Breast Cancer and Environment Pre-Conference - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 45
About This Presentation
Title:

Breast Cancer and Environment Pre-Conference

Description:

One study found that articles in women's magazines discussing breast cancer ... Newsletters, business correspondence, flyers. Direct Mail ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:40
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 46
Provided by: katherine46
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Breast Cancer and Environment Pre-Conference


1
Strategic Risk Communication Involving
Environmental Health Issues
  • Breast Cancer and Environment Pre-Conference
  • November 8, 2005
  • Michigan State University, Lansing, MI
  • Katherine McComas, PhD
  • Department of Communication
  • Cornell University, Ithaca, NY

2
What is Risk?
  • things, forces, or circumstances that pose
    danger to people or to what they value (Stern
    Fineberg, 1996, p. 215)
  • It encompasses subjective and objective qualities
  • Risk judgments, to some degree, are by-products
    of social, cultural, and psychological influences
    (Slovic, 1999)

3
What is Risk Communication?
  • An iterative process among scientists and
    non-scientists about the assessment,
    characterization, and management of risk.
  • Includes purposeful and unintentional messages
    about risk,
  • Is multi-directional,
  • Encompasses verbal and nonverbal cues, and
  • Occurs at personal, group, organizational,
    community, and societal levels.

4
  • Risk communication enters our lives in a
    multitude of forms, sometimes part of the imagery
    of advertising, sometimes a local corporations
    formal statement, or its failure to say anything,
    sometimes a multi-volumed and impenetrable
    technical risk assessment
  • (Kasperson Palmlund, 1987, as cited in Plough
    Krimsky, 1987)

5
The Challenge of Risk Communication
  • How do we communicate complex science-based
    health or environmental risk information to help
    audiences make the best decisions?

6
The Rub
  • No matter how accurate it is, risk information
    may be misperceived or rejected if those who give
    information are unaware of the complex,
    interactive nature of risk communication and the
    various factors affecting the reception of the
    risk message.
  • (Fessenden-Raden et al., 1987, p. 100)

7
Premise
  • The manner by which information is provided,
  • the structure of arguments,
  • the persuasive nature of the message,
  • the sources used, and
  • the nature of the risk
  • all influence audience response to environmental
    health risk messages.

8
Questions to Consider
  • How complex is the information?
  • What if the science is uncertain?
  • Are unintentional risk messages also being sent?
  • Are there multiple messengers?
  • Do some messages conflict with others?
  • What are the media saying?

9
Mass Media
  • If most people get most of their information from
    the mass media, what are the media saying?
  • One study found that articles in womens
    magazines discussing breast cancer risks tended
    to report incorrect or incomplete information
    (Marino Gerlach, 1999)

10
(No Transcript)
11
(No Transcript)
12
Effective Message Design
  • To communicate risk and design effective
    messages, it is imperative to understand how
  • lay audiences process understand alternative ways
    of characterizing environmental health risk
    assessments, and
  • the role of message structure in influencing
    perceptions and behaviors.

13
Audience Characteristics
  • Past experiences with the topic and information
    sources
  • Prior knowledge of the topic
  • Health of the individual and family members
  • Where relevant, attitudes toward the organization
    viewed responsible for the risk
  • Culture
  • Risk perceptions

14
  • In the face of scientific information, why do
    people behave irrationally?

15
What Do You Fear Most?
  • Driving?
  • Smoking?
  • Flying?
  • Eating?
  • Boating?
  • Guns?
  • Alcohol?
  • Lightning?
  • Big Trucks?
  • Flu?
  • Lightening?

16
Annual Deaths/Risk
  • Smoking? (435,000 deaths)
  • Eating? (365,000 deaths-diet and inactivity)
  • Alcohol? (85,000 deaths)
  • Driving? (42,000 deaths)
  • Flu? (36,000 deaths)
  • Guns? (29,000 deaths)
  • Big Trucks? (5,000 car-truck deaths)
  • Boating? (1,643 deaths)
  • Flying? (176 deaths)
  • Lightening? (43 deaths)

17
Some Factors Influencing Risk Perceptions
  • Can I see it?
  • Will I know if Im exposed?
  • Are the effects immediate?
  • Do scientists know and understand the risks?
  • Can I control my exposure?
  • Can I easily reduce my exposure
  • Is my exposure voluntary?
  • Is it a dreaded risk?
  • Are the risks borne equally or fairly?
  • Does it pose a risk to future generations?
  • Are the risks decreasing or increasing?
  • (Slovic, 2000)

18
Why Does It Matter?
  • Understanding how people evaluate risk may help
    risk communicators predict how concerned people
    may be about a risk.
  • Under some circumstances, risk communicators may
    be able to lessen unnecessary concern by
    emphasizing certain actions that people can take,
    for example, to reduce or control their exposure
    to a risk.

19
Affect and Risk Judgments
  • People judge risk based not only on what they
    think about it, but also on what they feel about
    it.
  • When people associate positive feelings with the
    activity, they view it as less risky, and vice
    versa.
  • When people worry more, they are more likely to
    seek information.
  • (Slovic, 1999 Griffin et al., 1999)

20
Why Does It Matter?
  • Emotions can override other considerations.
  • May help to explain gender and age-related
    differences in risk perceptions.
  • May help to explain why vivid language,
    narratives, or affective-laden imagery strongly
    influence peoples reactions to risk.
  • (Loewenstein, 2001)

21
Optimistic Bias
  • When asked to rate their chances of being harmed
    or experiencing a positive event, people tend to
    rate their chances as above or below average.
  • So, why is this called unrealistic optimism?
  • If these beliefs were not biased, in a
    representative sample, claims of being below
    average risk, for instance, would be balanced by
    claims of being above average risk.
  • (Weinstein, 1989)

22
Why Causes Optimistic Bias?
  • People compare themselves to an incorrect norm.
  • People tend to have stereotypes in mind when they
    think about who is usually at risk from
    something. If they do not fit this stereotype,
    then they will downplay the likelihood of the
    event happening to them.
  • People interpret risk information in a
    self-serving manner.
  • People employ ego-defensive mechanisms to
    downplay their risks.
  • People believe they have more control over a
    situation than they really do.

23
Limiting Optimistic Bias
  • When comparing our chances of being exposed to a
    risk to someone elses, the more like us that
    someone else is, the less we have unrealistic
    optimism.
  • When we perceive less control over our exposure
    to risk, or view this exposure as less voluntary,
    unrealistic optimism decreases.
  • Amount of information people are exposed to about
    a risk, and how salient or meaningful that
    information is to them personally can influence
    optimistic biases.
  • Personal exposure to a risk can reduce
    unrealistic optimism.

24
Why Does It Matter?
  • Unrealistic optimism may hinder efforts to
    promote risk decreasing behavior.
  • People do not think they are at risk or that the
    risks pose much danger to their health and safety.

25
Trust and Source Credibility
  • To what extent do individuals perceive the source
    as trustworthy and/or credible?
  • Can depend on perceived shared values (Siegrist
    et al., 2001)
  • Also influenced by sources perceived
  • Openness
  • Caring/Concern
  • Bias
  • Fairness
  • Expertise
  • (Meyer, 1988)

26
Why Does It Matter?
  • When individuals distrust the source, they
    distrust the information.
  • They also often perceive the risks as more
    severe.

27
When Should Risk Communication Occur?
  • Should we wait until were certain?
  • Proactive vs. reactive risk communication

28
Proactive Risk Communication
  • Calls attention to a risk issue, both potential
    and existing, suggests the agenda for discussion,
    and provides mechanisms for information exchange
  • Disadvantages
  • May alert people to something of which they are
    not aware
  • Advantages
  • May alert people to something of which they are
    not aware
  • May allow for a much more meaningful discussion
    of risk
  • May generate more balanced discussion
  • (Scherer, 1991)
  • Can increase trust

29
Reactive Risk Communication
  • Does not call attention to a particular risk but
    waits until there is already considerable public
    and media attention about a risk issue
  • Advantage
  • Allows the public to vent about the issue
  • Disadvantages
  • Science may be less relevant when issues become
    highly emotionally charged
  • Places communicator in defensive position
  • People may not believe information that is
    delayed
  • People may not have information they need to
    protect their or their familys health and safety

30
Developing Risk Messages
  • Now that youve decided to communicate, what
    should your message include?

31
Message Strategies
  • Persuasive
  • Balanced
  • Narrative
  • Dialectical

32
(1) Persuasive Approaches
  • Typically one-sided approach seeking to convince
    audience to change attitudes and behaviors in a
    particular direction.
  • Sometimes referred to as advocacy approaches.
  • Key factors influencing persuasion include source
    characteristics (e.g., credibility), message
    design, and audience characteristics.
  • Work best where there is scientific consensus and
    social agreement about risks.
  • (Fischhoff, 1999)

33
(No Transcript)
34
(2) Balanced Models
  • Typical of mass media coverage.
  • Often presents multiple perspectives or opinions
    but stops short of advocating a particular
    position
  • thus frequently leaving audiences without
    specific behavioral guidance.
  • Presents all sides as equally as possible and
    then lets individuals make up their mind.
  • Sometimes referred to as journalistic approach
    (minus the editorializing).

35
(No Transcript)
36
(3) Narrative Approaches
  • Stories can personalize the risk, making it seem
    more real
  • Media usually highlight someones story
  • Narrative factors guide audience reaction to the
    messages
  • Stories help disseminate shared values, which may
    promote trust
  • (Greene Brinn, 2003 Siegrist et al., 2001)

37
(No Transcript)
38
(4) Dialectical Models
  • Uses a series of questions and answers to probe
    through possibilities and weigh contradictory
    facts and opinions with a view to their
    resolution.
  • Does not advocate a particular position but tries
    to equip audiences with tools necessary to
    evaluate information.
  • Interactive techniques used to involve the public
    in environmental decision making employ similar
    strategies.
  • (Scherer et al., 1999)

39
(No Transcript)
40
(No Transcript)
41
(No Transcript)
42
Questions to Consider
  • When choosing a message strategies, risk
    communication efforts should ask
  • Who is the target audience?
  • What is the objective of the message?
  • Provide information
  • Promote more critical thinking or informed
    judgments about risk
  • Promote attitude or behavior change
  • Build trust among communicators and audience

43
Additional Message Elements
  • Risk severity
  • More severe risks gain more attention, but can go
    too far
  • Efficacy statements
  • When people believe they have the ability to
    change, and that the change will indeed help,
    they are more likely to adopt protective behavior
    (Witte, 1994).
  • Comparisons
  • Work best when source is trusted and comparisons
    are within the same family of risks (Johnson
    Chess, 2003)
  • Visuals
  • A strong communication tool when used
    thoughtfully (Lipkus Hollands, 1999)

44
Where Should Risk Communication Occur?
Format Channel Advantages Disadvantages
One-on-one House calls, healthcare appointments, internal meetings Direct interaction, Greater control over message and outcomes Limited audience reach
Public Forums Seminars, conferences, industry and public meetings, workshops Direct interaction, Greater control over message Limited audience reach, Less control over outcomes
Mass Mediated (Non-specialized media) Paid ads, press releases, media interviews Wide audience reach Less control over message (except paid ads), one-way, Difficulty assessing effectiveness
Electronic Web sites, 1-800 hotlines, listservs, broadcast emails Wide audience reach, can be interactive Difficultly assessing effectiveness, can be one-way
Direct Mail Newsletters, business correspondence, flyers Wide audience reach Difficulty assessing effectiveness, one-way
45
References
  • Fessenden-Raden, J., Fitchen, J. M., Heath, J.
    S. (1987). Providing Risk Information in
    Communities Factors Influencing What Is Heard
    and Accepted. Science, Technology, and Human
    Values, 12(3 4), 94-101.
  • Fischhoff, B. (1999). Why (cancer) risk
    communication can be hard. Journal of the
    National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25, 7-13.
  • Greene, K., Brinn, L. S. (2003). Messages
    influencing college women's tanning bed use
    Statistical versus narrative evidence format and
    a self-assessment to increase perceived
    susceptibility. Journal Of Health Communication,
    8(5), 443-461.
  • Griffin, R. J., Dunwoody, S., Neuwirth, K.
    (1999). Proposed model of the relationship of
    risk information seeking and processing to the
    development of preventive behaviors.
    Environmental Research, 80(2), S230.
  • Johnson, B. B., Chess, C. (2003). How
    reassuring are risk comparisons to pollution
    standards and emission limits? Risk Analysis,
    23(5), 999-1007.
  • Lipkus, I. M., Hollands, J. G. (1999). The
    visual communication of risk. Journal of the
    National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25,
    149-163.
  • Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K.,
    Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological
    Bulletin, 127(2), 267-286.
  • Marino, C., Gerlach, K. K. (1999). An analysis
    of breast cancer coverage in selected women's
    magazines, 1987-1995. American Journal Of Health
    Promotion, 13(3), 163-170.
  • Meyer, P. (1988). Defining and measuring
    credibility of newspapers Developing an Index.
    Journalism Quarterly, 65, 567-574, 588.
  • Plough, A., Krimsky, S. The emergence of risk
    communication studies Social and political
    context. Science, Technology, Human Values, 12
    (34), 4-10.
  • Scherer, C. (1991). Strategies for communicating
    risks to the public. Food Technology, 45,
    110-116.
  • Scherer, C. W., McComas, K. A., Juanillo, N.,
    Pelstring, L. (1999). Promoting Informed
    Decision-Making The Role of Message Structure.
    Risk Health, Safety Environment, 10, 209-220.
  • Siegrist, M., Cvetkovich, G. T., Gutscher, H.
    (2001). Shared values, social trust, and the
    perception of geographic cancer clusters. Risk
    Analysis, 21(6), 1047-1053.
  • Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics,
    and science Surveying the risk-assessment
    battlefield (Reprinted from Environment, ethics,
    and behavior, pg 277-313, 1997). Risk Analysis,
    19(4), 689-701.
  • Slovic, P. (Ed.). (2000). Perception of risk.
    London Earthscan Publications.
  • Stern, P. C., Fineberg, H. V. (Eds.). (1996).
    Understanding risk Informing decisions in a
    democratic society. Washington, D. C. National
    Academy Press.
  • Weinstein, N. D. (1989). Optimistic biases about
    personal risks. Science, 246, 1232-1233.
  • Witte, K. (1994). Fear Control And Danger Control
    - A Test Of The Extended Parallel Process Model
    (Eppm). Communication Monographs, 61(2), 113-134.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com