POLITENESS AND INDIRECTNESS - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 23
About This Presentation
Title:

POLITENESS AND INDIRECTNESS

Description:

e.g. imperative form without any redress: Wash your hands' ... 1. Mood derivable. 2. Explicit performative. 3. Hedged performative. 4. Locution derivable ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:1341
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 24
Provided by: ley1
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: POLITENESS AND INDIRECTNESS


1
POLITENESS AND INDIRECTNESS
  • Leyla Marti
  • Bogaziçi University

2
POLITENESS THEORY
  • Brown and Levinsons (1987) Politeness Theory
  • Face (Goffman 1955, 1967)
  • the public self image or reputation,
    self-esteem of a person
  • ...face is something that is emotionally
    invested, and that can be lost, maintained, or
    enhanced, and must be constantly attended to in
    interaction. In general, people cooperate (and
    assume each others cooperation) in maintaining
    face in interaction, such as cooperation being
    based on the mutual vulnerability of face.
  • (Brown
    and Levinson, 1987 61)

3
Negative face - Positive face
  • Brown and Levinson (1987 62)
  • Negative face is the want to interact without
    being impeded by others. It represents the desire
    for autonomy.
  • Positive face, on the other hand, is related to
    the want to be approved of by other people. It is
    associated with ones desire for approval.

4
Face-threatening Acts (FTAs)
  • Acts like promises, apologies, expressing thanks,
    even non-verbal acts such as stumbling, falling
    down, are considered to threaten primarily the
    speakers face, whereas warnings, criticisms,
    orders, requests, etc. are viewed to threaten
    primarily the hearers face.

5
Face-threatening Acts (FTAs)
  • Brown and Levinson argue that, since it is seen
    of mutual interest to save, maintain, or support
    each others face, FTAs are either avoided (if
    possible) or different strategies are employed to
    counteract or soften the FTAs.
  • These different strategies are presented in the
    form of five superstrategies for performing FTAs

6
Brown and Levinsons Superstrategies
  • 1. Bald-on record FTA performed bald-on-record,
    in a direct and concise way without redressive
    action.
  • e.g. imperative form without any redress Wash
    your hands
  • 2. Positive Politeness FTA performed with
    redressive action. Strategies oriented towards
    positive face of the hearer.
  • e.g. strategies seeking common ground or
    co-operation, such as in jokes or offers Wash
    your hands, honey
  • 3. Negative Politeness FTA performed with
    redressive action. Strategies oriented towards
    negative face of the hearer.
  • e.g. indirect formulation Would you mind
    washing your hands?
  • 4. Off-record FTA performed off-record.
    Strategies that might allow the act to have more
    than one interpretation.
  • e.g. off-record strategies, which consist of all
    types of hints, metaphors, tautologies, etc.
    Gardening makes your hands dirty.
  • 5. Avoidance FTA not performed.
    (Marti, 2000 25)

7
Superstrategies
  • Briefly summarised, positive politeness does not
    have to be oriented to the imposition, i.e. to
    the FTA, itself. Instead, the main strategy is to
    claim common ground, convey that the speaker and
    hearer are co-operators etc., i.e. attend to the
    hearers face wants. For example, the endearment
    term honey, in the utterance Wash your hands,
    honey, which is an in-group identity marker,
    counter-balances the imperative by claiming
    common ground and expressing affection. Telling
    jokes, promising etc. are other strategies for
    positive facework.
  • Negative politeness, on the other hand, focuses
    on minimising the imposition by attempting to
    soften it. This is usually achieved by
    indirectness, hedging, etc. For example, the same
    illocutionary act above could be softened by a
    more indirect formulation such as Would you mind
    washing your hands?
  • As Culpeper notes (1994 165) positive facework
    attempts to provide the pill with a sugar
    coating negative facework attempts to soften the
    blow.

8
Which superstrategy?
  • Exercise1 Identify the superstrategy.
  • I wonder if you know whether John went out.
  • Watch out!
  • A penny saved is a penny earned.
  • Lend us two quid then, wouldja mate?
  • Id like to borrow a cup of flour if I may.
  • Dont you want some dinner now?
  • Could you possibly by any chance lend me your car
    for just a few minutes?
  • That house needs a touch of paint.
  • Passengers will please refrain from flushing
    toilets on the train.
  • Accept my thanks
  • Help me with the bags will you, love?
  • OK.If I tackle those cookies now?
  • Can you open the window?
  • Give money

  • (taken or adapted from Brown and Levinson, 1987
    96-227)

9
Which superstrategy?
  • Answers to Exercise 1
  • I wonder if you know whether John went out.
    (negative politeness)
  • Watch out! (bald-on-record)
  • A penny saved is a penny earned. (off-record)
  • Lend us two quid then, wouldja mate? (positive
    politeness)
  • Id like to borrow a cup of flour if I may.
    (negative politeness)
  • Dont you want some dinner now? (positive
    politeness)
  • Could you possibly by any chance lend me your car
    for just a few minutes? (negative politeness)
  • That house needs a touch of paint. (off-record)
  • Passengers will please refrain from flushing
    toilets on the train. (negative politeness)
  • Accept my thanks (bald-on-record)
  • Help me with the bags will you, love? (positive
    politeness)
  • OK. If I tackle those cookies now? (positive
    politeness)
  • Can you open the window? (negative politeness)
  • Give money (bald-on-record)

  • (Brown and Levinson,
    1987 96-227)

10
Weightiness of a Face Threatening Act (FTA)
  • The superstrategies are employed according to
    the degree of face threat that a person might
    encounter or estimate for an act.
  • The assessment of the amount of face threat
    depends predominantly on the following variables
  • relative power of the speaker,
  • social distance (between the interlocutors), and
  • rank (degree of imposition).

11
Weightiness of a Face Threatening Act (FTA)
  • Wx D(S,H) P(H,S) Rx
  • According to Brown and Levinson, by adding these
    values, we should be able to calculate the weight
    of an FTA.
  • However, research shows that matters are more
    complicated (see, e.g., Craig et al., 1986
    Holtgraves and Yang, 1990, 1992 Wood and Kroger,
    1991 Bargiela-Chiappini and Harris, 1996 Yeung,
    1997).

12
INDIRECTNESS
  • The first superstrategy (bald-on-record) is
    ranked as the most direct strategy.
    Bald-on-record covers strategies usually using
    the imperative form without any redress, and is
    employed when face threat is minimal.
  • The last strategy (avoidance), at the other end
    of the continuum, is considered as the most
    indirect superstrategy and is employed when there
    is maximum face-threat.

13
Leechs Scales
  • Leech (1983) proposed scales of politeness (e.g.
    cost-benefit scale) in order to determine
    politeness. One of them is the indirectness
    scale. Leech (1983 108) claims that, when
    propositional content is kept constant, the use
    of more and more indirect illocutions will
    generally result in more politeness.
  • One reason for this is, according to him, the
    increase of optionality given to the hearer. The
    other reason is the more indirect an illocution
    is, the more diminished and tentative its force
    tends to be (Leech, 1983 108). The indirectness
    scale is illustrated below

14
(No Transcript)
15
Blum-Kulka and Olshtain (1984) scale
  • According to Blum-Kulka and Olshtain (1984),
    three major levels of directness for requests can
    be identified that seem to apply on theoretical
    grounds cross-linguistically
  • impositives,
  • conventionally indirect requests
  • nonconventionally indirect requests
  • A finer scale of nine directness categories
    based on the three major levels was used in the
    CCSARP. The starting point for the development of
    the

16
Blum-Kulka and Olshtain (1984) scale
  • 1. Clean up the kitchen.
  • 2. Im asking you to move your car.
  • 3. I would like to ask you to move your car.
  • 4. Youll have to move your car.
  • 5. I would like you to clean the kitchen.
  • 6. How about cleaning up?
  • 7. Could you clean up the mess in the kitchen?
  • 8. Youve left the kitchen in a right mess.
  • 9. We dont want any crowding (as a request to
    move the car).
  • 1. Mood derivable
  • 2. Explicit performative
  • 3. Hedged performative
  • 4. Locution derivable
  • 5. Want statement
  • 6. Suggestory formula
  • 7. Preparatory
  • 8. Strong hint
  • 9. Mild hint

17
More than off-record
  • Opting out
  • Situaton Lift
  • I would not ask for such a thing. I would
    wait even if the bus is in 2 hours. The offer has
    to come from the other side. If I want to go to a
    concert I would take care of everything
    beforehand. (Marti, 2006 1856)
  • Alternative responses
  • Situation Guest
  • Im sorry, Im invited at 7.30 for dinner,
    come, lets go together. (Marti, 2006 1858)

18
Complex Illocutionary Acts
  • Multivalent illocutionary act
  • Situation Guest
  • I would phone the people who have invited me
    and say the following loud enough so that the
    guestwould hear it and decide to leave
  • Hello, this is Çilem, a guest has (just)
    arrived, I might be a bit late. (Marti, 2006
    1861)
  • Multivalence is defined by Thomas as a single
    utterance performing two different illocutionary
    acts either for different receivers within the
    same discourse role ... or different receivers
    within different discourse roles (1985 17).
  • In other words, the host by (potentially)
    informing the person on the phone about the
    situation is indirectly requesting the guest to
    leave. This particular example makes it clear
    that the directness of a request is also affected
    by how the request orients to the participants.

19
Complex Illocutionary Acts
  • Bivalent/Plurivalent Act
  • Other forms of ambivalence are bivalence and
    plurivalence, where in a single utterance the
    speaker encodes two (bivalent) or more
    (plurivalent) unrelated forces.
  • Bivalent Act
  • For instance, backhanded compliments are
    bivalent acts
  • You look really good in those jeans now
    youve lost all that weight (Thomas, 1996 13).
  • two distinct illocutionary forces are present a
    compliment and an insult.

20
Complex Illocutionary Acts
  • The major difference of multivalent acts
    compared with bivalent or plurivalent acts is
    that the forces encoded in an utterance are
    intended for different receivers rather than just
    one receiver.
  • A multivalent act is referred to as the
    linguistic equivalent of killing two birds with
    one stone (Leech and Thomas, 1990 196).
  • Exercise 2
  • Jennifer is talking to a friend on the
    phone. There are people around her making noise.
    Jennifer loudly says
  • S Sorry theres a lot of noise at this end
    (adapted from Thomas, 1996 21).

21
  • Answer to Exercise 2
  • By uttering the above sentence, Jennifer is
    apologising to addressee 1 (friend on the phone),
    and at the same time reprimanding addressee(s) 2
    (people in the room).

22
REFERENCES
  • Bargiela-Chiappini, Francesca and Harris, Sandra
    J. (1996). Request and status in business
    correspondence. Journal of Pragmatics, 28,
    635-662.
  • Blum-Kulka, Shoshana and Olshtain, Elite (1984).
    Requests and apologies a cross-cultural study of
  • speech act realization patterns (CCSARP).
    Applied Linguistics, 5(3), 196-213.
  • Brown, Penelope and Levinson, Stephen (1978).
    Universals in language usage politeness
  • phenomena. In Goody, E. (Ed.), Questions and
    Politeness Strategies in Social
  • Interaction( pp. 56-311). Cambridge University
    Press Cambridge.
  • Brown, Penelope and Levinson, Stephen C. (1987).
    Politeness Some Universals in
  • Language Usage. Cambridge University Press
    Cambridge.
  • Craig, Robert T. Tracy, Karen and Spisak,
    Frances (1986). The discourse of requests
  • assessment of a politeness approach. Human
    Communication Research, 12(4), 437-468.
  • Culpeper, Jonathan (1996). Towards an anatomy of
    impoliteness. Journal of Pragmatics, 25,
  • 349-367.
  • Goffman, Ervin (1955). On face-work an analysis
    of ritual elements in social

23
  • Holtgraves, Thomas and Yang, Joong-Nam (1990).
    Politeness as universal cross-cultural
  • perceptions of request strategies and inference
    based on their use. Journal of
  • Personality and Social Psychology, 59(4),
    719-729.
  • Holtgraves, Thomas and Yang, Joong-Nam (1992).
    Interpersonal underpinnings of request
  • strategies general principles and differences
    due to culture and gender. Journal of
  • Personality and Social Psychology, 62(2),
    246-256.
  • Leech, Geoffrey (1983). Principles of
    Pragmatics. Longman  Essex.
  • Leech, Geoffrey and Thomas, Jenny (1990).
    Language, meaning and context pragmatics. In
  • N.E. Collinge (ed.). An Encyclopaedia of
    Language (pp. 173-206). London
  • Routledge.
  • Marti, Leyla (2000). (In)directness and
    politeness in Turkish requests With special
    reference
  • to Turkish-German bilingual returnees.
    Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, Lancaster University,
  • England.
  • Thomas, Jenny (1985). The language of power
    Towards a dynamic pragmatics. Journal
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com