Minding Animals: companion animals and cognitive biophilia - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 13
About This Presentation
Title:

Minding Animals: companion animals and cognitive biophilia

Description:

... traditional cultures (e.g. totemism, from ototeman', the Obijwa for he is my relative'). Levi-Strauss emphasised universality of totemism as basic way in which ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:172
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 14
Provided by: liverpoolh
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Minding Animals: companion animals and cognitive biophilia


1
Minding Animals companion animals and cognitive
biophilia
  • Human-pet relationships rich source of
    information about how we define ourselves and
    others (boundaries that we draw)
  • Pet-keeping is a form of domestication but its
    structure and function is different. Many
    cultures have kept domestic animals but most have
    tended to have more functional and casual
    relationships with them Western habits of
    pet-keeping involve more intimate and intense
    relationships
  • Pet-keeping involves elevating certain members of
    a species to a privileged position in the human
    society/household this position does not
    necessarily extend to all members of that species

2
The function of pets
  • Function varies according to culture and time
  • Pets as resources (New Guineans use tame
    cassowary birds as sources of feathers)
  • Pets as teachers (keeping species considered as
    prey to learn about behaviour and improve hunting
    techniques)
  • Pets as status symbols (royalty keeping dangerous
    or exotic (expensive) animals)
  • Pets as children (recipients of parental care
    and nurturance)

3
The pet as marginal animal
  • Domesticated animals bridge the gap between
    wild and tame, nature and culture. Pets
    are no longer wild, but neither are they
    fully-fledged members of human society.
  • Animals may act as transitional objects to help
    humans bridge the gap between nature and culture,
    self and other (see Berman, 1990)
  • Domestication is a transactional event
    (Savishinsky, 1983) between animals and humans
    some traditional peoples talk of domesticated
    animals as having lost their spirituality by
    becoming tamed

4
The development of the human-pet relationship
  • Most human-pet relationships are primarily social
    and involve social species, e.g. dogs. Pet
    owners engage in four processes of identity
    construction when assigning identity to pets and
    incorporating them into the home (Sanders, 1993)
  • attributing thought, beliefs, intentions and
    desires to the pet
  • treating pet as an individual in its own right
    and having attributes like personality
  • seeing the pet as an active, reciprocal partner
    of the relationship
  • incorporating the pet into the family sphere
  • Does this process consist of anthropomorphism or
    does the relationship facilitate the development
    of reciprocity and mindedness?

5
Psychological relationships with animals
positive effects
  • Interactions with animals reduce stress and may
    help isolated, mentally disturbed and depressed
    people (e.g. Katcher Wilkins, 1992)
  • The presence of a pet can facilitate social
    interaction and make owners appear more
    attractive and more friendly
  • Empathy is a characteristic that seems to
    structure our relationships with both humans and
    animals there is a correlation between highly
    empathic responses towards people and towards
    pets (Paul, 2000)

6
Psychological relationships with animals
negative effects
  • Conflicting research highlighting the extent to
    which those who are closely attached to pets may
    not form close attachments to other humans (but
    see research on the elderly)
  • Shepard suggests pet-keeping is a form of mental
    hygiene, and that they fulfil the need we have
    to feel a connection with nature in an
    increasingly alienated world
  • The pet is our effort to extend a hand to the
    poor, broken brutes of the barnyard or
    agribusiness shed, to compensate for the
    vanishing bucolic community, a gray mass of
    egg-laying, meat-growing, milk-secreting
    machines, closed in the sterile barns of the new
    industrial farms. If we look at pet-keeping from
    this perspective of totemic thought a need for
    animals as urgent as good nutrition and good
    mothering pets appear quite different from our
    usual careless association.
  • Shepard, 1998, pp192

7
Misplaced relationships?
  • Are the limited relationships we have with
    animals exploitative (e.g. lab animals, zoos) or
    pathological (keeping pets)?
  • Our relationships with animals are ambiguous
    modern human-pet relationships are very close,
    but we distance ourselves from certain species in
    certain circumstances e.g. farmers and lab
    technicians adopt distancing strategies and
    special terminology to depersonalise them (e.g.
    animals are consumables to be harvested).

8
Cognitive biophilia
  • Humans seem biologically predisposed to affiliate
    with nature and form close connections with other
    species (the biophilia hypothesis, Wilson
    Kellert, 1993)
  • Creating a kinship with animals did more than
    illuminate the complexity of human social
    relationshipsit made the world a more
    comfortable place by reducing human isolation.
    The presence of animals, their touch, their
    attention, became part of the pattern of social
    dialogue that helps to maintain human health.
    (p.187)

9
Animals are good to think
  • Close interconnection with nature often more
    characteristic of traditional cultures (e.g.
    totemism, from ototeman, the Obijwa for he is
    my relative). Levi-Strauss emphasised
    universality of totemism as basic way in which we
    categorise the world and use the natural world to
    mirror their own culture
  • Deeper argument for the central role nature plays
    in structuring our cognitive worlds (Shepard,
    Wilson) relationships with animals are crucial
    to imagination, self-identity, symbolic thought
    and play (e.g. childhood naming of animals
    predates (and is the basis for) categorical
    thinking and helps to develop metaphorical
    thinking (which is the basis for all thought).
    Processes such as these can only happen in a
    context of relatedness with the natural world.

10
Real and symbolic relationships
  • In our imaginative life (e.g. play, dreams, fairy
    tales and myths) there are few boundaries between
    ourselves and animals. The dreams and stories of
    young children are predominated by the presence
    of animals (Lawrence, 1992), though adults dream
    less about animals (but they are still important
    symbolic figures in our dreams and fantasy life
    (Hillman, 1983).
  • Are our relationships with animals in our modern,
    urban, industrial age more symbolic than real?
    If the presence of animals is necessary for our
    emotional and psychological health (and important
    for the development of imagination,
    categorisation and language) what happens in
    their absence?
  • During nearly all the history of our species
    man has lived in association with large, often
    terrifying, but often exciting animals. Models
    of the survivors, toy elephants, giraffes and
    pandas, are an integral part of contemporary
    childhood. If all these animals become extinct,
    as is quite possible, are we sure that some
    irreparable harm to our psychological development
    would not be done?

11
References
  • Hillman, J. McLean, M. (1997) Dream Animals.
    Chronicle Books.
  • Hindley, M. P. (1999). Minding animals the role
    of animals in childrens mental development.
    (ed.) F.L. Dolins, Attitudes to animals.
    Cambridge University Press.
  • Katcher, A. Wilkins, G. (1993). Dialogue with
    animals Its nature and culture. In The Biophilia
    Hypothesis, (eds.) S.R. Kellert, E.O. Wilson,
    E.O. Washington, D.C. Island Press.
  • Lawrence, E.A. (1993). The sacred bee, the filthy
    pig, and the bat out of hell Animal symbolism as
    cognitive biophilia. Also In The Biophilia
    Hypothesis.
  • Paul, E.S. (2000). Love of pets and love of
    people. In Companion animals and us, (eds.) A.L.
    Podberscek, E.S. Paul, J.A. Serpell. Cambridge
    CUP
  • Passariello, P. (1999). Me and my totem
    cross-cultural attitudes towards animals. In
    (ed.) F.L. Dolins, Attitudes to animals. CUP.

12
References
  • Rajecki, D.W., Rasmussen, J.L., Sanders, C.R.,
    Modlin, S.J. Holder, A.M. (1999). Good dog
    Aspects of humans causal attributions for a
    companion animals social behavior. Society and
    Animals, 7, 1, 17-34. (Access this online, follow
    link to S A from Anthro webpage)
  • Sanders, C.R. (1993) Understanding dogs
    Caretakers attributions of mindedness in
    canine-human relationships. Journal of
    Contemporary Ethnography, 22, 2, 205-226.
  • Savishinsky, J.S. (1983) Pet ideas The
    domestication of animals, human behavior, and
    human emotions. In New Perspectives on our Lives
    with Companion Animals, (eds.) A.H. Katcher
    A.M. Beck. Philadelphia University of
    Pennsylvania Press.

13
Readings for next weeks session
  • Asquith, P.J. (1997) Why anthropomorphism is not
    metaphor, In (eds.) R.W. Mitchell, N.S. Thompson
    H.L. Miles, Anthropomorphism, Anecdotes, and
    Animals. NY State University of New York Press.
  • In AAA book, also try Ch.4 by E.C. Spada,
    Amorphism, Mechanomorphism and Anthropomorphism,
    and Ch 9 by H. Lehman, Anthropomorphism and
    scientific evidence for animal mental states.
  • Schilhab, T.S.S. (2002) Anthropomorphism and
    mental state attribution. Animal Behaviour, 63,
    1021-1026.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com