Title: Analysis of mediation and moderation using instrumental variables
1Methods of explanatory analysis for psychological
treatment trials workshop
- Session 3
- Analysis of mediation and moderation using
instrumental variables - Richard Emsley
Funded by MRC Methodology Grant G0600555 MHRN
Methodology Research Group
2Plan for session 3
- Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
talk. - Why do we use instrumental variables?
- Where do we find instrumental variables?
- Examples
- PROSPECT mediator example
- SoCRATES SAS model.
- Designing trials with instruments in mind.
3Quick review of IVs from Ians talk
- Ian has demonstrated how we can use instrumental
variable methods to infer a causal effect of
treatment in the presence of departures from
randomised intervention. - This utilises randomisation as the instrumental
variable. As we will see, randomisation meets
the assumptions required for an IV - But we will also need to consider the situation
where we cannot use randomisation as an
instrument
4Instrumental Variables (IVs)
- In a standard regression model, if an explanatory
variable is correlated with the error term (known
as endogeneity) its coefficient cannot be
unbiasedly estimated. - An instrumental variable (IV) is a variable that
does not appear in the model, is uncorrelated
with the error term and is correlated with the
endogenous explanatory variable randomisation,
where available, often satisfies this criteria. - A two stage least squares (2SLS) procedure can
then be applied to estimate the coefficient. At
its simplest, the first stage involves using a
simple linear regression of the endogenous
variable on the instrument and saving the
predicted values. In the second stage the
outcome is then regressed on the predicted
values, with the latter regression coefficient
being the required estimate of the coefficient.
5Some notation
- Ri treatment group the outcome of
randomisation (Ri1 for treatment, 0 for
controls). - Xi' X1i, X2i Xpi baseline covariates.
- Yi observed outcome.
- Di actual treatment received. This is an
intermediate outcome that is a putative mediator
of the effects of treatment on outcome (either a
quantitative measure or binary).
6Instrumental variables (IV) (from session 1)
- Popular in econometrics
- Simplest idea is
- Outcome Yi a b Di ei
- Treatment Di g d Ri fi
- Allow error ei to be correlated with Di but
assume its independent of Ri - randomisation Ri only affects outcome through its
effect on compliance Di - Estimation by two-stage least squares
- EYi Ri a b EDi Ri
- so first regress Di on Ri to get EDi Ri
- then regress Yi on EDi Ri
- NB standard errors not quite correct by this
method general IV uses different standard errors
7Simple Mediation Idea (from session 2)
dX
Mediator
ß
a
Treatment
Outcomes
dY
?
The total effect is the sum of the direct effect
(?) and the indirect effect (aß)
8Confounded Mediation Diagram
U the unmeasured confounders
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Treatment
Outcomes
dY
?
If treatment is randomised then assumption of no
confounding of treatment and other variables
(outcomes) is justified.
9Confounded Mediation Diagram
dX
U
U
Mediator
ß
a
Treatment
Outcomes
dY
?
U
If treatment is not randomised then there is
likely to be even more unmeasured confounding.
10Confounded Mediation Diagram
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
Thankfully were talking about randomised trials!
11Linking the two previous sessions Compliance as
a mediator
dX
Treatment Received
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
12Linking the two previous sessions Randomisation
as an IV
dX
Treatment Received
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
By assuming the absence of a direct path from
randomisation to outcome, we assume the entire
effect of randomisation acts through receipt of
treatment. ? randomisation is an instrumental
variable.
13Plan for session 3
- Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
talk. - Why do we use instrumental variables?
- Where do we find instrumental variables?
- Examples
- PROSPECT mediator example
- SoCRATES SAS model.
- Designing trials with instruments in mind.
14Why do we use instrumental variables?
- All available statistical methods we usually use
(for any standard analysis), including - Stratification
- Regression
- Matching
- Standardization
- require the one unverifiable condition we
identified previously - NO UNMEASURED CONFOUNDING
15Why do we use instrumental variables?
- Unlike all other methods, IV methods can be used
to consistently estimate causal effects in the
presence of unmeasured confounding AND
measurement error. - SO WE CAN SOLVE THE PROBLEM OF
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
16Definition of an instrumental variable
- A variable is an instrumental variable Z if
- Z has a causal effect on the mediator D
- This can be tested in the data.
- ii. Z affects the outcome Y only through D
- i.e. there is no direct effect of Z on Y
- This is an assumption (sometimes a strong
assumption). - iii. Z does not share common causes with the
outcome Y - i.e. there is no confounding for the effect of Z
on Y. - This is another assumption which randomisation
satisfies but other IVs may not.
17Assumptions for instrumental variables
- IV methods require FOUR assumptions
- The first 3 assumptions are from the definition
- The association between instrument and mediator.
- no direct effect of the instrument on outcome.
- no unmeasured confounding for the instrument and
outcome. - There are a wide variety of fourth assumptions
and different assumptions result in the
estimation of different causal effects - E.g. no interactions, monotonicity (no defiers).
18Testing assumptions
- There are a number of tests we can use for some
of these assumptions. - Stata has three postestimation commands following
ivregress - estat overid
- estat endogenous
- estat firststage
- This final option is perhaps the most useful. It
gives an indication of whether the set of
instruments strongly predict the mediator see
PROSPECT example later on.
19Advantages of IVs
- Can allow for unmeasured confounding
- Can allow for measurement error
- Randomisation meets the definition so is an ideal
instrument - When available.
- Obviously not in observational studies.
20Disadvantages of IVs
- 1. It is impossible to verify that Z is an
instrument and using a non instrument introduces
additional bias. - 2. A weak instrument Z increases the bias over
that of ordinary regression. - 3. Instruments by themselves are actually
insufficient to estimate causal effects and we
require additional unverifiable assumptions such
as the no defiers assumption. - 4. Standard IV methods do not cope well with
time-varying exposures/mediatorsyet.
See Hernán and Robins (2006), Epidemiology for
further details
21Assumption trade-off
- IV methods replace one unverifiable assumption of
no unmeasured confounding between the mediator
and the outcome by other unverifiable assumptions - no unmeasured confounding for the instruments, or
- no direct effect of the instruments.
- We need to decide which assumptions are more
likely to - hold in our mediation analysis.
- An IV analysis will also increase the precision
of our estimates because of allowing for the
unmeasured confounding.
22Also
- What about if we want to estimate the direct
effect of randomisation in the presence of a
potential mediator?
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
Clearly we cant use randomisation as an
instrument herewe need another instrument.
23Plan for session 3
- Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
talk. - Why do we use instrumental variables?
- Where do we find instrumental variables?
- Examples
- PROSPECT mediator example
- SoCRATES SAS model.
- Designing trials with instruments in mind.
24Multiple instruments
- When we are trying to estimate the direct effect
of randomisation we need alternative instruments. - Likewise, if we have more than one endogenous
variable (multiple mediators), then we need
multiple instruments. - For IV model identification, we always need to
have as many instruments as we have endogenous
variables. - i.e. if considering two mediators in the model
(therapeutic alliance and number of sessions of
therapy attended), then we need at least two
instrumental variables.
25Where do we find instruments?
- Possibilities for IVs
- Randomisation-by-baseline variable interactions.
- Randomisation involving more than one active
treatment i.e. to interventions specifically
targeted at particular intermediate
variables/mediators. - Randomisation-by-trial (multiple trials).
- Genetic markers (Mendelian Randomisation) used
together with randomisation.
26Confounded Mediation Diagram
U the unmeasured confounders
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
If treatment is randomised then assumption of no
confounding of treatment and other variables
(outcomes) is justified.
27Mediation Diagram with instruments
U the unmeasured confounders
dX
U
RandomisationCovariates
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
Covariates
28Multiple Instruments
- Here, treatment by covariates interactions
represent instrumental variables. - Assumptions
- The interactions are significant in the first
stage regression (individually and joint F-test). - The only effect of the interactions on outcome is
through the mediator, and not a direct effect.
This is a very strong assumption - No other unmeasured confounders between the
interactions and outcome.
29Summary so far
- The analysis of mediation is more complex than it
first seems because of potential unmeasured
confounding (mediators are endogenous). - We use moderators of the relationship between
randomisation and the mediator (i.e. the baseline
by randomisation interactions) as instruments. - The analysis of mediation by instrumental
variables requires additional assumptions.
Primarily, that these covariates are not
moderators of the randomisation on outcome
relationship (no direct effect). - We illustrate these points on two examples now
30Plan for session 3
- Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
talk. - Why do we use instrumental variables?
- Where do we find instrumental variables?
- Examples
- PROSPECT mediator example
- SoCRATES SAS model.
- Designing trials with instruments in mind.
31Example PROSPECT
- PROSPECT (Prevention of Suicide in Primary Care
Elderly Collaborative Trial) was a multi-site
prospective, randomised trial designed to
evaluate the impact of a primary care-based
intervention on reducing major risk factors
(including depression) for suicide in elderly
depressed primary care patients. - The two conditions were either
- (a) an intervention based on treatment guidelines
tailored for the elderly with care management, - (b) treatment as usual.
- An intermediate outcome in the PROSPECT trial was
whether the trial participant adhered to
antidepressant medication during the period
following allocation of the intervention. - The question here is whether changes in
medication adherence following the intervention
might explain some or all of the observed (ITT)
effects on clinical outcome.
See Bruce et al, JAMA (2004) Ten Have et al,
Biometrics (2007) Bellamy et al, Clinical
Trials (2007) Lynch et al, Health Services and
Outcome Research Methodology (2008). Thanks to
Tom Ten Have for use of the data.
32Example PROSPECT - question of interest
RandomisationCovariates
Antidepressant Use
Depression Score
Randomisation
Covariates
33Example PROSPECT - summary stats
34PROSPECT data Stata describe
- . describe
- Contains data from P\SMinMR paper\Prospect.dta
- obs 297
- vars 8 11
Sep 2009 1601 - size 20,196 (99.9 of memory free)
- --------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------ - storage display value
- variable name type format label
variable label - --------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------ - cad1 double 10.0g
Anti-depressant use at baseline visit - hdrs0 double 10.0g
Hamilton depression score at baseline visit - ssix01 double 10.0g
Suicide ideation at baseline visit - scr01 double 10.0g
Past medication use at baseline visit - hdrs4 double 10.0g
Hamilton depression score at 4 month visit - site double 10.0g
Location of practices - interven double 10.0g
Randomized assignment to intervention - Amedx double 10.0g
Adherence to prescribed anti-depressant
medication
35PROSPECT data Stata ivregress
- . xi ivregress 2sls hdrs4 hdrs0 cad1 ssix01
scr01 i.site i.interven (amedx i.intervenhdrs0
i.intervencad1 i.intervenssix01
i.intervenscr01 i.interveni.site), first -
- First-stage regressions
- --------------------
Number of obs 296 -
F( 13, 282) 21.71 -
Prob gt F 0.0000 -
R-squared 0.5002 -
Adj R-squared 0.4772 -
Root MSE 0.3465 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - amedx Coef. Std. Err. t
Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval - -------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - hdrs0 .0065731 .0051473 1.28
0.203 -.0035588 .0167051 - cad1 .166495 .0254223 6.55
0.000 .1164533 .2165366 - ssix01 -.0475454 .0721387 -0.66
0.510 -.1895441 .0944533 - scr01 .2530611 .0746616 3.39
0.001 .1060962 .4000259 - _Isite_2 -.018463 .0664307 -0.28
0.781 -.149226 .1123 - _Isite_3 .1969925 .0734302 2.68
0.008 .0524516 .3415334 - _Iinterven_1 .7825965 .1398924 5.59
0.000 .5072307 1.057962
36PROSPECT data Stata ivregress
- Instrumental variables (2SLS) regression
Number of obs 296 -
Wald chi2(8) 102.68 -
Prob gt chi2 0.0000 -
R-squared 0.2582 -
Root MSE 6.8425 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - hdrs4 Coef. Std. Err. z
Pgtz 95 Conf. Interval - -------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - amedx -1.95302 2.672201 -0.73
0.465 -7.190438 3.284397 - hdrs0 .6226062 .070337 8.85
0.000 .4847482 .7604642 - cad1 -.0654087 .4304821 -0.15
0.879 -.9091381 .7783208 - ssix01 1.251204 .9399736 1.33
0.183 -.5911102 3.093518 - scr01 1.585044 1.074312 1.48
0.140 -.5205695 3.690658 - _Isite_2 -.4971475 .9469522 -0.52
0.600 -2.35314 1.358845 - _Isite_3 -2.046048 1.08319 -1.89
0.059 -4.169062 .0769655 - _Iinterven_1 -2.375598 1.328982 -1.79
0.074 -4.980353 .2291584 - _cons 3.344043 1.467043 2.28
0.023 .4686928 6.219394 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - Instrumented amedx
37Example PROSPECT - results
- Using all baseline variables as covariates in an
ANCOVA. - ITT effect -3.15 (0.82)
- Small but statistically significant effect
- Direct effect Indirect effect
- ? (s.e.) ß (s.e.)
- Analytical method
- Standard regression -2.66 (0.93) -1.24 (1.09)
- (Baron Kenny)
38Example PROSPECT - results
-
- Direct effect Indirect effect
- ? (s.e.) ß (s.e.)
- Analytical method
- IV (ivreg) -2.38 (1.35) -1.95 (2.71)
- IV (treatreg - ml) -2.34 (1.27) -2.05 (2.49)
- G-estimation -2.58 (1.27) -1.43 (2.34)
- Conclusion
- Allowing for hidden confounding appears to have
had little effect, except to increase the SE of
the estimate.
From Ten Have et al, Biometrics (2007)
39PROSPECT data ivregress postestimation
- . estat firststage
- First-stage regressions
- --------------------
Number of obs 296 -
F( 13, 282) 21.71 -
Prob gt F 0.0000 -
R-squared 0.5002 -
Adj R-squared 0.4772 -
Root MSE 0.3465 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - amedx Coef. Std. Err. t
Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval - -------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - hdrs0 .0065731 .0051473 1.28
0.203 -.0035588 .0167051 - cad1 .166495 .0254223 6.55
0.000 .1164533 .2165366 - ssix01 -.0475454 .0721387 -0.66
0.510 -.1895441 .0944533 - scr01 .2530611 .0746616 3.39
0.001 .1060962 .4000259 - _Isite_2 -.018463 .0664307 -0.28
0.781 -.149226 .1123 - _Isite_3 .1969925 .0734302 2.68
0.008 .0524516 .3415334 - _Iinterven_1 .7825965 .1398924 5.59
0.000 .5072307 1.057962 - _IintXhdrs1 -.003633 .0071484 -0.51
0.612 -.0177041 .010438
40PROSPECT data ivregress postestimation
- (no endogenous regressors)
- ( 1) _IintXhdrs0_1 0
- ( 2) _IintXcad1_1 0
- ( 3) _IintXssix0_1 0
- ( 4) _IintXscr01_1 0
- ( 5) _IintXsit_1_2 0
- ( 6) _IintXsit_1_3 0
- F( 6, 282) 9.10 Prob gt F
0.0000 - First-stage regression summary statistics
- ------------------------------------------------
-------------------------- - Adjusted Partial
- Variable R-sq. R-sq. R-sq.
F(6,282) Prob gt F - -----------------------------------------------
-------------------------- - amedx 0.5002 0.4772 0.1622
9.10057 0.0000 - ------------------------------------------------
-------------------------- - Minimum eigenvalue statistic 9.10057
41Instrumental Variables in SPSS
Generate interactions as additional variables
using compute
Analyse Regression 2-stage Least Squares
42Instrumental Variables in SPSS
Outcome
Covariates and endogenous variable (mediator)
Covariates and instruments
43Example the SoCRATES trial
- SoCRATES was a multi-centre RCT designed to
evaluate the effects of cognitive behaviour
therapy (CBT) and supportive counselling (SC) on
the outcomes of an early episode of
schizophrenia. - 201 participants were allocated to one of three
groups - Control Treatment as Usual (TAU)
- Treatment TAU plus psychological intervention,
either CBT TAU or SC TAU - The two treatment groups are combined in our
analyses - Outcome psychotic symptoms score (PANSS) at 18
months
44Example SoCRATES - summary stats
Lewis et al, BJP (2002) Tarrier et al, BJP
(2004) Dunn Bentall, Stats in Medicine (2007)
Emsley, White and Dunn, Stats Methods in Medical
Research (2009).
45Confounded Dose-Response
dX
U
Sessions Attended
ß
a
Randomisation
Psychotic Symptoms
dY
Are the effects of Randomisation on Sessions (a)
and, more interestingly, the effects of Sessions
on Outcome (ß), influenced by the strength of the
therapeutic alliance?
46The S AS model
- We want to estimate the joint effects of the
strength of the therapeutic alliance as measured
by CALPAS (A) and number of sessions attended
(S). - We postulate a structural model as follows
- EYi(1)-Yi(0) Xi, Di(1)s, Di(0)0 Aia
- ßss ßsas(a-7)
- No sessions implies no treatment effect.
- The effect of alliance is multiplicative so we
only have an interaction effect of alliance no
sessions no alliance.
Dunn and Bentall, SiM (2007)
47SoCRATES analysis results
- Method ßs (se) ßsa (se)
- Instrumental variables -2.40 (0.70) -1.28 (0.48)
- Standard regression (BK) -0.95 (0.22) -0.39
(0.11) - Note A has been rescaled so that maximum0.
- When A0 (i.e. maximum alliance)
- the slope for effect of Sessions is -2.40
- When A-7 (i.e. minimum alliance)
- the slope is -2.40 71.28 6.56
- This suggests that when alliance is very poor
attending more sessions makes the outcome worse!
48SoCRATES S AS using regress
- . regress pant18 sessions s_a pantot logdup c1
c2 yearsed - Source SS df MS
Number of obs 153 - -------------------------------------------
F( 7, 145) 15.78 - Model 24414.5544 7 3487.79349
Prob gt F 0.0000 - Residual 32051.4194 145 221.044272
R-squared 0.4324 - -------------------------------------------
Adj R-squared 0.4050 - Total 56465.9739 152 371.48667
Root MSE 14.868 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - pant18 Coef. Std. Err. t
Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval - -------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - sessions -.9459469 .2209236 -4.28
0.000 -1.382593 -.5093003 - s_a -.3866447 .1117784 -3.46
0.001 -.6075702 -.1657192 - pantot .3843765 .087454 4.40
0.000 .2115272 .5572259 - logdup 2.331363 2.398488 0.97
0.333 -2.409152 7.071878 - c1 4.322976 3.48805 1.24
0.217 -2.571014 11.21697 - c2 -11.96141 3.292382 -3.63
0.000 -18.46867 -5.454147
49SoCRATES S AS using ivregress
- . ivregress 2sls pant18 pantot logdup c1 c2
yearsed (sessions s_a group lgp c1gp c2gp yrgp
pgp) - First-stage regressions
- -----------------------
Number of obs 153 -
F( 11, 141) 78.68 -
Prob gt F 0.0000 -
R-squared 0.8599 -
Adj R-squared 0.8490 -
Root MSE 3.3588 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - sessions Coef. Std. Err. t
Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval - -------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - pantot 1.71e-14 .0310634 0.00
1.000 -.0614103 .0614103 - logdup 2.46e-13 .858628 0.00
1.000 -1.697449 1.697449 - c1 -3.59e-13 1.125814 -0.00
1.000 -2.225657 2.225657 - c2 4.70e-14 1.022741 0.00
1.000 -2.021889 2.021889 - yearsed 1.17e-13 .1929797 0.00
1.000 -.3815077 .3815077 - group 16.09465 5.201659 3.09
0.002 5.811326 26.37798 - lgp .1800265 1.104039 0.16
0.871 -2.002583 2.362636
Model for sessions
50SoCRATES S AS using ivregress
-
Number of obs 153 -
F( 11, 141) 16.59 -
Prob gt F 0.0000 -
R-squared 0.5641 -
Adj R-squared 0.5301 -
Root MSE 12.0225 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - s_a Coef. Std. Err. t
Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval - -------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - pantot -1.89e-14 .1111878 -0.00
1.000 -.2198106 .2198106 - logdup -1.89e-13 3.073353 -0.00
1.000 -6.075809 6.075809 - c1 3.31e-13 4.029712 0.00
1.000 -7.966465 7.966465 - c2 -3.78e-14 3.660775 -0.00
1.000 -7.237101 7.237101 - yearsed -1.00e-13 .6907472 -0.00
1.000 -1.36556 1.36556 - group -16.2085 18.6187 -0.87
0.385 -53.0164 20.59939 - lgp -6.186983 3.951771 -1.57
0.120 -13.99936 1.625398 - c1gp -11.44637 5.635471 -2.03
0.044 -22.58731 -.3054279
Model for sessionsalliance
51SoCRATES S AS using ivregress
- Instrumental variables (2SLS) regression
Number of obs 153 -
Wald chi2(7) 83.17 -
Prob gt chi2 0.0000 -
R-squared 0.1795 -
Root MSE 17.401 - --------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - pant18 Coef. Std. Err. z
Pgtz 95 Conf. Interval - -------------------------------------------------
---------------------------- - sessions -2.401159 .6776074 -3.54
0.000 -3.729245 -1.073073 - s_a -1.281461 .4380021 -2.93
0.003 -2.139929 -.4229929 - pantot .3864756 .1024045 3.77
0.000 .1857664 .5871848 - logdup -.2044085 3.091853 -0.07
0.947 -6.264329 5.855512 - c1 -1.21612 4.868577 -0.25
0.803 -10.75836 8.326116 - c2 -16.32291 4.324444 -3.77
0.000 -24.79866 -7.847155 - yearsed -.9923864 .6258703 -1.59
0.113 -2.21907 .2342968 - _cons 49.26983 13.27743 3.71
0.000 23.24655 75.29311 - --------------------------------------------------
----------------------------
52Plan for session 3
- Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
talk. - Why do we use instrumental variables?
- Where do we find instrumental variables?
- Examples
- PROSPECT mediator example
- SoCRATES SAS model.
- Designing trials with instruments in mind.
53Instrumental Variables in observational studies
- There are numerous examples of instruments in the
absence of randomisation - Access to health care
- Distance to hospital
- Genes (known as Mendelian randomisation)
- Proxy measures of genes (product intolerance)
- Physicians preference (ask, or use proportion of
patients on treatment)
54Designing trials with IVs in mind
- Thinking back to some of the possibilities for
IVs we introduced earlier with design
considerations - Randomisation-by-baseline variable
interactions.Can we measure any extra baseline
variables? - Randomisation involving more than one active
treatment i.e. to interventions specifically
targeted at particular intermediate
variables/mediators. - More complicated designs/parallel trials
- Randomisation-by-trial (multiple trials).
- Meta-regression approaches (new MRC grant)
- Genetic markers (Mendelian Randomisation) used
together with randomisation. - Need to measure genotype in patients
55Example Series of parallel trials
Mediator 1
Randomisation 1
Common Outcome
Trial 1
Mediator 2
Randomisation 2
Common Outcome
Trial 2
Mediator 3
Randomisation 3
Common Outcome
Trial 3
56Example measuring additional variables
Putative mediator is a measure of the
therapist/patient interaction or relationship
e.g. Measure of patients interaction with other
individuals Care coordinator, family members,
etc. e.g. Patient characteristics which could
influence ability to form alliance personality
disorders, etc.
Similar Baseline measures
Therapeutic Alliance
Randomisation
Outcomes
57Short small group discussion
- We will work in small groups again.
- We are thinking about designing psychological
treatment trials in order to answer some of the
explanatory questions discussed in this session? - When considering the following potential
mediators - How would we accurately measure the mediator?
- What additional baseline variables might we be
able to collect which would help in the causal/IV
analysis? - What problems could you foresee in the collection
of this information? - How might you justify the need to collect this
information to funders of the trials who would
prefer to keep it large and simple?
58Potential mediators for discussion
- What are the participants beliefs?
- Does psychotherapy change attributions
(beliefs), which, in turn, lead to better
outcome? - What is the concomitant medication?
- Does psychotherapy improve compliance with
medication which, in turn, leads to better
outcome? - What is the concomitant substance abuse?
- Does psychotherapy reduce substance use, which
in turn leads to improvements in psychotic
symptoms? -
59References Mediation Effect Moderation in
Psychological Treatment Trials
- Methodology for IV methods with mediation
- Emsley RA, Dunn G White IR (2009). Mediation
and moderation of treatment effects in randomised
trials of complex interventions. Statistical
Methods in Medical Research. In press (available
online). - Maracy M Dunn G (2009). Estimating
dose-response effects in psychological treatment
trials the role of instrumental variables.
Statistical Methods in Medical Research. In
press (available online). - Dunn G Bentall R (2007). Modelling
treatment-effect heterogeneity in randomized
controlled trials of complex interventions
(psychological treatments). Statistics in
Medicine 26, 4719-4745. - Website with downloads
- http//www.medicine.manchester.ac.uk/healthmethodo
logy/research/biostatistics/
60Some Further Reading
- Ten Have TR, Joffe MM, Lynch KG, Brown GK, Maisto
SA Beck AT (2007). Causal mediation analyses
with rank preserving models. Biometrics 63,
926-934. - Gallop R, Small DS, Lin JY, Elliot MR, Joffe MM
Ten Have TR (2009). Mediation analysis with
principal stratification. Statistics in Medicine
28, 1108-1130. - Bellamy SL, Lin JY Ten Have TR (2007). An
introduction to causal modelling in clinical
trials. Clinical Trials 4, 58-73. - Lynch K, Cary M, Gallop R, Ten Have TR (2008).
Causal mediation analyses for randomized trials.
Health Services Outcomes Research Methodology
8, 57-76. - Albert JM (2008). Mediation analysis via
potential outcomes models. Statistics in Medicine
27, 1282-1304. - Jo B (2008). Causal inference in randomized
experiments with mediational processes.
Psychological Methods 13, 314-336. - Gennetian LA, Morris PA, Bos JM Bloom HS
(2005). Constructing instrumental variables from
experimental data to explore how treatments
produce effects. In Bloom HS, editor. Learning
More From Social Experiments Evolving Analytic
Approaches. New York Russell Sage Foundation
pp. 75-114. - MacKinnon DP (2008). Introduction to Statistical
Mediation Analysis. New York Taylor Francis
Group.