Title: What Difference Does Early Childhood Education Really Make
1What Difference Does Early Childhood Education
Really Make?
- State Partnership Conference
- Alexandria, Virginia
- October 20, 2003
- Frances Campbell
- Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute
- University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
2Why Should the Maternal and Child Health Bureau
be Concerned About Early Childhood Education?
- Goals for Healthy People 2010
- Special needs of children are a prime concern
- Developmental disabilities
- High school graduation
3Americas children are increasingly cared for
outside their homes
- Over the last century, the proportion of children
who are growing up in 232-parent families where
only one parent works outside the home has fallen
dramatically. - Welfare reform increases the need for quality
childcare among poor families.
4Family Types for Children 0-17, 1900-2000
5Poverty among some minority families is increasing
- Minority families tend to be over-represented
among low-income families. - Of minorities, only African Americans showed an
increase in poverty rate from 2002 to 2003 (now
24). - For many years, state and Federal governments
played a role in providing some basic health
programs for young children, such as
immunizations. - We have been slower to agree on the need to
provide quality childcare and early childhood
education.
6Statement of the problem
- Individuals who grow up in poverty are at high
risk for developmental delays, school failure, a
lesser degree of economic self-sufficiency, and
more socially maladaptive behaviors.
7Why worry about educating very young children?
- Scientists increasingly believe that the early
years are very important for childrens cognitive
development. - It may be that if opportunities for early
stimulation are missed, children will have a much
harder time catching up.
8Can we combine the need for group care and early
education?
- The need for group care gives an opportunity to
stimulate cognitive growth for young children. - Enhancing the educational value of the early
environment of poor children should increase the
degree to which they develop to their full
potential and lead productive lives. - How could we find out if an early childhood
program helps?
9Statement of the problem
- For a variety of reasons, the early years are
believed to be the most efficacious period to
intervene in the lives of poor children. - Development appears to be more malleable in the
early years. - Children who arrive at kindergarten lacking basic
readiness skills tend to fall further behind in
later years. - Massive attempts to prepare poor children for
school success, such as Head Start, were
initially disappointing. - Research was needed to learn whether intensive
early intervention that began in the infancy
period could make more lasting difference for
poor children.
10Why is the Abecedarian study of interest?
- Attempted to ameliorate or prevent a progressive
decline in intellectual test performance of young
children at risk - A very intensive early childhood intervention
program - Randomized trial
- Low attrition
- Periodic intellectual assessments over 21-year
span
11Acknowledgements
- Thanks are due the Maternal and Child Health
Bureau for supporting important follow-up phases
of the Abecedarian study. - NICHD provided primary primary support of the
early childhood and adolescent phases of this
research. - Other sources of support for earlier follow-up
work include the David and Lucile Packard
Foundation and the Office of Educational Research
and Improvement.
12Original Investigators
- Craig Ramey, Ph. D., Principal Investigator
- Joseph Sparling, Ph. D., Investigator
- Albert Collier, M. D., Investigator
- Frank Loda, M. D., Investigator
- Floyd Denney, M. D., Investigator
- Barbara Wasik, Ph. D., Investigator (CARE)
13Eligibility
- Recruitment in community agencies serving poor
women - High Risk Index
- Other criteria
- Healthy newborn child
- Living within commuting distance of FPG
- Likely to remain in area
14Who was invited to take part?
- 120 families invited to enroll
- 8 refused random assignment
- 2 infants reassigned at insistence of authorities
- 1 ineligible due to biological condition
(seizure disorder with moderate MR)
15Participants
- 4 cohorts of children born between 1972 and 1977
- Half randomly assigned to preschool education
program, half were controls - Original Sample N Males Females
- Treated 57 29 28
- Control 54 23 31
- Total 111 52 59
16Study Design
EE
E
EC
29 Males 28 Females
R
23 Males 31 Females
CE
N111
C
CC
Follow-up Assessments
Preschool Treatment Ages 0-5
Preschool Treatment Ages 0-5
School-age Treatment Ages 5-8
Age 12
Age 15
Age 21
17Educational Intervention
- Very intense
- Full day childcare program
- 5 days/week
- Year round
- Began in infancy (mean entry age 4.4 months,
range 6 weeks to 6 months) - 5 years, until kindergarten entry
- University-based setting
- Medical care on site
- Stable staff
- Low adult child ratios
- 1 3 infants
- 1 45 toddlers
- 1 7 preschoolers
18Preschool Curriculum
- Eclectic in nature
- Stressed contingently responsive early
environment for infants - Learningames for the First Three Years
(Sparling Lewis, 1979, recently reissued) - Natural part of infants or toddlers day
- Learningames for Threes and Fours A Guide to
Adult and Child Play (Sparling Lewis, 1984)
19Preschool Curriculum
- Emphasis
- Language, especially pragmatic language
- Preliteracy activities
- Fine motor development
- Social development
To see samples of the methods, play the video
clips below .
Showing a scarf
Showing a roll
Placing a block
Choosing a spoon
Saying a word
20Preschool Results
- Tests of cognitive development constituted major
outcome during early years - Infants equivalent at outset (3 month Bayley MDI
scores) - Cognitive measures
- Bayley Scales of Infant Development
- 318 months
- Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (Form LM, 1972
norms) - 24 years
- Wechsler Preschool Primary Scale of
Intelligence - 5 years
21Preschool Test Scores
22School-Age Program
EE
E
EC
R
CE
C
CC
Follow-up Assessments
Preschool Treatment Ages 0-5
Preschool Treatment Ages 0-5
School-age Treatment Ages 5-8
Age 12
Age 15
Age 21
23School-Age Program
- Home-School Resource teacher (first three years
of school) - Consultation with classroom teacher
- Individualized curriculum items based on needs
- Encouragement of parental involvement with home
activities - Liaison between home school
- Family support as needed
24School-Age Results
- Reading Scores
- Strong preschool effect on reading scores at age
8 (plt.01) - Significant trend for EEgtECgtCEgtCC (plt.05)
- No effect for school-aged treatment alone
25(No Transcript)
26School-Age Results
- Trend for preschool effect on mathematics scores
(plt.10) - Linear trend in mathematics scores not
significant - No effect for school-age treatment alone
27(No Transcript)
28Long-Term Effects
EE
E
EC
R
CE
C
CC
Follow-up Assessments
Preschool Treatment Ages 0-5
Preschool Treatment Ages 0-5
School-age Treatment Ages 5-8
Age 12
Age 15
Age 21
29Long-Term Effects
- Intellectual measures in the follow-up
- Age-appropriate Wechsler scales administered
- Full Scale IQ used in plots
- Examiners unaware of earlier treatment/control
status - Because school-age treatment had no effect,
reverted to two-group model
30Long-Term Effects on intellectual development
31Long-Term Effects, continued
- Treated children earned higher scores across time
- Treatment/control group difference was greater
during the early, treatment years - Slopes differ in treatment/post-treatment phases
- Treated children differed from control children
in rates of change during treatment years but not
during post-treatment years - Both groups showed upward trends during the early
years and declines in post-treatment years - Up to young adulthood, the group with early
treatment maintained an advantage over controls.
32 Can we identify mediators of long-term treatment
effects on cognitive development?
- Early task orientation mediated effects of early
treatment on test scores but effect size did not
show much change when this factor was entered
into the model. - Early verbal development accounted for much of
the treatment effect on test performance, and in
later years, wholly accounts for it.
33Long-Term Effects, continued
- Adding child and family characteristics to the
prediction model - No significant effect for child gender
- No significant treatment x gender interaction
- Gender x time2 interaction reflects complex
pattern of change in intellectual test
performance of males and females over time
34Long-Term Effects, continued
- Females change more rapidly in early childhood
- Females decline more sharply than males in early
adolescence - Males decline more sharply than females in later
adolescence
35Long-Term Effects, continued
- There is also a main effect for the HOME score
- Main effect is moderated by a HOME x age
interaction - Effect of HOME is stronger in the early years
- Parental attitudes in early life did not have a
significant effect on intellectual test
performance. - No significant effects of mothers marital status.
36Long-Term Effects, continued
- Does a modest effect on IQ test performance
matter? - The evidence indicates that the significant
long-term effect of treatment on academic
performance was mediated by its effect on early
cognitive performance.
37Long-Term Effects, continued
- Does a modest effect on IQ performance really
matter? - Early treatment was associated with significantly
higher scores on reading from age 8 to age 21
38Long-Term Effects, continued
- Does a modest effect on IQ matter?
- Early treatment was associated with significantly
higher scores on math from age 8 to age 21
39Long-Term Effects, continued
- Real-life benefits in young adulthood
- Treated group attained more years of education
- Treatment associated with increased likelihood of
attending a 4-year college or university
40Long-Term Effects, continued
- Teenaged parenthood was less likely for those
having preschool treatment.
41What was the early intervention worth?
- Cost-benefit study was carried out at the
National Institute of Early Education Research
(NIEER) at Rutgers University - Leonard Masse and Steven Barnett
42Cost of Abecedarian Program Compared with Others
(1999 Dollars)
43Ratio of Costs to Benefits
- NIEER researchers estimated that the Abecedarian
program will save society, on average, 4 for
every dollar spent.
44Why would it save money?
- Individuals who had early childhood education are
projected to earn, on average, roughly 143,000
more over their lifetimes than preschool
controls. - Mothers of children who had the free preschool
program are expected to earn about 133,000 more
over their lifetimes (preliminary figures).
45Why would it save money? (continued)
- School districts can expect to save about 11,000
per child because of reductions in the need for
special or remedial education services. - There appeared to be a reduction in smoking among
those with early childhood education, leading to
better health and longer lives (saving
approximately 164,000 per person).
46NIEER Conclusions
- The Abecedarian program effected significant
savings to society. - The estimate may be conservative because of the
community where the children grew up relatively
affluent and low crime. - Benefits of a similar analysis for the Perry
Preschool study estimate 7 saved for each dollar
spent. This was due largely to a significant
reduction in criminal involvement among treated
individuals.
47Questions and conclusions
48Questions and conclusions
- Intellectual test performance can be enhanced
through early childhood programs. - Modest gains are sustainable
- Treatment/control differences narrowed but did
not converge. - Even at the last stage, the difference, although
modest, was significant. - There is a practical significance to even modest
intellectual development - Students who received preschool treatment
attained more years of education. - They were more likely to attend college or
university. - They stand a better chance of holding good jobs
as adults. - Teenaged pregnancy was reduced among the
treatment group. - .
49Questions and conclusions
- Early treatment was responsible for these gains
because - Infants were randomly assigned to the treatment
and control groups.. - There is no evidence the members of the groups
differed to begin with. - Families were from the same communities, and the
children attended the same school system in
public school. - Attrition was low. Most of those lost to
treatment during the early years have been
included in the follow-up, in their original
groups, so the current analyses are conservative
tests of the treatment effect.