Title: Cognitive Neuropsychiatry:
1Cognitive Neuropsychiatry The Normal Mind and
Brain Through the Lens of Disorder
Vaughan Bell
Departamento de PsiquiatrÃa, Universidad de
Antioquia
Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London
2Outline
- Cognitive neuropsychology
- Cognitive neuropsychiatry
- Delusions
- Hysteria
- Conclusions
3Phineas Gage
- A railroad worker in Vermont, known for his good
character and responsible attitude. - Suffered an injury in 1848 where a tamping iron
was shot through his head when setting gunpowder
to break rocks. - He did not lose consciousness and he walked home.
- He was later seen by Dr Harlow who wrote up his
case.
4Skull and Life Mask
5Damasio et al (1994) Reconstruction
6Effect on Gage
Dr Harlow reported He is fitful, irreverent,
indulging at times in the grossest profanity
capricious and vacillating, devising many plans
of future operation, which are no sooner arranged
than they are abandoned.
7Link to Function
- This was some of the first evidence that damage
to specific areas of the brain could affect
personality and behaviour.
- Later Paul Brocas autopsy on a patient with
expressive aphasia found a specific lesion in the
left frontal lobe, now known as Brocas area.
- This suggested language was not single function
and could be linked to certain brain circuits.
8(No Transcript)
9Trench Warfare and Scotoma
10Holmes (1916) Scotoma Lesion Map
11The Modern Era Begins
- Marshall and Newcombe (1966, 1973) ushered in the
new era by studying dyslexia after brain injury. - They used dissociations between different
impairments to understand the cognitive structure
of language. - This approach has continued to the present day,
focusing on impairments in relatively easy to
measure concepts like memory, attention,
language, perception and so on.
12(No Transcript)
13Unknown Lands
- However, cognitive neuropsychology is not so good
at dealing with other aspects of human
psychology, such as - Belief
- Free will
- Intentionality
- Body ownership
- Self-knowledge
- Delusion
- Passivity phenomena
- Hysteria
- Somatoparaphrenia
- Anosognosia
14Cognitive Neuropsychiatry
- So a new field was developed to
- Study how these functions break down to better
understand the normal mind and brain. - To explain mental disorders within models of
normal neuropsychological function. - Typically looks at symptoms rather than diagnoses.
15(No Transcript)
16Freeman et al. (2002) model
- A psychological model of persecutory delusions.
- Almost entirely based on research with idiopathic
psychosis patients. - Using psychometric and cognitive measures.
17Precipitant
Anomalous experiences / arousal
Emotion Beliefs about self, world, others
Cognitive biases
Search for meaning
Selection of explanation
DELUSION
18Precipitant
19Langdon and Coltheart (2000)
- A cognitive model of belief formation.
- Largely based on patients with monothematic
delusions, and often after brain injury. - Using single case and double dissociation method
of cognitive neuropsychology.
20Sensory information
Monitoring
Web of belief
Hypotheses
Prioritised list of explanations
Evaluation
Belief accepted
Belief rejected
21Sensory information
Monitoring
Web of belief
Hypotheses
Prioritised list of explanations
Evaluation
Belief accepted
Belief rejected
DELUSION
22Psychometrics
- Standardised questionnaires designed to reliably
measure which experiences are typical in patients
and the general population - 10 of the general population score above the
mean of psychotic inpatients on - PDI (Peters et al., 2004) a measure of
delusional thinking. - CAPS (Bell et al., 2006) a measure of anomalous
perceptual experiences.
23CAPS Frequency Distribution
24Cognitive Measures
- Bell et al. (2006b) reviewed differences between
delusional patients, other psychiatric patients
and controls - Probabilistic reasoning (data gathering)
- Attributions for causes of events
- Attentional bias
- Attribution of memory source
25After Brain Damage
- Reviews of psychosis after brain damage also
stress the importance of the temporal and frontal
lobes. - Psychosis has been found most commonly after
damage to these areas in studies of - Cerebrovascular accident (Starkstein et al.,
1992) - Tumour (Lisanby et al., 1998)
- Traumatic brain injury (Fujii and Ahmed, 2002)
26Neuroimaging
- Commonly finds frontal / temporal changes.
- The DTI literature indicates pathways connecting
these areas are most commonly abnormal (Kubicki
et al., 2007)
- Functional neuroimaging of delusions typically
implicates frontal and temporal areas (Blackwood
et al., 2001), although results can be task and
delusion specific (e.g. Blakemore et al., 2000).
27Cognitive Modelling
- Computer models of delusions have existed since
Colbys (1975) natural language simulation PARRY. - Recent simulations tend to be based on
connectionist models (Rolls et al., 2008). - These are designed to perform a particular
cognitive task - and are then damaged to simulate the proposed
neurobiological dysfunction. - To see if they produce delusion-like behaviour.
28Neurocognitive Modelling
- We wanted to look at the causative role of the
temporal cortices in anomalous experience. - Used a paradigm from Brugger et al. (1993)
- Demo Here
29Bell et al. (2007)
- In reality, all patterns were completely random.
- Brugger et al. (1993) found that healthy
participants who believe in telepathy were more
likely to see meaningful information in visual
noise. - We did the same experiment, but controlled for
anomalous experience in the 12 participants.
30Bell et al. (2007)
- Used transcranial magnetic stimulation on the
vertex, left and right lateral temporal cortices
before stimulus. - TMS caused no significant effect on reaction time.
31Effect on detect responses
Sig main effect plt 0.05 Sig diff from left at
p lt 0.05
32Hysteria
- Now diagnosed as conversion disorder or
dissociative disorder and typically based on
three main assumptions (Miller, 1999) - Symptoms are not adequately explained by tissue
damage. - The patient has no voluntary control over the
symptom. - They can be caused by the conversion of
psychological distress into physical symptoms.
33Hysteria
- Can involve
- Paralysis
- Amnesia (psychogenic amnesia)
- Blindness / deafness
- Walking / gait problems (atasia-abasia)
- Loss of voice (psychogenic aphonia)
- Seizures (psychogenic non-epileptic seizures)
- and many others
34Hysteria and Dissociation
- The conversion hypothesis was originally an
idea from Ferriar, popularised by Freud, but
lacks evidence. - There is more evidence for dissociation.
- Defined as the unconscious compartmentalisation
of normally integrated mental functions (Janet,
1887 Aybek et al., 2008). - Recent evidence suggests that this
compartmentalisation works by top-down
attentional modulation.
35Neuropsychology of Dissociation
- EEG studies find that early sensory pathways are
intact in hysterical sensory impairment - but signals from higher level perception areas
are abnormal (e.g. Xu et al., 2001). - TMS studies of hysterical paralysis show that
primary motor pathways are intact (e.g. Cantello
et al., 2001). - Neuroimaging typically shows functional decreases
in areas linked to impairment with increases in
prefrontal cortex activity (review in Bell et
al., forthcoming) - Suggesting inhibition at the cognitive level.
36 Demo Here
37Distribution of Hypnotisability
38Hypnosis and Dissociation
- 19th century French neurologist Jean-Martin
Charcot noted the similarities between the
effects of hypnosis and hysteria. - He noted that hypnosis could simulate and treat
hysteria.
- He argued that hysteria occurred due to
functional inhibitions of the motor cortex that
were produced by a form of autosuggestion.
39Hypnosis and Hysteria
- Marshall et al. (1997) increases in frontal
activity with motor cortex decrease in hysterical
paralysis. - Halligan et al. (2000) repeated the study but
with someone with hypnotic paralysis and found
remarkably similar pattern. - Our research group is continuing to investigate
hypnosis as a model of hysteria.
40Hypnosis and Hysteria
- As well as the functional similarities, it seems
people with hysteria are more hypnotisable than
the general population (review in Bell et al.,
forthcoming) - So we can use hypnosis to model hysteria
- and studying high hypnotisable people might give
us a clue to susceptibility to dissociation.
41Imaging State Related Changes
Perfusion imaging using arterial spin labelling
High hypnotisables, hypnotised, absorption
correlation
42In Psychopathology Research
- There is now an increasing interest in hypnosis
as a psychopathology research tool (Oakley,
2006) - For example, used to simulate
- Hysterical paralysis (Halligan et al., 2000)
- Peri-traumatic dissociation (Holmes et al., 2006)
- Delusions of alien control (Blakemore et al.,
2003) - Functional pain (Derbyshire et al., 2004)
43Conclusions
- Cognitive neuropsychiatry aims to understand
mental disorder in terms of models of normal
neuropsychological function. - It also aims to study disorder as a window on to
the normal mind and brain. - This helps us understand the less easily
accessible concepts like belief, free will,
intentionality etc. - We need to integrate phenomenology, psychology,
cognitive science and neuroscience.