Title: SelfEfficacy and Resource Allocation Planning: A Discontinuous Model
1Self-Efficacy and Resource Allocation Planning A
Discontinuous Model
- Jeffrey B. Vancouver
- Karen L. Scherbaum
- Ohio University
2Abstract
- A discontinuous segmented model of the
relationship between self-efficacy and resource
allocation was tested. The non-monotonic model
was largely supported based on 138 undergraduate
students participating in a computer game.
Discussion focuses on the implications regarding
the distinction of goal processes and the
interpretation of experimental research with
self-efficacy.
3Introduction
- Self-efficacy has been imbued with many positive
qualities with regard to performance (e.g.,
Bandura, 1977 Locke, 2001). - choice of goal level,
- persistence in challenging goal pursuit, and
- a direct positive effect on performance.
- Bandura (1977) has argued that self-efficacy is a
general mediator towards all performance hence,
the higher the self-efficacy, the higher the
performance, although with some caveats (Bandura,
1997). - Yet, early on, researchers complained that it was
not clear how self-efficacy had these effects
(see Rachman, 1978).
4 - We argue that the addition of the goal construct
to self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1997) helped
explain the self-efficacy effect substantially. - Specifically, self-efficacy positively influences
goal choice, which has a positive effect on
effort toward a task. - However, it also likely to negatively influence
effort allocation within a goal choice. - Combining these processes, we suggested that
resource allocation, which can be assessed in a
planning context, would vary as a function of
self-efficacy (see Figure 1). - For very low self-efficacy (i.e., point a),
individuals will not choose to accept the goal,
so they will put no resources toward it. - At point b, the individual has enough
self-efficacy to accept the goal, but anticipates
that substantial resources will need to be
applied to accomplish it. - Finally, at point c, the individual perceives
that the goal should be accepted and will be
relatively easy for him or her to achieve.
5Figure 1 Discontinuous Model
6Method
- Participants
- 138 undergraduate students (68 female, average
age was 19.4 years). - Task
- The Hurricane Game (see Figure 1).
- Participants were instructed to click on squares
that move rapidly and randomly about in a
specific area on a computer screen. The goal of
the game was to hit as many boards (i.e.,
squares) as possible in a three-minute period of
time (i.e., a trial).
7- Manipulations and Measures
- Self-Efficacy Manipulation
- Board sizes from 0 (the smallest) to 5 (the
largest). - Manipulation check participants were asked how
likely they felt they were to hit each board size
given each of the possible time limits (1 to 10
s) using a scale from 1 (not at all likely) to 5
(very likely). - Resource Allocation Measure (DV)
- The participants had 3 s to allocate between 0
(pass) and 10 s to try to hit the board for the
given board size. - Procedure
- Six, 10-sec. practice rounds for each of the 6
board sizes, progressing from largest size to the
smallest. - Two practice trials,
- 3 min. each
- Each trial consisted of rounds where board sizes
were randomly presented. - Self-efficacy manipulation check measure
- Four experimental trials (same as practice
trials) - Self-efficacy manipulation check measure
8Figure 2 Hurricane game screen shot.
9Results
- Reliabilities and Manipulations Checks
- Participants allocation decisions were stable
across trials (ICC2 .92 for Trials 1-4). - Board size was highly related to the
self-efficacy manipulation check (r .89, p lt
.001). - Assessing the Discontinuous Model (Figure 1)
- Figure 3 shows the frequency of seconds allocated
for each of the board sizes. - The probability that a board is rejected (i.e.,
skipped) was a function of the board size. - Correlation between the probability of endorsing
0 s and the board size was -.95 (i.e., the
smaller the board, the more likely the board was
skipped).
10- Correlation between the probabilities of
endorsing the highest amount of time available
(i.e., 10 s) was also highly negatively
correlated (r -.94). - Thus, the two boards that were perceived as most
difficult had bimodal distributions. - After removing the 10 s response, Figures 4 5
describe the dummy codes and results of the
regression models used to test the discontinuous
model. - Regressions were run for each participant
individually. The median results are presented.
11(No Transcript)
126
-.558 (-.317)
2.66 (.586)
Resources (seconds allocated)
Median R2 .601
.134 (.041)
0
Board size 0 1 2 3 4 5 a -1 1 0
0 0 0 b 0 0 1 1 1 1 c 0 0 -2 -1 1
2
Dummy codes
Figure 4 Median regression coefficients (and
betas) for Model 1 dummy codes.
136
1.474 (.321)
-1.210 (-.336)
Resources (seconds allocated)
1.632 (.413)
Median R2 .633
0
Board size 0 1 2 3 4 5 a -1 0 1
0 0 0 b 0 0 0 1 1 1 c 0 0 0 -1 0
1
Dummy codes
Figure 5 Median regression coefficients (and
betas) for Model 2 dummy codes.
14Discussion
- The findings confirmed the hypothesis that the
nature of the relationship between expectancies
and resource allocation is non-monotonic in a
planning context. - The discontinuous model reflects the key role
goal acceptance is believed to play in motivated
behavior (Austin Vancouver, 1996). - Beliefs about the ability to realize the goal,
which in this case was clicking on the rapidly
moving board, were expected to largely determine
the likelihood that the individual would try for
the goal. Indeed, it accounted for 90 of the
variance in this choice.
15- On the other hand, we expected that if the person
chooses to pursue the goal, beliefs about the
difficulty of reaching the goal would largely
determine the resources applied. - When just considering the probability of putting
the maximum amount of resources (10 s) to the
goal, difficulty accounted for 88 of the
variance, with greater difficulty predicting
probability of maximum resource allocation.
16References
- Austin, J. T., Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal
constructs in psychology Structure, process, and
content. Psychological Bulletin,120, 338-375. - Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy Toward a
unifying theory of behavioral change.
Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. - Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy The exercise
of control. New YorkFreeman. - Locke, E. A. (2001). Self-set goals and
self-efficacy as mediators of incentives and
personality. In M. Erez, U. Kleinbeck, H.
Thierry (Eds.), Work motivation in the context of
a globalizing economy (pp. 13-26). Mahwah, NJ
Lawrence Erlbaum. - Powers, W.T. (1991). Commentary on Banduras
Human Agency. American Psychologist, 46,
151-153. - Rachman, S. (Ed.) (1978). Special issue of
perceived self-efficacy. Advances in Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1, 137-269.