Title: BetweenSubjects and WithinSubjects Experimental Design
1Between-Subjects and Within-Subjects Experimental
Design
- Types of Experimental Design, Error Variance,
Betwee-Ss, Within-Ss, Carryover Effects
2Experimental Designs
- Between-Ss Design
- Different groups of Ss randomly assigned to
levels of IV - Scores are averaged and analyzed between levels
- Within-Ss Design
- Single group of Ss is exposed to all levels of IV
- Scores are averaged and analyzed between levels
- Single Ss Design
- Single group of Ss is exposed to all levels of IV
- Scores are individually analyzed between levels
3Error Variance
- Statistical variability of scores caused by the
influence of VARs other than Ivs - subject-related variance or extraneous variables
4Distribution Curves
5Error Variance and Normal Curve
6As variance increases
7Distribution curves for 2 groups
- Two groups under two conditions with two
different averages for the DV
8Minimizing Error Variance
- Why is this necessary?
- Controlling for extraneous VARs as much as
possible. - Increase strength of IV
- get as much info about options to manipulate IV
before conducting study - Random assignment to different levels of IV
(i.e., different conditions of exp.) - Using more powerful statistical tools and/or
using more conservative criteria for statistical
significance.
9(No Transcript)
10(No Transcript)
11(No Transcript)
1247. Single-Factor Randomized Groups Designs
- Subjects are randomly assigned to treatment
groups. - Two groups (EXPERIMENTAL and CONTROL) are needed
to constitute an experiment. - The TWO-GROUP DESIGN is the simplest experiment
to conduct, but the amount of information yielded
may be limited. - Additional levels of the independent variable can
be added to form a MULTIGROUP DESIGN. - If different levels of the independent variable
represent quantitative differences, the design is
a PARAMETRIC DESIGN. - If different levels of the independent variable
represent qualitative differences, the design is
a NONPARAMETRIC DESIGN.
13Conducting a Two-Group Matched Groups Experiment
- Obtain a sample of subjects.
- Measure the subjects for a certain characteristic
(e.g., intelligence) that you feel may relate to
the dependent variable. - Match the subjects according to the
characteristic (e.g., pair subjects with similar
intelligence test scores) to form pairs of
similar subjects. - Randomly assign one subject from each pair of
subjects to the control group and the other to
the experimental group. - Carry out the experiment in the same manner as a
randomized group experiment.
14(No Transcript)
15Sources of Carryover
- Learning
- Learning a task in the first treatment may affect
performance in the second. - Fatigue
- Fatigue from earlier treatments may affect
performance in later treatments. - Habituation
- Repeated exposure to a stimulus may lead to
unresponsiveness to that stimulus. - Sensitization
- Exposure to a stimulus may make a subject respond
more strongly to another. - Contrast
- Subjects may compare treatments, which may affect
behavior. - Adaptation
- If a subject undergoes adaptation (e. g., dark
adaptation), then earlier results may differ from
later ones.
16Dealing With Carryover Effects
- Counterbalancing
- The various treatments are presented in a
different order for different subjects. May be
complete or partial. - The Latin Square Design for partial
counterbalancing - Used when you make the number of treatment orders
equal to the number of treatments. - Taking steps to minimize carryover
- Techniques such as pretraining, practice
sessions, or rest periods between treatments can
reduce some forms of carryover. - Make treatment order an independent variable
- Allows you to measure the size of carryover
effects, which can be taken into account in
future experiments.
17When to Use a Within-Subjects Design
- A within-subjects design may be best when
- Subject variables are correlated with the
dependent variable. - It is important to economize on participants or
subjects. - When you want to assess the effects of increasing
exposure on behavior.
18Within-Subjects Versus Matched Groups Design
- Both try to manage error variance associated with
correlation between subject variables(SV) and
DV. - If you think carry over effects are serious, use
matched groups design, even though it may be less
economical. - If correlation between SV and DV is weak, a
randomized groups design would be more powerful
than either a within-subjects design or a matched
groups design.
19Single factor vs. Multiple factor designs
- Single factor design (one IV)
- two level design simplest for within-Ss desgin
- multilevel design more than two levels of a
single IV - Ex Peterson and Peterson (1959) study on the
effect of retention interval (3, 6, 9, 12, 15, 18
seconds) on memory of three-consonant trigrams
(e.g., JHK) - Can be between Ss but more economical if
Within-Ss - If Within-Ss, requires counterbalancing.
- Is this a parametric or nonparamentric design?
- Multiple factor design or factorial design (more
than one IV) - Ex Peterson and Peterson (1959), adding one more
factor (e.g., the time trigrams are displayed 2
second 5 seconds) - This is a 6 X 2 factorial design.
20Factorial Designs
- Adding a second independent variable to a
single-factor design results in a FACTORIAL
DESIGN. - The MAIN EFFECT of each independent variable and
the INTERACTION between independent variables can
be evaluated. - Main effects are the separate effects of each
independent variable. They are analogous to
separate experiments involving those variables. - An interaction exists if the effect of one
independent variable changes over levels of a
second. - As factors are added, the complexity of the
experimental design increases, with increasing
numbers of possible main effects and interactions
(higher order factorial designs).
21(No Transcript)
22(No Transcript)
23Combining Experimental and Correlational design
- If you think a variable other than the IV is
correlated with the DV, you can measure that
variable to partial it out from the effects of
the IV on the DV. - The correlational variable is called a covariate.
- Ex In the Peterson Peterson study, you may
want to partial out intelligence (by measuring
this with an IQ test) to learn the impact varying
lengths of intervals between exposure and recall
have on the memory retention. - Intelligence in this case is a quasi-independent
variable. - A quasi-independent variable is a correlational
variable that resembles an independent variable.
24Quasi-Experimental Research
- Using Quasi-Independent Variable (e.g.,
demographic variables) - Cannot make causal inferences.
- Time Series Design
- Make several observations of behavior before and
after introducing your independent variable. - Interrupted Time Series Design
- Make several observations before and after some
naturally occurring event. - Equivalent Time Samples Design
- Repeatedly introduce the treatment condition,
alternated with periods of observation without
the treatment. - Ex studying operant conditioning with a variable
ratio schedule. - Nonequivalent Control Group Design
- Include a time series component and a control
group that is not exposed to the independent
variable.
25(No Transcript)
26(No Transcript)
27Developmental Studies
- Changes in behavior as a function of ones
chronological development. - Three ways to study development
- Cross-sectional design (Between Ss)
- generation effect is a concern
- Longitudanal design (Within Ss)
- cross-generation effect is a concern
- Cohort-Sequential design (Mixed)
- To account for generation effect and
cross-generation effect.
28(No Transcript)
29(No Transcript)
30(No Transcript)