Indo-European consonant segments Proto-Indo-European consonant segments Proto-Indo-European consonant segments Proto-Indo-European consonant segments
Labial Coronal Velar Velar Velar Laryngeal
Labial Coronal palatal plain labial Laryngeal
Nasal Nasal m n
Plosive voiceless p t ? k k?
Plosive voiced (b) d ? g g?
Plosive aspirated b? d? ?? g? g??
Fricative Fricative s h1, h2, h3
Liquid Liquid r, l
Semivowel Semivowel y w
11What we know about PIE IIMorphology
- Singular Dual Plural
- Anim Neut Anim Neut Anim. Neut.
- Nominative -s, -Ø -m, -Ø -h1(e) -ih1
-es -h2, -Ø - Accusative -m -m,-Ø -ih1 -ih1
-ns -h2, -Ø - Vocative -Ø -m, -Ø -h1(e) -ih1
-es -h2, -Ø - Genitive -(o)s -h1e
-om - Dative -(e)i -me
-mus - Instrumental -(e)h1 -b?ih1
-b?i - Ablative -(o)s -ios
-ios - Locative -i, -Ø -h1ou -su
- (from Beekes (1995) Comparative Indo-European
Linguistics An Introduction, John Benjamins).
12Syntax?
- Lightfoot (1998257) the kind of reanalyses
that occur in catastrophic change constitute
cutoff points to reconstruction. Proto-languages
are no more amenable to reconstruction than
proto-weather. - Harris Campbell (1995353) syntactic
reconstruction may be possible provided we can
solve the correspondence problem. In phonology,
this problem is straightforward yesterdays
segments correspond in some fairly systematic way
to todays (e.g. Gmc /f/ is the inherited reflex
of PIE /p/). But what was the Latin parent of
Létat, cest moi? - Watkins (1976306) the confirmation by Hittite
of virtually every assertion about Indo-European
word order patterns made by Berthold Delbrück ..
is .. as dramatic as the surfacing of the
laryngeals in that language.
13The IE clause
- Hales (1995) structure for the Vedic Sanskrit
clause - Topic CP C Focus IP
-
- A tendency for the verb to be second
- (cf. also Garrett (1990) on Anatolian, Kiparsky
(1995) on the prehistory of Germanic, Newton
(2006) on Celtic, and Fortson (2004) for a
summary)
14Older Germanic
- Fuß (2008) on Old High German
- Topic wh-phrase V IP
- Roberts (1996) on Old English
- Topic Focus (Verb) weak pronoun ..
15Latin
- a. Si bovem .. serpens momorderit.
- If cow-Acc.sg. snake-Nom.sg. has-bitten
- If a snake has bitten a cow
- (DS 116 Cato De Agri Cultura 102.1)
- b. in adulterio uxorem tuam si
prehendisses - in adultery wife-Acc. your-Acc if
you-have-caught - If you have caught your wife in adultery
- (DS 119 Cato Orat 222.1)
-
- probably represents a more archaic typology
- (Devine Stephens (2006) Latin Word Order OUP).
16PIE Syntax
- Null subjects (like Modern Italian, Greek)
- SOV word order (like Modern Indic)
- Wh-movement (like Modern English)
- Productive topicalisation/focalisation to the
left periphery (like Modern Slavonic) - Second-position effects (pronouns, adverbs,
verbs) (like Modern Germanic, South Slavonic)
17Nostratic Syntax
- Definitely head-final (Dolgopolsky, Bomhard
2008). - Uh
18A related question quantifying distances between
languages
- while languages differ from one another in all
aspects of their structure, some pairs of
languages differ from each other more than others
do Spanish and Portuguese are very similar to
each other indeed, English is quite similar to
German and Japanese is significantly unlike
almost all other languages. - many, if not all, of these degrees of structural
and lexical difference can be correlated to
historical relationships. The central activity of
historical linguists for two centuries has been
the establishment and organisation of these
relationships. - Recent developments are changing this picture
though
19Indo-European cladistics
- Ringe et al (2000) used techniques from
evolutionary biology to try to identify the
first-order subgrouping of Indo-European
languages. - More recent work on the same idea by Nakleh et al
(2005), Warnow et al (2005) and
http//www.cs.rice.edu/nakhleh/CPHL
20Character
- "an identifiable point of grammar or lexical
meaning which evolves formally over the course of
the language family's development, .. each state
of the character ought to represent an
identifiable unique historical stage of
development - a true homology shared trait
inherited from a common ancestor" (71).
21Examples of characters
- Ø a lexical character Eng hand (1), Ger Hand
(1), Fr main (2), It mano (2), Rus ruká (3). - Ø a phonological character sequence of changes
Grimm's Law, Verner's Law, initial-syllable
stress, merger of unstressed e with i except
before r. Absent 1, present 2 (singles out
Germanic). - Ø a morphological character most archaic
superlative suffix 1. -isto-, 2. -ismo-, 3 etc.
absent (2 singles out Italo-Celtic). -
22Database
- 24 languages representing all 10 IE
subgroups, and 322 characters (22 phonological,
15 morphological and the rest lexical). - Result of running tree-optimisation software
18 characters were incompatible with the best
tree, "in computational terms our result is a
total failure (86). - Italo-Celtic, Balto-Slavonic, the satem
group and Graeco-Armenian emerge as IE subgroups.
23 Possible Indo-European tree(Ringe, Warnow and
Taylor 2000)
24Controversies for Indo-European history
- Subgrouping Other than the 10 major subgroups,
what is likely to be true? In particular, what
about - Indo-Hittite
- Italo-Celtic,
- Greco-Armenian,
- Anatolian Tocharian,
- Satem Core?
25Our best PPN (Language, 2005)
26Modularised Global Parametrisation
- Developed by Chiara Gianollo, Christina Guardiano
and Giuseppe Longobardi, U of Trieste - Uses Universal Grammar syntactic parameters to
measure distances among languages
27Universal Grammar (UG)
- the set of grammatical principles which makes
human language possible (and defines a possible
human language) - determined by the human genome
- physically exists (in res extensa) in the brain
- otherwise known as the language faculty, the
language acquisition device (LAD), the initial
state of language acquisition and the language
bioprogram.
28Some assumptions
- a. There exists a rich, innate language faculty
(UG) which is a species characteristic. - b. There are no racial or cultural biases towards
particular languages or language types. - c. There is clear evidence that a sentence which
is well-formed in one language L may be
ill-formed in some other language L - i. John Mary hit.
- ii. John-ga Mary-o butta.
29UG contains
- invariant principles
- associated parameters of variation
- OV (e.g. Japanese, German) vs. VO (e.g. English,
Italian). - UG principles define V, O and how they go
together (VP) a parameter determines their
order.
30Parameters tell us what is variant (and by
implication what is invariant) in grammars, and
as such they -
- predict the dimensions of language
typology - predict aspects of language acquisition
- predict what can change in the
diachronic dimension. -
- A particular language L is an instantiation of
the initial state of the cognitive system of the
language faculty with options specified (Chomsky
(1995219)). -
31Things that can vary inside a simple nominal
expression (a DP)
- Ø is number marked?
- (English YES Japanese NO)
- Ø is there a system of articles?
- (English YES Japanese NO)
- Ø is there a system of classifiers?
- (English NO Japanese YES)
32The expression of possession
- Type A Possessor gt Possessee
- Johns sister
- John-no imooto-ga
- Type B Possessee gt Possessor
- la soeur de Jean (French)
- chwaer Siôn (Welsh)
33Method
- Parameters limited to the extended nominal phrase
(DP) - 27 languages from 4 families (IE, Semitic, Uralic
and Niger-Kordofanian) - 57 binary parameters
34TABLE A
35Optimisation by Kitch
36Optimisation by UPGMA57
37An approach to measuring relatedness which relies
on parametric syntax has certain advantages over
an approach based on lexical similarities
- discreteness the values of a parameter do not
form a continuum or cline of any kind - binarity a maximally simple range of
possibilities - finiteness the number of parameters is finite,
and in fact rather small, usually thought to be
more than 20 but less than 100 - no uncertainty of comparanda we are in principle
always sure when we are comparing like with
like (Guardiano Longobardi (20034))
38A possible synthesis
- treat parameter values as characters, thereby
adding syntax to the cladistic comparison. - The parametric grid can be taken to indicate the
syntactic characters.
39Backmutation (or homeoplasy)
- "either improbable or vanishly rare" (70), i.e.
"we simply do not find cases in which the
contrast between two elements A and B in a
structured system is eliminated from the
language, then .. reintroduced in precisely the
same distribution that it originally exhibited"
(ibid). Clearly true of phonemic split/merger,
loss/gain of inflection, changes in word-meaning,
etc. But is it true of parameters? - The case of French and the null-subject parameter.
40Homoplasy-free evolution
- When a character changes state, it changes to a
new state not in the tree - In other words, there is no homoplasy (character
reversal or parallel evolution) - First inferred for weird innovations in
phonological characters and morphological
characters in the 19th century.
0
0
1
0
0
0
0
1
1
41Two issues
- Parallel development (analogy rather than
homology) such cases must simply be set aside.
Alternatively one can take sets of changes rather
than individual changes as evidence for clades.
In any case, it seems doubtful that syntactic
change poses any problems not already encountered
in the area of phonology. - much less is known about the syntax of a number
of older IE languages compared to their
phonology, lexicon and morphology (see above).
42Conclusion
- Syntax has played a relatively minor role in
establishing relations among languages, but this
can change. - Parametric comparison can quantify grammatical
differences and thus play a major role in
developing our theories of typology, acquisition
and change. - Cladistic methods combined with parametric
comparison may shed light on major questions in
historical linguistics.
43THANK YOU!!