Title: Breast Cancer and Environment Pre-Conference
1Strategic Risk Communication Involving
Environmental Health Issues
- Breast Cancer and Environment Pre-Conference
- November 8, 2005
- Michigan State University, Lansing, MI
- Katherine McComas, PhD
- Department of Communication
- Cornell University, Ithaca, NY
2What is Risk?
- things, forces, or circumstances that pose
danger to people or to what they value (Stern
Fineberg, 1996, p. 215) - It encompasses subjective and objective qualities
- Risk judgments, to some degree, are by-products
of social, cultural, and psychological influences
(Slovic, 1999)
3What is Risk Communication?
- An iterative process among scientists and
non-scientists about the assessment,
characterization, and management of risk. - Includes purposeful and unintentional messages
about risk, - Is multi-directional,
- Encompasses verbal and nonverbal cues, and
- Occurs at personal, group, organizational,
community, and societal levels.
4- Risk communication enters our lives in a
multitude of forms, sometimes part of the imagery
of advertising, sometimes a local corporations
formal statement, or its failure to say anything,
sometimes a multi-volumed and impenetrable
technical risk assessment - (Kasperson Palmlund, 1987, as cited in Plough
Krimsky, 1987)
5The Challenge of Risk Communication
- How do we communicate complex science-based
health or environmental risk information to help
audiences make the best decisions?
6The Rub
- No matter how accurate it is, risk information
may be misperceived or rejected if those who give
information are unaware of the complex,
interactive nature of risk communication and the
various factors affecting the reception of the
risk message. - (Fessenden-Raden et al., 1987, p. 100)
7Premise
- The manner by which information is provided,
- the structure of arguments,
- the persuasive nature of the message,
- the sources used, and
- the nature of the risk
- all influence audience response to environmental
health risk messages.
8Questions to Consider
- How complex is the information?
- What if the science is uncertain?
- Are unintentional risk messages also being sent?
- Are there multiple messengers?
- Do some messages conflict with others?
- What are the media saying?
9Mass Media
- If most people get most of their information from
the mass media, what are the media saying? - One study found that articles in womens
magazines discussing breast cancer risks tended
to report incorrect or incomplete information
(Marino Gerlach, 1999)
10(No Transcript)
11(No Transcript)
12Effective Message Design
- To communicate risk and design effective
messages, it is imperative to understand how - lay audiences process understand alternative ways
of characterizing environmental health risk
assessments, and - the role of message structure in influencing
perceptions and behaviors.
13Audience Characteristics
- Past experiences with the topic and information
sources - Prior knowledge of the topic
- Health of the individual and family members
- Where relevant, attitudes toward the organization
viewed responsible for the risk - Culture
- Risk perceptions
14- In the face of scientific information, why do
people behave irrationally?
15What Do You Fear Most?
- Driving?
- Smoking?
- Flying?
- Eating?
- Boating?
- Guns?
- Alcohol?
- Lightning?
- Big Trucks?
- Flu?
- Lightening?
16Annual Deaths/Risk
- Smoking? (435,000 deaths)
- Eating? (365,000 deaths-diet and inactivity)
- Alcohol? (85,000 deaths)
- Driving? (42,000 deaths)
- Flu? (36,000 deaths)
- Guns? (29,000 deaths)
- Big Trucks? (5,000 car-truck deaths)
- Boating? (1,643 deaths)
- Flying? (176 deaths)
- Lightening? (43 deaths)
17Some Factors Influencing Risk Perceptions
- Can I see it?
- Will I know if Im exposed?
- Are the effects immediate?
- Do scientists know and understand the risks?
- Can I control my exposure?
- Can I easily reduce my exposure
- Is my exposure voluntary?
- Is it a dreaded risk?
- Are the risks borne equally or fairly?
- Does it pose a risk to future generations?
- Are the risks decreasing or increasing?
- (Slovic, 2000)
18Why Does It Matter?
- Understanding how people evaluate risk may help
risk communicators predict how concerned people
may be about a risk. - Under some circumstances, risk communicators may
be able to lessen unnecessary concern by
emphasizing certain actions that people can take,
for example, to reduce or control their exposure
to a risk.
19Affect and Risk Judgments
- People judge risk based not only on what they
think about it, but also on what they feel about
it. - When people associate positive feelings with the
activity, they view it as less risky, and vice
versa. - When people worry more, they are more likely to
seek information. - (Slovic, 1999 Griffin et al., 1999)
20Why Does It Matter?
- Emotions can override other considerations.
- May help to explain gender and age-related
differences in risk perceptions. - May help to explain why vivid language,
narratives, or affective-laden imagery strongly
influence peoples reactions to risk. - (Loewenstein, 2001)
21Optimistic Bias
- When asked to rate their chances of being harmed
or experiencing a positive event, people tend to
rate their chances as above or below average. - So, why is this called unrealistic optimism?
- If these beliefs were not biased, in a
representative sample, claims of being below
average risk, for instance, would be balanced by
claims of being above average risk. - (Weinstein, 1989)
22Why Causes Optimistic Bias?
- People compare themselves to an incorrect norm.
- People tend to have stereotypes in mind when they
think about who is usually at risk from
something. If they do not fit this stereotype,
then they will downplay the likelihood of the
event happening to them. - People interpret risk information in a
self-serving manner. - People employ ego-defensive mechanisms to
downplay their risks. - People believe they have more control over a
situation than they really do.
23Limiting Optimistic Bias
- When comparing our chances of being exposed to a
risk to someone elses, the more like us that
someone else is, the less we have unrealistic
optimism. - When we perceive less control over our exposure
to risk, or view this exposure as less voluntary,
unrealistic optimism decreases. - Amount of information people are exposed to about
a risk, and how salient or meaningful that
information is to them personally can influence
optimistic biases. - Personal exposure to a risk can reduce
unrealistic optimism.
24Why Does It Matter?
- Unrealistic optimism may hinder efforts to
promote risk decreasing behavior. - People do not think they are at risk or that the
risks pose much danger to their health and safety.
25Trust and Source Credibility
- To what extent do individuals perceive the source
as trustworthy and/or credible? - Can depend on perceived shared values (Siegrist
et al., 2001) - Also influenced by sources perceived
- Openness
- Caring/Concern
- Bias
- Fairness
- Expertise
- (Meyer, 1988)
26Why Does It Matter?
- When individuals distrust the source, they
distrust the information. - They also often perceive the risks as more
severe.
27When Should Risk Communication Occur?
- Should we wait until were certain?
- Proactive vs. reactive risk communication
28Proactive Risk Communication
- Calls attention to a risk issue, both potential
and existing, suggests the agenda for discussion,
and provides mechanisms for information exchange - Disadvantages
- May alert people to something of which they are
not aware - Advantages
- May alert people to something of which they are
not aware - May allow for a much more meaningful discussion
of risk - May generate more balanced discussion
- (Scherer, 1991)
- Can increase trust
29Reactive Risk Communication
- Does not call attention to a particular risk but
waits until there is already considerable public
and media attention about a risk issue - Advantage
- Allows the public to vent about the issue
- Disadvantages
- Science may be less relevant when issues become
highly emotionally charged - Places communicator in defensive position
- People may not believe information that is
delayed - People may not have information they need to
protect their or their familys health and safety
30Developing Risk Messages
- Now that youve decided to communicate, what
should your message include?
31Message Strategies
- Persuasive
- Balanced
- Narrative
- Dialectical
-
-
32(1) Persuasive Approaches
- Typically one-sided approach seeking to convince
audience to change attitudes and behaviors in a
particular direction. - Sometimes referred to as advocacy approaches.
- Key factors influencing persuasion include source
characteristics (e.g., credibility), message
design, and audience characteristics. - Work best where there is scientific consensus and
social agreement about risks. - (Fischhoff, 1999)
33(No Transcript)
34(2) Balanced Models
- Typical of mass media coverage.
- Often presents multiple perspectives or opinions
but stops short of advocating a particular
position - thus frequently leaving audiences without
specific behavioral guidance. - Presents all sides as equally as possible and
then lets individuals make up their mind. - Sometimes referred to as journalistic approach
(minus the editorializing).
35(No Transcript)
36(3) Narrative Approaches
- Stories can personalize the risk, making it seem
more real - Media usually highlight someones story
- Narrative factors guide audience reaction to the
messages - Stories help disseminate shared values, which may
promote trust - (Greene Brinn, 2003 Siegrist et al., 2001)
37(No Transcript)
38(4) Dialectical Models
- Uses a series of questions and answers to probe
through possibilities and weigh contradictory
facts and opinions with a view to their
resolution. - Does not advocate a particular position but tries
to equip audiences with tools necessary to
evaluate information. - Interactive techniques used to involve the public
in environmental decision making employ similar
strategies. - (Scherer et al., 1999)
39(No Transcript)
40(No Transcript)
41(No Transcript)
42Questions to Consider
- When choosing a message strategies, risk
communication efforts should ask - Who is the target audience?
- What is the objective of the message?
- Provide information
- Promote more critical thinking or informed
judgments about risk - Promote attitude or behavior change
- Build trust among communicators and audience
43Additional Message Elements
- Risk severity
- More severe risks gain more attention, but can go
too far - Efficacy statements
- When people believe they have the ability to
change, and that the change will indeed help,
they are more likely to adopt protective behavior
(Witte, 1994). - Comparisons
- Work best when source is trusted and comparisons
are within the same family of risks (Johnson
Chess, 2003) - Visuals
- A strong communication tool when used
thoughtfully (Lipkus Hollands, 1999)
44Where Should Risk Communication Occur?
Format Channel Advantages Disadvantages
One-on-one House calls, healthcare appointments, internal meetings Direct interaction, Greater control over message and outcomes Limited audience reach
Public Forums Seminars, conferences, industry and public meetings, workshops Direct interaction, Greater control over message Limited audience reach, Less control over outcomes
Mass Mediated (Non-specialized media) Paid ads, press releases, media interviews Wide audience reach Less control over message (except paid ads), one-way, Difficulty assessing effectiveness
Electronic Web sites, 1-800 hotlines, listservs, broadcast emails Wide audience reach, can be interactive Difficultly assessing effectiveness, can be one-way
Direct Mail Newsletters, business correspondence, flyers Wide audience reach Difficulty assessing effectiveness, one-way
45References
- Fessenden-Raden, J., Fitchen, J. M., Heath, J.
S. (1987). Providing Risk Information in
Communities Factors Influencing What Is Heard
and Accepted. Science, Technology, and Human
Values, 12(3 4), 94-101. - Fischhoff, B. (1999). Why (cancer) risk
communication can be hard. Journal of the
National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25, 7-13. - Greene, K., Brinn, L. S. (2003). Messages
influencing college women's tanning bed use
Statistical versus narrative evidence format and
a self-assessment to increase perceived
susceptibility. Journal Of Health Communication,
8(5), 443-461. - Griffin, R. J., Dunwoody, S., Neuwirth, K.
(1999). Proposed model of the relationship of
risk information seeking and processing to the
development of preventive behaviors.
Environmental Research, 80(2), S230. - Johnson, B. B., Chess, C. (2003). How
reassuring are risk comparisons to pollution
standards and emission limits? Risk Analysis,
23(5), 999-1007. - Lipkus, I. M., Hollands, J. G. (1999). The
visual communication of risk. Journal of the
National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25,
149-163. - Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K.,
Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological
Bulletin, 127(2), 267-286. - Marino, C., Gerlach, K. K. (1999). An analysis
of breast cancer coverage in selected women's
magazines, 1987-1995. American Journal Of Health
Promotion, 13(3), 163-170. - Meyer, P. (1988). Defining and measuring
credibility of newspapers Developing an Index.
Journalism Quarterly, 65, 567-574, 588. - Plough, A., Krimsky, S. The emergence of risk
communication studies Social and political
context. Science, Technology, Human Values, 12
(34), 4-10. - Scherer, C. (1991). Strategies for communicating
risks to the public. Food Technology, 45,
110-116. - Scherer, C. W., McComas, K. A., Juanillo, N.,
Pelstring, L. (1999). Promoting Informed
Decision-Making The Role of Message Structure.
Risk Health, Safety Environment, 10, 209-220. - Siegrist, M., Cvetkovich, G. T., Gutscher, H.
(2001). Shared values, social trust, and the
perception of geographic cancer clusters. Risk
Analysis, 21(6), 1047-1053. - Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics,
and science Surveying the risk-assessment
battlefield (Reprinted from Environment, ethics,
and behavior, pg 277-313, 1997). Risk Analysis,
19(4), 689-701. - Slovic, P. (Ed.). (2000). Perception of risk.
London Earthscan Publications. - Stern, P. C., Fineberg, H. V. (Eds.). (1996).
Understanding risk Informing decisions in a
democratic society. Washington, D. C. National
Academy Press. - Weinstein, N. D. (1989). Optimistic biases about
personal risks. Science, 246, 1232-1233. - Witte, K. (1994). Fear Control And Danger Control
- A Test Of The Extended Parallel Process Model
(Eppm). Communication Monographs, 61(2), 113-134.