Title: Research Methods in Communication Disorders
1Research Methods in Communication Disorders
2V. Research Design Principles
- There are as many research designs as there are
hypotheses to be tested. - Two major classes of research design that have
broad applicability in communicative disorders
research include group designs and single-subject
designs. - For both experimental and descriptive research,
group designs can be classified as
between-subjects, within-subjects, or mixed.
3A. Group Designs
- In group designs, one or more groups of subjects
are exposed to one or more levels of the
independent variable, and the average performance
of the group of subjects on the dependent
variable is examined to determine the
relationship between the independent and the
dependent variables.
4A. Group Designs
- In between-subject designs, different groups of
subjects are compared to each other. - In within-subject designs the same group of
subjects is compared in different situations. - Mixed designs include both types of comparisons
in the same study.
51. Between-Subjects Designs Experimental
- In experimental between-subject designs,
different groups of subjects are exposed to
different treatments or levels of the independent
variable. - The independent variable or experimental
treatment is applied to one group of subjects
(the experimental group) but not applied to
another group of subjects (the control group). - The difference between the performance of the two
groups is taken as an index of the effect of the
independent variable on the dependent variable.
6a. Random Selection Designs
- There are four types of between-subject designs
random selection designs, random assignment
designs, matched group, and natural group
designs. - The natural group design is the most commonly
used in communication disorders research. - With random selection designs, two groups are
randomly selected from the same population. - Variables such as age, gender, and education are
controlled by random selection of subjects.
7b. Random Assignment Designs
- Random assignment designs overcome the difficulty
of access to an entire population. - When only a restricted population of subjects is
available, they can be randomly assigned to one
group or another. - Like the random selection procedure, subject
variables are controlled by allowing them to vary
randomly. - Except for the population from which subjects are
selected, there is no difference between random
selection and random assignment designs.
8c. Matched Group Designs
- The subject variability that may be a problem in
random assignment designs can be overcome by
matched group designs. - In a simple matched group design, one or more
variables that may affect the dependent variable
can be held constant between groups by matching
the groups on those variables. - The two groups can be matched on the dependent
variable prior to treatment. - The two groups can be matched also on the
independent variable prior to training.
9c. Matched Group Designs
- When matching to either a dependent or
independent variable, it is important that the
groups have the same mean and distribution of the
scores, or ages, around the mean. - Restrictions in the range of variation of matched
variables will enhance the observed effects of
the independent variable.
10d. Naturalistic Group Designs
- In this design, the groups are selected from two
different populations. - In the previous designs, the two groups were
selected from the same population and the
independent variable was different treatment of
the two groups. - In natural group design, the independent variable
is a difference between groups created by
nature that exists prior to the selection of the
groups. - The effect of this independent variable is
studied.
11e. Combined Natural Group and Matched Group Design
- In studies in which communication disorder groups
are compared with normal control groups, the
natural group design must be used because the two
groups come from different populations. - This is also true of comparisons between
communication disorder groups that differ in type
or severity of disorder, or in other variables
such as age and gender.
12e. Combined Natural Group and Matched Group Design
- Natural/matched group designs can also be used to
compare two groups of communication disorders
where the natural groupings are defined by
differences in experience. - Some limitations of natural/matched group designs
are that matching groups restricts the
applicability of the results to groups with the
same characteristics, it is difficult to match
natural groups on all relevant variables, and a
difference between natural groups does not prove
that the independent variable has caused the
difference.
132. Between-Subjects Designs Descriptive
- Between-subject designs are also common in
descriptive research. - In descriptive between-subject designs, different
groups of subjects are compared with each other
with regard to their performance on some
criterion variable. - Examples of between-subject descriptive research
include comparative research, cross-sectional
developmental research, and surveys that compare
the responses of different groups.
142. Between-Subjects Designs Descriptive
- Comparative research involves the description of
dependent variable differences between groups of
subjects who differ with respect to some
classification variable (e.g., children with
palatal clefts vs. children without palatal
clefts). - Cross-sectional development research uses a
between-subject design because separate groups of
subjects who differ with respect to age are
compared.
152. Between-Subjects Designs Descriptive
- Some surveys are conducted for the purpose of
comparing the interview or questionnaire
responses of subjects who fall into different
classifications (e.g., hearing-aid users vs.
nonusers). - Between-subject descriptive research designs may
be bivalent, in which cases the classification
variable is broken down into two mutually
exclusive categories (e.g., laryngectomees vs.
speakers with normal larynges).
162. Between-Subjects Designs Descriptive
- Between-subject descriptive designs can also be
multivalent, in which case the classification
variable is divided into categories that are
ordered along some continuum (e.g., mild vs.
moderate vs. severe hearing loss). - Between subject descriptive designs can also
include comparisons of subjects who are
simultaneously categorized with respect to more
than one classification variable (e.g., male vs.
female mild vs. moderate vs. severe mental
retardation).
172. Between-Subjects Designs Descriptive
- The first step in between-subject descriptive
research is to define criteria for selecting
subjects from each category of the classification
variable. - Classifications must be constructed that are
mutually exclusive, that is, subjects should fall
into only one category with regard to each
classification variable. - Sometimes it may be necessary to use several
measures in a battery of selection test in order
to classify subjects.
182. Between-Subjects Designs Descriptive
- The second step to between-subject descriptive
research is the attempt to equate subjects on
extraneous variables. - Because subjects cannot be assigned randomly to
the various classification, equivalence of groups
on all extraneous variables is quite difficult to
achieve. - The best alternative is to try to minimize group
differences on extraneous variables known to
correlate with the dependent variable.
191. Between-Subjects Designs Descriptive
- A common way to do this is to match the various
groups on the extraneous variables known to be
most highly correlated with the dependent
variable. - Both overall matching or pair matching can be
used.
202. Within-Subjects (Repeated Measures) Designs
- In repeated measures design, the levels of the
independent variable are varied within a single
group of subjects. - They are used in communication disorders research
when there are not enough subjects available for
two independent groups, when it is difficult to
match relevant variables in two independent
groups, or when it is more efficient to carry out
the experimental procedures with one group.
212. Within-Subjects (Repeated Measures) Designs
- The basic rules of the within-subjects design are
to assess the dependent variable twice in a
single group of subjects. - The difference between two assessments
demonstrates the effect of the independent
variable. - Subject variables such as age, gender, and
education do not have to be controlled because
the same group is used of both values of the
independent variable.
222. Within-Subjects (Repeated Measures) Designs
- Limitations of repeated measurement designs
include order effects. - Order effects may take the form of a practice
effect that improves performance or a fatigue or
boredom effect that impairs performance. - When repeated measurement designs are used to
assess the effects of training, there is a lack
of control for the possibility that improvement
might have occurred without training.
232. Within-Subjects (Repeated Measures) Designs
- A control procedure for order effects is to
counterbalance the order of presentation. - However, counterbalancing cannot be used to
control for order effects in training studies
because the pretraining must always be the first
measure. - To control for the possibility of improvement
without training, the independent group and
repeated measure designs can be combined, with a
trained and untrained group tested before and
after training.
243. Between-Group and Within-Subjects (Mixed)
Designs
- In many research studies, more than one
independent variable is considered. - The effects of two or more independent variables
on a dependent variable may be examined. - In other cases, one independent variable is
studied with a between-subjects comparison, and
the other independent variable is studied with a
within-subject comparison. - This mixed design incorporates both the
between-group and the within-subjects tactics.
25B. Single-Subject Designs
- Single-subject designs focus on the individual
behavior of subjects rather than considering the
average performance of a group of subjects. - Single case research designs are appropriate for
communication disorders research because they are
intended to demonstrate that interventions cause
changes in behavior. - Single-subject designs can examine the behavior
of more than one person, but the data of each
person will be evaluated individually rather than
as part of a group average. - A number of different designs are available.
261. ABA, ABAB Designs
- The most basic single subject designs are
withdrawal and reversal designs. - After several baseline (A) measures, a treatment
is given until the target behavior (B) changes. - Then the treatment is taken away (withdrawal
design) or nontarget behaviors are reinforced
(reversal design) until the target behavior
returns to baseline, and the treatment (B) is
usually given once more. - This procedure is designed to prove that the
treatment caused the change in behavior.
271. ABA, ABAB Designs
- When there is baseline, treatment, and either
withdrawal or reversal , it is an ABA design. - When the treatment is is given again after the
withdrawal or reversal, it is an ABAB design,
which provides more complete proof of the
effectiveness of treatment. - These designs should only be used in
communication disorders research only when
treatment effects may be temporary or can be
reversed, and may be ethically inadvisable.
282. Multiple Baseline Designs
- Another way to demonstrate the effect of a
treatment is the multiple baseline design. - Treatment effects are first demonstrated for one
dependent variable, and then for two or more
additional dependent variables. - The additional variables can be target behaviors,
conditions, or subjects.
292. Multiple Baseline Designs
- In the multiple baseline design across behaviors,
three or more target behaviors (e.g., the
articulation of three different phonemes) are
selected. - Baseline measures (A) are taken for all three
behaviors. - Then treatment of the first target behavior (B)
is begun, while baseline measures are continued
for the other two behaviors.
302. Multiple Baseline Designs
- When the treatment effect for B has reached the
desired level, treatment of the second target
behavior (C) is begun, while baseline measures
are continued for the other behavior. - Finally, when target behavior C reaches the
desired level, training of a third target
behavior (D) can be begun, and continued until it
reaches the desired level. - If the baseline behaviors change only when the
appropriate treatment is introduced, there is
evidence of a causal relationship.
312. Multiple Baseline Designs
- In the multiple baseline design across
conditions, a single target behavior is trained
in three or more different training conditions. - For example, the treatment of nonfluencies might
be carried out in a research laboratory (B), at
home (C), and in a public place (D). - The sequence of changes from baseline measures
(A) to the three conditions follows exactly the
same sequence as those in the multiple baseline
design across behaviors.
322. Multiple Baseline Designs
- In the multiple baseline design across subjects,
a baseline (A) is established for three or more
subjects (B, C, and D), and then the treatment of
a target behavior is introduced at different
times for the subjects, following the sequence
described for the multiple baseline design across
behaviors. - The multiple baseline design across subjects is
frequently used in communication disorders
research.
333. Multiple Treatment Designs
- The effects of two or more different treatments
can be compared with several single subject
designs. - In some cases the treatments are given one after
the other, and in other cases the treatments are
trained at the same time. - Â The simple ABAB design can be extended to ABABC,
ABABAC, ABABCD, and so forth, where new
treatments are introduced after training with the
first treatment.
343. Multiple Treatment Designs
- Such designs are called multi-treatment designs.
- The usual multi-treatment design involves a
preplanned comparison of methods. - Baselines can be taken between each treatment
(ABACAD), and a theoretically optimal sequence of
different treatments can be presented, such as
training in imitating speech sounds followed by
training in naming the speech sounds in words
represented by pictures.
353. Multiple Treatment Designs
- The alternating treatment design, also called the
multiple schedules design, presents the
treatments (usually only two) in each session in
counterbalanced order, or in alternating
sessions. - It is not necessary to take baseline measures
because the treatment effects are compared, but
it is advisable to take the baseline measures to
demonstrate the magnitude of the effects. - The simultaneous treatment design, also called
the concurrent schedule design, is difficult to
conceptualize. Â
363. Multiple Treatment Designs
- The difficulty with multiple treatment designs in
communication disorder research is assessing the
possible carryover effects from one treatment to
another in the treatment of nonreversible
behaviors. - This difficulty may be overcome by using
different target behaviors for each treatment,
e.g., treatment B for one misarticulated phoneme
and treatment C for a second misarticulated
phoneme.
374. Generalization Designs
- In multiple baseline designs across behaviors and
conditions, generalization from one target
behavior to another or from one condition to
another is undesirable, because the baselines for
the untrained behaviors or conditions will
change. In practical training studies, however,
it is hoped that training effects will not be
confined to the exact targets used in training,
but will generalize to nontrained behaviors
(e.g., a phoneme correctly articulated in a set
of training words will be articulated correctly
in non-trained words).
384. Generalization Designs
- There are several ways of assessing
generalization to nontrained behaviors. - One simple method is to probe nontrained
behaviors during baseline, at intervals during
training, and after training. - If training is done in a laboratory setting, it
is important to assess generalization, that is,
carryover, to a normal conversational setting.
39B. Single-Subject Designs
- Single subject designs have some of the
advantages of group designs and some of the
advantages of observational and case study
designs. - They provide direct quantitative measures of the
behaviors studied, being averaged neither across
subjects in groups nor across studies. - Experimental conditions are rigorously controlled
to obtain information about causal relationships
between independent and dependent variables.
40B. Single-Subject Designs
- Large groups of subjects are not needed.
- Information is obtained for individuals rather
than groups. - The information may have direct practical
applications. - Single subject designs have their own inherent
limitations. - Stable baselines may be difficult to establish.
- If the treatment is not immediately effective in
changing the baseline behavior, it may be
difficult to demonstrate causal relationships.
41B. Single-Subject Designs
- Reversal designs cannot be used if treatment
effects do not or should not reverse. - Multiple baseline designs cannot be used if
treatment effects generalize to nontreated
behaviors. - Multiple treatment designs may yield ambiguous
results if treatment effects carry over and if
differences between treatment effects are not
clear-cut.
42B. Single-Subject Designs
- The very rigid specification of target behaviors,
treatments, and control procedures may make the
treatment too artificial for direct application
to clinical intervention. - The treatment may change the target behaviors
only in the experimental condition and not in
natural communication situations. - Finally, there is the problem of generalizing the
results. - There is no way of predicting that all subjects
of the same type will show the same treatment
effects. - Â
- Â