Title: There
1Theres more to emotion than meets the eye
Processing of emotional prosody in the auditory
domain Lauren Cornew,1 Tracy Love,1,2 Georgina
Batten,1 and Leslie Carver1 1University of
California, San Diego, 2San Diego State University
General Discussion
Experiment 1
Experiment 2
Background
Methods
Questions
- Contrary to predictions, adults seemed to
demonstrate a processing advantage for neutral
prosody, which was identified more accurately and
more rapidly than happy or angry prosody. - Early school-aged children seem to show a
pattern similar to that of adults for speed of
processing, but not accuracy. - Relationship between age and accuracy suggests
developmental progression in recognition of
emotional prosody. - It is unclear whether the neutral bias
reflects perception, attention,
decision/response, or a language processing or
acoustic parameter. - We are currently using ERPs to further examine
the time course of processing and conducting
acoustic analyses to pinpoint acoustic parameters
which might contribute to the observed effects.
- Participants randomly assigned to 1 of 3 groups
- What is the developmental progression associated
with the processing of emotional prosody? Could
it provide insight into the patterns seen in
Experiment 1?
- Perceiving emotional expressions is essential
for social interactions and enables people to
recognize (and hopefully avoid) danger. - Emotion interacts with cognition at many levels
of processing, from basic perceptual1 and
attentional2 stages to higher cognitive functions
such as decision-making3 and categorization.4 - Studies using visual emotional stimuli have
demonstrated enhanced processing of negative
content.5, 6 - Lesion and neuroimaging studies have highlighted
the amygdalas role in visual emotion
recognition,7, 8 and results from studies of
auditory emotion processing suggest that there
may be significant overlap in the brain areas
recruited across modalities.9, 10 However, a
potential processing advantage for auditory
emotional stimuli remains under-explored.
Participants
- Every clip judged to be happy, angry, or neutral
- Variables of interest
- Percent correct
- Isolation point (length of the clip at which
participants chose the correct emotion and did
not subsequently change their decision)
- N 16 (8 female) children, ages 5-7 (mean
6.4, SD .7, range 5.37.7) - All monolingual native English speakers, free
from developmental disorders
Stimuli
- Same as in Experiment 1, but a subset of the
original 48 sentences (the 24 with highest
accuracy in Exp. 1) chosen to maximize correct
emotion identification in children - As in Experiment 1, sentences edited into
successive clips, with duration increasing in
increments of 250ms
Results
- Greater accuracy for neutral prosody, F(2, 34)
7.87, p .001
Methods
- Participants randomly assigned to 1 of 3 groups
- Task presented as a game deciphering aliens
feelings based on their tone of voice - Every clip judged to be happy, angry, or neutral
- Children pressed a happy, neutral, or angry face
on a touch screen (Figure 4).
Questions
- Is there a processing advantage or bias for
negative prosody? - Alternatively, is there an advantage for
emotional (whether positive or negative) compared
to non-emotional prosody?
References
- Faster correct identification of neutral
prosody, F(2, 34) 24.67, p .000
1 Phelps et al (2006). Psych. Science, 17,
292-299. 2 Carretié et al (2003). Psychophys.,
40, 381-288. 3 Bechara, Damasio, Damasio
(2003). Ann. NY Academy Sciences, 985, 356-369. 4
Ito et al. (1998). J Personality and Social
Psych., 75, 887-900. 5 Dijksterhuis Aarts
(2003). Psych. Science, 14, 14-18. 6 Ohman,
Lundqvist, Esteves (2001). J Personality and
Social Psych., 80, 381-396. 7 Adolphs et al.
(1999). Neuropsychologia, 37, 1111-1117. 8
Vuilleumier et al. (2004). Nat. Neurosci., 7,
1271-1278. 9 Scott et al. (1997). Nature, 385,
254-257. 10 Morris et al.(1999).
Neuropsychologia, 37, 1155- 1163. 11 Grosjean,
F. (1980). Perception Psychophys., 28, 267-283.
Preliminary Results
Participants
- Age correlated with overall accuracy and
accuracy for recognition of neutral (but not
happy or angry) prosody
- N 43 (27 female) UCSD undergraduates (mean age
21, SD 2.9, range 18-31) - All monolingual native English speakers
Stimuli
- 48 Jabberwocky sentences spoken by an actress
with happy, angry, and neutral prosody - Length ranged from 1.6s to 4.4s (mean 2.7)
- Gating paradigm11 Sentences edited into
successive clips, with duration increasing in
increments of 250ms 5s of silence in between
(Figure 1)
- No emotion effect for accuracy (Figure 5), but
faster correct identification of neutral prosody,
F(2.14) 3.28, p .05 (Figure 6).
- Sentences incorrectly labeled as neutral more
frequently than either happy or angry, F(2, 34)
36.13, p .000 - No tendency to label sentences as neutral by
default Incorrect selection of neutral after
hearing the first clip occurred at chance level,
t(40) -.036, p .97 (2-tailed) - Pattern of errors suggests a trend toward a
negative bias (labeling neutral as angry or happy
as neutral), t(40) 1.65, p .06
Acknowledgements
- This research was supported in part by an NSF
Graduate Research Fellowship to the first author
and NIH grants (DC00494 and DC03885) to the
second author. - Special thanks to Sandra Ahumada,Jessica
Belisle, Mara Bromberg, Chris Lonner, Mark
Martin, Maxwell Moholy, Jenn Navala, and to the
families who participated in Experiment 2!
Figure 1. Schematic of a spliced sentence