Analysis of mediation and moderation using instrumental variables - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 60
About This Presentation
Title:

Analysis of mediation and moderation using instrumental variables

Description:

Methods of explanatory analysis for psychological treatment trials workshop ... Genetic markers (Mendelian Randomisation) used together with randomisation. 26 ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:662
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 61
Provided by: richard816
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Analysis of mediation and moderation using instrumental variables


1
Methods of explanatory analysis for psychological
treatment trials workshop
  • Session 3
  • Analysis of mediation and moderation using
    instrumental variables
  • Richard Emsley

Funded by MRC Methodology Grant G0600555 MHRN
Methodology Research Group
2
Plan for session 3
  • Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
    talk.
  • Why do we use instrumental variables?
  • Where do we find instrumental variables?
  • Examples
  • PROSPECT mediator example
  • SoCRATES SAS model.
  • Designing trials with instruments in mind.

3
Quick review of IVs from Ians talk
  • Ian has demonstrated how we can use instrumental
    variable methods to infer a causal effect of
    treatment in the presence of departures from
    randomised intervention.
  • This utilises randomisation as the instrumental
    variable. As we will see, randomisation meets
    the assumptions required for an IV
  • But we will also need to consider the situation
    where we cannot use randomisation as an
    instrument

4
Instrumental Variables (IVs)
  • In a standard regression model, if an explanatory
    variable is correlated with the error term (known
    as endogeneity) its coefficient cannot be
    unbiasedly estimated.
  • An instrumental variable (IV) is a variable that
    does not appear in the model, is uncorrelated
    with the error term and is correlated with the
    endogenous explanatory variable randomisation,
    where available, often satisfies this criteria.
  • A two stage least squares (2SLS) procedure can
    then be applied to estimate the coefficient. At
    its simplest, the first stage involves using a
    simple linear regression of the endogenous
    variable on the instrument and saving the
    predicted values. In the second stage the
    outcome is then regressed on the predicted
    values, with the latter regression coefficient
    being the required estimate of the coefficient.

5
Some notation
  • Ri treatment group the outcome of
    randomisation (Ri1 for treatment, 0 for
    controls).
  • Xi' X1i, X2i Xpi baseline covariates.
  • Yi observed outcome.
  • Di actual treatment received. This is an
    intermediate outcome that is a putative mediator
    of the effects of treatment on outcome (either a
    quantitative measure or binary).

6
Instrumental variables (IV) (from session 1)
  • Popular in econometrics
  • Simplest idea is
  • Outcome Yi a b Di ei
  • Treatment Di g d Ri fi
  • Allow error ei to be correlated with Di but
    assume its independent of Ri
  • randomisation Ri only affects outcome through its
    effect on compliance Di
  • Estimation by two-stage least squares
  • EYi Ri a b EDi Ri
  • so first regress Di on Ri to get EDi Ri
  • then regress Yi on EDi Ri
  • NB standard errors not quite correct by this
    method general IV uses different standard errors

7
Simple Mediation Idea (from session 2)
dX
Mediator
ß
a
Treatment
Outcomes
dY
?
The total effect is the sum of the direct effect
(?) and the indirect effect (aß)
8
Confounded Mediation Diagram
U the unmeasured confounders
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Treatment
Outcomes
dY
?
If treatment is randomised then assumption of no
confounding of treatment and other variables
(outcomes) is justified.
9
Confounded Mediation Diagram
dX
U
U
Mediator
ß
a
Treatment
Outcomes
dY
?
U
If treatment is not randomised then there is
likely to be even more unmeasured confounding.
10
Confounded Mediation Diagram
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
Thankfully were talking about randomised trials!
11
Linking the two previous sessions Compliance as
a mediator
dX
Treatment Received
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
12
Linking the two previous sessions Randomisation
as an IV
dX
Treatment Received
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
By assuming the absence of a direct path from
randomisation to outcome, we assume the entire
effect of randomisation acts through receipt of
treatment. ? randomisation is an instrumental
variable.
13
Plan for session 3
  • Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
    talk.
  • Why do we use instrumental variables?
  • Where do we find instrumental variables?
  • Examples
  • PROSPECT mediator example
  • SoCRATES SAS model.
  • Designing trials with instruments in mind.

14
Why do we use instrumental variables?
  • All available statistical methods we usually use
    (for any standard analysis), including
  • Stratification
  • Regression
  • Matching
  • Standardization
  • require the one unverifiable condition we
    identified previously
  • NO UNMEASURED CONFOUNDING

15
Why do we use instrumental variables?
  • Unlike all other methods, IV methods can be used
    to consistently estimate causal effects in the
    presence of unmeasured confounding AND
    measurement error.
  • SO WE CAN SOLVE THE PROBLEM OF

dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
16
Definition of an instrumental variable
  • A variable is an instrumental variable Z if
  • Z has a causal effect on the mediator D
  • This can be tested in the data.
  • ii. Z affects the outcome Y only through D
  • i.e. there is no direct effect of Z on Y
  • This is an assumption (sometimes a strong
    assumption).
  • iii. Z does not share common causes with the
    outcome Y
  • i.e. there is no confounding for the effect of Z
    on Y.
  • This is another assumption which randomisation
    satisfies but other IVs may not.

17
Assumptions for instrumental variables
  • IV methods require FOUR assumptions
  • The first 3 assumptions are from the definition
  • The association between instrument and mediator.
  • no direct effect of the instrument on outcome.
  • no unmeasured confounding for the instrument and
    outcome.
  • There are a wide variety of fourth assumptions
    and different assumptions result in the
    estimation of different causal effects
  • E.g. no interactions, monotonicity (no defiers).

18
Testing assumptions
  • There are a number of tests we can use for some
    of these assumptions.
  • Stata has three postestimation commands following
    ivregress
  • estat overid
  • estat endogenous
  • estat firststage
  • This final option is perhaps the most useful. It
    gives an indication of whether the set of
    instruments strongly predict the mediator see
    PROSPECT example later on.

19
Advantages of IVs
  • Can allow for unmeasured confounding
  • Can allow for measurement error
  • Randomisation meets the definition so is an ideal
    instrument
  • When available.
  • Obviously not in observational studies.

20
Disadvantages of IVs
  • 1. It is impossible to verify that Z is an
    instrument and using a non instrument introduces
    additional bias.
  • 2. A weak instrument Z increases the bias over
    that of ordinary regression.
  • 3. Instruments by themselves are actually
    insufficient to estimate causal effects and we
    require additional unverifiable assumptions such
    as the no defiers assumption.
  • 4. Standard IV methods do not cope well with
    time-varying exposures/mediatorsyet.

See Hernán and Robins (2006), Epidemiology for
further details
21
Assumption trade-off
  • IV methods replace one unverifiable assumption of
    no unmeasured confounding between the mediator
    and the outcome by other unverifiable assumptions
  • no unmeasured confounding for the instruments, or
  • no direct effect of the instruments.
  • We need to decide which assumptions are more
    likely to
  • hold in our mediation analysis.
  • An IV analysis will also increase the precision
    of our estimates because of allowing for the
    unmeasured confounding.

22
Also
  • What about if we want to estimate the direct
    effect of randomisation in the presence of a
    potential mediator?

dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
Clearly we cant use randomisation as an
instrument herewe need another instrument.
23
Plan for session 3
  • Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
    talk.
  • Why do we use instrumental variables?
  • Where do we find instrumental variables?
  • Examples
  • PROSPECT mediator example
  • SoCRATES SAS model.
  • Designing trials with instruments in mind.

24
Multiple instruments
  • When we are trying to estimate the direct effect
    of randomisation we need alternative instruments.
  • Likewise, if we have more than one endogenous
    variable (multiple mediators), then we need
    multiple instruments.
  • For IV model identification, we always need to
    have as many instruments as we have endogenous
    variables.
  • i.e. if considering two mediators in the model
    (therapeutic alliance and number of sessions of
    therapy attended), then we need at least two
    instrumental variables.

25
Where do we find instruments?
  • Possibilities for IVs
  • Randomisation-by-baseline variable interactions.
  • Randomisation involving more than one active
    treatment i.e. to interventions specifically
    targeted at particular intermediate
    variables/mediators.
  • Randomisation-by-trial (multiple trials).
  • Genetic markers (Mendelian Randomisation) used
    together with randomisation.

26
Confounded Mediation Diagram
U the unmeasured confounders
dX
U
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
If treatment is randomised then assumption of no
confounding of treatment and other variables
(outcomes) is justified.
27
Mediation Diagram with instruments
U the unmeasured confounders
dX
U
RandomisationCovariates
Mediator
ß
a
Randomisation
Outcomes
dY
?
Covariates
28
Multiple Instruments
  • Here, treatment by covariates interactions
    represent instrumental variables.
  • Assumptions
  • The interactions are significant in the first
    stage regression (individually and joint F-test).
  • The only effect of the interactions on outcome is
    through the mediator, and not a direct effect.
    This is a very strong assumption
  • No other unmeasured confounders between the
    interactions and outcome.

29
Summary so far
  • The analysis of mediation is more complex than it
    first seems because of potential unmeasured
    confounding (mediators are endogenous).
  • We use moderators of the relationship between
    randomisation and the mediator (i.e. the baseline
    by randomisation interactions) as instruments.
  • The analysis of mediation by instrumental
    variables requires additional assumptions.
    Primarily, that these covariates are not
    moderators of the randomisation on outcome
    relationship (no direct effect).
  • We illustrate these points on two examples now

30
Plan for session 3
  • Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
    talk.
  • Why do we use instrumental variables?
  • Where do we find instrumental variables?
  • Examples
  • PROSPECT mediator example
  • SoCRATES SAS model.
  • Designing trials with instruments in mind.

31
Example PROSPECT
  • PROSPECT (Prevention of Suicide in Primary Care
    Elderly Collaborative Trial) was a multi-site
    prospective, randomised trial designed to
    evaluate the impact of a primary care-based
    intervention on reducing major risk factors
    (including depression) for suicide in elderly
    depressed primary care patients.
  • The two conditions were either
  • (a) an intervention based on treatment guidelines
    tailored for the elderly with care management,
  • (b) treatment as usual.
  • An intermediate outcome in the PROSPECT trial was
    whether the trial participant adhered to
    antidepressant medication during the period
    following allocation of the intervention.
  • The question here is whether changes in
    medication adherence following the intervention
    might explain some or all of the observed (ITT)
    effects on clinical outcome.

See Bruce et al, JAMA (2004) Ten Have et al,
Biometrics (2007) Bellamy et al, Clinical
Trials (2007) Lynch et al, Health Services and
Outcome Research Methodology (2008). Thanks to
Tom Ten Have for use of the data.
32
Example PROSPECT - question of interest
RandomisationCovariates
Antidepressant Use
Depression Score
Randomisation
Covariates
33
Example PROSPECT - summary stats
34
PROSPECT data Stata describe
  • . describe
  • Contains data from P\SMinMR paper\Prospect.dta
  • obs 297
  • vars 8 11
    Sep 2009 1601
  • size 20,196 (99.9 of memory free)
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ------------------------------------------
  • storage display value
  • variable name type format label
    variable label
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ------------------------------------------
  • cad1 double 10.0g
    Anti-depressant use at baseline visit
  • hdrs0 double 10.0g
    Hamilton depression score at baseline visit
  • ssix01 double 10.0g
    Suicide ideation at baseline visit
  • scr01 double 10.0g
    Past medication use at baseline visit
  • hdrs4 double 10.0g
    Hamilton depression score at 4 month visit
  • site double 10.0g
    Location of practices
  • interven double 10.0g
    Randomized assignment to intervention
  • Amedx double 10.0g
    Adherence to prescribed anti-depressant
    medication

35
PROSPECT data Stata ivregress
  • . xi ivregress 2sls hdrs4 hdrs0 cad1 ssix01
    scr01 i.site i.interven (amedx i.intervenhdrs0
    i.intervencad1 i.intervenssix01
    i.intervenscr01 i.interveni.site), first
  • First-stage regressions
  • --------------------
    Number of obs 296

  • F( 13, 282) 21.71

  • Prob gt F 0.0000

  • R-squared 0.5002

  • Adj R-squared 0.4772

  • Root MSE 0.3465
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • amedx Coef. Std. Err. t
    Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • hdrs0 .0065731 .0051473 1.28
    0.203 -.0035588 .0167051
  • cad1 .166495 .0254223 6.55
    0.000 .1164533 .2165366
  • ssix01 -.0475454 .0721387 -0.66
    0.510 -.1895441 .0944533
  • scr01 .2530611 .0746616 3.39
    0.001 .1060962 .4000259
  • _Isite_2 -.018463 .0664307 -0.28
    0.781 -.149226 .1123
  • _Isite_3 .1969925 .0734302 2.68
    0.008 .0524516 .3415334
  • _Iinterven_1 .7825965 .1398924 5.59
    0.000 .5072307 1.057962

36
PROSPECT data Stata ivregress
  • Instrumental variables (2SLS) regression
    Number of obs 296

  • Wald chi2(8) 102.68

  • Prob gt chi2 0.0000

  • R-squared 0.2582

  • Root MSE 6.8425
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • hdrs4 Coef. Std. Err. z
    Pgtz 95 Conf. Interval
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • amedx -1.95302 2.672201 -0.73
    0.465 -7.190438 3.284397
  • hdrs0 .6226062 .070337 8.85
    0.000 .4847482 .7604642
  • cad1 -.0654087 .4304821 -0.15
    0.879 -.9091381 .7783208
  • ssix01 1.251204 .9399736 1.33
    0.183 -.5911102 3.093518
  • scr01 1.585044 1.074312 1.48
    0.140 -.5205695 3.690658
  • _Isite_2 -.4971475 .9469522 -0.52
    0.600 -2.35314 1.358845
  • _Isite_3 -2.046048 1.08319 -1.89
    0.059 -4.169062 .0769655
  • _Iinterven_1 -2.375598 1.328982 -1.79
    0.074 -4.980353 .2291584
  • _cons 3.344043 1.467043 2.28
    0.023 .4686928 6.219394
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • Instrumented amedx

37
Example PROSPECT - results
  • Using all baseline variables as covariates in an
    ANCOVA.
  • ITT effect -3.15 (0.82)
  • Small but statistically significant effect
  • Direct effect Indirect effect
  • ? (s.e.) ß (s.e.)
  • Analytical method
  • Standard regression -2.66 (0.93) -1.24 (1.09)
  • (Baron Kenny)

38
Example PROSPECT - results
  • Direct effect Indirect effect
  • ? (s.e.) ß (s.e.)
  • Analytical method
  • IV (ivreg) -2.38 (1.35) -1.95 (2.71)
  • IV (treatreg - ml) -2.34 (1.27) -2.05 (2.49)
  • G-estimation -2.58 (1.27) -1.43 (2.34)
  • Conclusion
  • Allowing for hidden confounding appears to have
    had little effect, except to increase the SE of
    the estimate.

From Ten Have et al, Biometrics (2007)
39
PROSPECT data ivregress postestimation
  • . estat firststage
  • First-stage regressions
  • --------------------
    Number of obs 296

  • F( 13, 282) 21.71

  • Prob gt F 0.0000

  • R-squared 0.5002

  • Adj R-squared 0.4772

  • Root MSE 0.3465
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • amedx Coef. Std. Err. t
    Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • hdrs0 .0065731 .0051473 1.28
    0.203 -.0035588 .0167051
  • cad1 .166495 .0254223 6.55
    0.000 .1164533 .2165366
  • ssix01 -.0475454 .0721387 -0.66
    0.510 -.1895441 .0944533
  • scr01 .2530611 .0746616 3.39
    0.001 .1060962 .4000259
  • _Isite_2 -.018463 .0664307 -0.28
    0.781 -.149226 .1123
  • _Isite_3 .1969925 .0734302 2.68
    0.008 .0524516 .3415334
  • _Iinterven_1 .7825965 .1398924 5.59
    0.000 .5072307 1.057962
  • _IintXhdrs1 -.003633 .0071484 -0.51
    0.612 -.0177041 .010438

40
PROSPECT data ivregress postestimation
  • (no endogenous regressors)
  • ( 1) _IintXhdrs0_1 0
  • ( 2) _IintXcad1_1 0
  • ( 3) _IintXssix0_1 0
  • ( 4) _IintXscr01_1 0
  • ( 5) _IintXsit_1_2 0
  • ( 6) _IintXsit_1_3 0
  • F( 6, 282) 9.10 Prob gt F
    0.0000
  • First-stage regression summary statistics
  • ------------------------------------------------
    --------------------------
  • Adjusted Partial
  • Variable R-sq. R-sq. R-sq.
    F(6,282) Prob gt F
  • -----------------------------------------------
    --------------------------
  • amedx 0.5002 0.4772 0.1622
    9.10057 0.0000
  • ------------------------------------------------
    --------------------------
  • Minimum eigenvalue statistic 9.10057

41
Instrumental Variables in SPSS
Generate interactions as additional variables
using compute
Analyse Regression 2-stage Least Squares
42
Instrumental Variables in SPSS
Outcome
Covariates and endogenous variable (mediator)
Covariates and instruments
43
Example the SoCRATES trial
  • SoCRATES was a multi-centre RCT designed to
    evaluate the effects of cognitive behaviour
    therapy (CBT) and supportive counselling (SC) on
    the outcomes of an early episode of
    schizophrenia.
  • 201 participants were allocated to one of three
    groups
  • Control Treatment as Usual (TAU)
  • Treatment TAU plus psychological intervention,
    either CBT TAU or SC TAU
  • The two treatment groups are combined in our
    analyses
  • Outcome psychotic symptoms score (PANSS) at 18
    months

44
Example SoCRATES - summary stats
Lewis et al, BJP (2002) Tarrier et al, BJP
(2004) Dunn Bentall, Stats in Medicine (2007)
Emsley, White and Dunn, Stats Methods in Medical
Research (2009).
45
Confounded Dose-Response
dX
U
Sessions Attended
ß
a
Randomisation
Psychotic Symptoms
dY
Are the effects of Randomisation on Sessions (a)
and, more interestingly, the effects of Sessions
on Outcome (ß), influenced by the strength of the
therapeutic alliance?
46
The S AS model
  • We want to estimate the joint effects of the
    strength of the therapeutic alliance as measured
    by CALPAS (A) and number of sessions attended
    (S).
  • We postulate a structural model as follows
  • EYi(1)-Yi(0) Xi, Di(1)s, Di(0)0 Aia
  • ßss ßsas(a-7)
  • No sessions implies no treatment effect.
  • The effect of alliance is multiplicative so we
    only have an interaction effect of alliance no
    sessions no alliance.

Dunn and Bentall, SiM (2007)
47
SoCRATES analysis results
  • Method ßs (se) ßsa (se)
  • Instrumental variables -2.40 (0.70) -1.28 (0.48)
  • Standard regression (BK) -0.95 (0.22) -0.39
    (0.11)
  • Note A has been rescaled so that maximum0.
  • When A0 (i.e. maximum alliance)
  • the slope for effect of Sessions is -2.40
  • When A-7 (i.e. minimum alliance)
  • the slope is -2.40 71.28 6.56
  • This suggests that when alliance is very poor
    attending more sessions makes the outcome worse!

48
SoCRATES S AS using regress
  • . regress pant18 sessions s_a pantot logdup c1
    c2 yearsed
  • Source SS df MS
    Number of obs 153
  • -------------------------------------------
    F( 7, 145) 15.78
  • Model 24414.5544 7 3487.79349
    Prob gt F 0.0000
  • Residual 32051.4194 145 221.044272
    R-squared 0.4324
  • -------------------------------------------
    Adj R-squared 0.4050
  • Total 56465.9739 152 371.48667
    Root MSE 14.868
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • pant18 Coef. Std. Err. t
    Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • sessions -.9459469 .2209236 -4.28
    0.000 -1.382593 -.5093003
  • s_a -.3866447 .1117784 -3.46
    0.001 -.6075702 -.1657192
  • pantot .3843765 .087454 4.40
    0.000 .2115272 .5572259
  • logdup 2.331363 2.398488 0.97
    0.333 -2.409152 7.071878
  • c1 4.322976 3.48805 1.24
    0.217 -2.571014 11.21697
  • c2 -11.96141 3.292382 -3.63
    0.000 -18.46867 -5.454147

49
SoCRATES S AS using ivregress
  • . ivregress 2sls pant18 pantot logdup c1 c2
    yearsed (sessions s_a group lgp c1gp c2gp yrgp
    pgp)
  • First-stage regressions
  • -----------------------
    Number of obs 153

  • F( 11, 141) 78.68

  • Prob gt F 0.0000

  • R-squared 0.8599

  • Adj R-squared 0.8490

  • Root MSE 3.3588
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • sessions Coef. Std. Err. t
    Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • pantot 1.71e-14 .0310634 0.00
    1.000 -.0614103 .0614103
  • logdup 2.46e-13 .858628 0.00
    1.000 -1.697449 1.697449
  • c1 -3.59e-13 1.125814 -0.00
    1.000 -2.225657 2.225657
  • c2 4.70e-14 1.022741 0.00
    1.000 -2.021889 2.021889
  • yearsed 1.17e-13 .1929797 0.00
    1.000 -.3815077 .3815077
  • group 16.09465 5.201659 3.09
    0.002 5.811326 26.37798
  • lgp .1800265 1.104039 0.16
    0.871 -2.002583 2.362636

Model for sessions
50
SoCRATES S AS using ivregress

  • Number of obs 153

  • F( 11, 141) 16.59

  • Prob gt F 0.0000

  • R-squared 0.5641

  • Adj R-squared 0.5301

  • Root MSE 12.0225
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • s_a Coef. Std. Err. t
    Pgtt 95 Conf. Interval
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • pantot -1.89e-14 .1111878 -0.00
    1.000 -.2198106 .2198106
  • logdup -1.89e-13 3.073353 -0.00
    1.000 -6.075809 6.075809
  • c1 3.31e-13 4.029712 0.00
    1.000 -7.966465 7.966465
  • c2 -3.78e-14 3.660775 -0.00
    1.000 -7.237101 7.237101
  • yearsed -1.00e-13 .6907472 -0.00
    1.000 -1.36556 1.36556
  • group -16.2085 18.6187 -0.87
    0.385 -53.0164 20.59939
  • lgp -6.186983 3.951771 -1.57
    0.120 -13.99936 1.625398
  • c1gp -11.44637 5.635471 -2.03
    0.044 -22.58731 -.3054279

Model for sessionsalliance
51
SoCRATES S AS using ivregress
  • Instrumental variables (2SLS) regression
    Number of obs 153

  • Wald chi2(7) 83.17

  • Prob gt chi2 0.0000

  • R-squared 0.1795

  • Root MSE 17.401
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • pant18 Coef. Std. Err. z
    Pgtz 95 Conf. Interval
  • -------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------
  • sessions -2.401159 .6776074 -3.54
    0.000 -3.729245 -1.073073
  • s_a -1.281461 .4380021 -2.93
    0.003 -2.139929 -.4229929
  • pantot .3864756 .1024045 3.77
    0.000 .1857664 .5871848
  • logdup -.2044085 3.091853 -0.07
    0.947 -6.264329 5.855512
  • c1 -1.21612 4.868577 -0.25
    0.803 -10.75836 8.326116
  • c2 -16.32291 4.324444 -3.77
    0.000 -24.79866 -7.847155
  • yearsed -.9923864 .6258703 -1.59
    0.113 -2.21907 .2342968
  • _cons 49.26983 13.27743 3.71
    0.000 23.24655 75.29311
  • --------------------------------------------------
    ----------------------------

52
Plan for session 3
  • Quick review of instrumental variables from Ians
    talk.
  • Why do we use instrumental variables?
  • Where do we find instrumental variables?
  • Examples
  • PROSPECT mediator example
  • SoCRATES SAS model.
  • Designing trials with instruments in mind.

53
Instrumental Variables in observational studies
  • There are numerous examples of instruments in the
    absence of randomisation
  • Access to health care
  • Distance to hospital
  • Genes (known as Mendelian randomisation)
  • Proxy measures of genes (product intolerance)
  • Physicians preference (ask, or use proportion of
    patients on treatment)

54
Designing trials with IVs in mind
  • Thinking back to some of the possibilities for
    IVs we introduced earlier with design
    considerations
  • Randomisation-by-baseline variable
    interactions.Can we measure any extra baseline
    variables?
  • Randomisation involving more than one active
    treatment i.e. to interventions specifically
    targeted at particular intermediate
    variables/mediators.
  • More complicated designs/parallel trials
  • Randomisation-by-trial (multiple trials).
  • Meta-regression approaches (new MRC grant)
  • Genetic markers (Mendelian Randomisation) used
    together with randomisation.
  • Need to measure genotype in patients

55
Example Series of parallel trials
Mediator 1
Randomisation 1
Common Outcome
Trial 1
Mediator 2
Randomisation 2
Common Outcome
Trial 2
Mediator 3
Randomisation 3
Common Outcome
Trial 3
56
Example measuring additional variables
Putative mediator is a measure of the
therapist/patient interaction or relationship
e.g. Measure of patients interaction with other
individuals Care coordinator, family members,
etc. e.g. Patient characteristics which could
influence ability to form alliance personality
disorders, etc.
Similar Baseline measures
Therapeutic Alliance
Randomisation
Outcomes
57
Short small group discussion
  • We will work in small groups again.
  • We are thinking about designing psychological
    treatment trials in order to answer some of the
    explanatory questions discussed in this session?
  • When considering the following potential
    mediators
  • How would we accurately measure the mediator?
  • What additional baseline variables might we be
    able to collect which would help in the causal/IV
    analysis?
  • What problems could you foresee in the collection
    of this information?
  • How might you justify the need to collect this
    information to funders of the trials who would
    prefer to keep it large and simple?

58
Potential mediators for discussion
  • What are the participants beliefs?
  • Does psychotherapy change attributions
    (beliefs), which, in turn, lead to better
    outcome?
  • What is the concomitant medication?
  • Does psychotherapy improve compliance with
    medication which, in turn, leads to better
    outcome?
  • What is the concomitant substance abuse?
  • Does psychotherapy reduce substance use, which
    in turn leads to improvements in psychotic
    symptoms?

59
References Mediation Effect Moderation in
Psychological Treatment Trials
  • Methodology for IV methods with mediation
  • Emsley RA, Dunn G White IR (2009). Mediation
    and moderation of treatment effects in randomised
    trials of complex interventions. Statistical
    Methods in Medical Research. In press (available
    online).
  • Maracy M Dunn G (2009). Estimating
    dose-response effects in psychological treatment
    trials the role of instrumental variables.
    Statistical Methods in Medical Research. In
    press (available online).
  • Dunn G Bentall R (2007). Modelling
    treatment-effect heterogeneity in randomized
    controlled trials of complex interventions
    (psychological treatments). Statistics in
    Medicine 26, 4719-4745.
  • Website with downloads
  • http//www.medicine.manchester.ac.uk/healthmethodo
    logy/research/biostatistics/

60
Some Further Reading
  • Ten Have TR, Joffe MM, Lynch KG, Brown GK, Maisto
    SA Beck AT (2007). Causal mediation analyses
    with rank preserving models. Biometrics 63,
    926-934.
  • Gallop R, Small DS, Lin JY, Elliot MR, Joffe MM
    Ten Have TR (2009). Mediation analysis with
    principal stratification. Statistics in Medicine
    28, 1108-1130.
  • Bellamy SL, Lin JY Ten Have TR (2007). An
    introduction to causal modelling in clinical
    trials. Clinical Trials 4, 58-73.
  • Lynch K, Cary M, Gallop R, Ten Have TR (2008).
    Causal mediation analyses for randomized trials.
    Health Services Outcomes Research Methodology
    8, 57-76.
  • Albert JM (2008). Mediation analysis via
    potential outcomes models. Statistics in Medicine
    27, 1282-1304.
  • Jo B (2008). Causal inference in randomized
    experiments with mediational processes.
    Psychological Methods 13, 314-336.
  • Gennetian LA, Morris PA, Bos JM Bloom HS
    (2005). Constructing instrumental variables from
    experimental data to explore how treatments
    produce effects. In Bloom HS, editor. Learning
    More From Social Experiments Evolving Analytic
    Approaches. New York Russell Sage Foundation
    pp. 75-114.
  • MacKinnon DP (2008). Introduction to Statistical
    Mediation Analysis. New York Taylor Francis
    Group.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com