Title: PowerPoint Poster Template
1 Mania Vulnerability and Chronic Positive
Emotional Responding June L. Gruber1, Sheri
Johnson,2 Dacher Keltner,1 Christopher
Oveis1 1University of California, Berkeley,
2University of Miami
- INTRODUCTION
- A Disorder of Emotion Bipolar Disorder (BPD) is
a disorder defined by episodes of mania whose
core symptoms involve abnormally and
persistently elevated or expansive mood or
irritability (APA, 2002). - Two Models of Positive Emotion Disturbance
Individual differences in emotional responding
can be differences in the magnitude of reactions
to affect-relevant events (i.e., an
affective-reactivity view Tellegen, 1985) or a
result of differences in baseline levels of
distinct emotions (i.e., a tonic-level view).
Applying this to mania, high levels of positive
affect could be a result of either (1) increased
response to a positive stimulus specifically
(affective-reactivity view) or as (2) chronically
increased levels of positive affect observable
regardless of whether positive, negative, or
neutral stimuli are present. - Empirical Evidence Limited It is unclear which
of the two models best fits mania. Support for
the affective-reactive view suggests that people
with, or vulnerable, to BPD exhibit increased
confidence after success (Ruggero Johnson, in
press Stern Berrenberg, 1979), increased
physiological reactivity (Sutton Johnson,
2002), neural activity in regions implicated in
emotion processing (Yurgelun-Todd et al., 2002
Lawrence et al., 2004) to positive stimuli.
Support for the tonic-view stems from
experience-sampling studies suggesting higher
levels of trait positive mood (Lovejoy
Steuerwald, 1995).
EMOTIONAL STIMULI FILM CLIPS
Which Positive Emotions are Chronically Elevated?
HAPPY
DISGUST
PRIDE
SAD
NEUTRAL
Measures of Emotional Responding
PHYSIOLOGICAL RECORDING
SELF-REPORTED EMOTION
FACIAL BEHAVIOR
To what extent did you experience each of the
following emotions while you were watching the
clip? 1 (not at all) ? 5 (very much)
Used EMFACS (Ekman Friesen,1978) to code
presence and intensity of emotion prototype
displays.
Approach, self-focused and not other-oriented
prosocial emotions elevated in high risk group.
-
- Model of Chronic (tonic) Positive Responding
Supported The study of emotional irregularities
in clinical disorder informs our knowledge as to
both basic emotional processes and mechanisms
involved in clinical disorders (e.g., Keltner
Kring, 1998). Data provides support for a
tonic-level view (e.g., Gross, Sutton,
Ketelaar, 1998) of positive valence responding
across experiential and physiological channels.
This is in contrast with previous studies
suggesting specific reactivity to success or
reward (Ruggero Johnson, in press Stern
Berrenberg, 1979). - Positive Emotion Specificity Specific positive
emotions involved, however, are those that appear
tied to self-focused pleasurable states
associated with pursuit of reward , consistent
with research suggesting that goal striving
behaviors are linked to and predict the onset of
manic symptoms (Lozano Johnson, 2001). This is
also consistent with models of mania suggesting
an overactive behavioral activation system (Depue
et al., 1985). - Vagal Tone and Positive Emotion Association The
current study is one of the first studies to
associate elevated parasympathetic activity
(i.e., vagal tone) with positive emotionality,
supporting a growing body of literature
implicating the association between elevated
vagal tone and dispositional positive emotional
experience (Oveis et al., 2005), resiliency to
stress (Fabes Eisenberg, 1997 Porges, 1995),
and environmental engagement. - Considerations Our findings should be
interpreted with the following considerations
(1) We utilized an analog sample of students at
risk but not diagnosed with bipolar disorder,
which may limit the magnitude of findings. (2)
Film clips are a passive means of inducing
emotion, thus paradigms that involve tasks that
engage a person in the pursuit of a goal or
incentive, may produce stronger effects
(Dickerson Kemeny, 2004).
DISCUSSION
- TESTING TWO COMPETING MODELS
- Chronic Positive Responding (Tonic-Level View)
The premise that mania is reflected by
chronically elevated levels of positive affect
(e.g., Lovejoy Steuerwald, 1995) would predict
that the high risk group would exhibit greater
reactivity to positive emotion film stimuli to
positive, negative, and neutral stimuli across
measures of experiential, behavioral, and
autonomic functioning. in response to the happy
and pride film stimuli - Enhanced Positive Reactivity (Affective-Reactivity
View) Based on the premise that mania is
associated with increased reactivity to positive
or rewarding stimuli (e.g., Meyer et al., 2001
Stern Berrenberg, 1979 Sutton Johnson,
2002), this would suggest that the high risk
group would exhibit greater emotional reactivity
to positive stimuli, across measures of
experiential, behavioral, and autonomic
functioning, but not to neutral or negative
stimuli.
BASIC
NEGATIVE EXPRESSIONS Sadness Fear Disgust Distres
s
POSITIVE EXPRESSIONS Happy Amusement Pride
- Cardiac Vagal Tone ( RSA)
- Skin Conductance (SCL)
- Heart Rate (HRA)
SOCIAL/ SELF- CONSICOUS
NEG EMO COMPOSITE
POS EMO COMPOSITE
Intra-Class Correlation Coefficients (Shrout
Fleiss, 1979) ranged from 0.82 to 0.95.
RESULTS Support for Chronic Positive Responding
Results
PARTICIPANTS Students Vulnerable to Mania
We conducted a series of 2 (high or low risk) x 5
(neutral, happy, pride, sad, or disgust film)
repeated-measures MANOVA for self-report,
behavior, and physiology.
HYPOMANIC PERSONALITY SCALE USED STANDARDIZED
CUTOFFS (Eckblad Chapman, 1986 Kwapil et al.,
2000)
HIGHER LEVELS OF SELF-REPORTED POSITIVE EMOTION
ACROSS ALL FILM CLIPS
HIGHER LEVELS OF CARDIAC VAGAL TONE ACROSS ALL
FILM CLIPS
RECRUITED INTO 2 GROUPS
No Significant Interactions or Main Effects for
Positive or Negative Emotional Behavior.
Figure 1 Reported Positive Emotion Across All
Film Clips
HIGH RISK (n 35)
LOW RISK (n 86)
Figure 2 Cardiac Vagal Tone Across All Film Clips
Groups did not differ in age, sex, or ethnic
composition
PROCEDURE
References
- American Psychiatric Association (2002).
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, Fourth Edition, Quick Reference Text
Revision. Washington, DC American Psychiatric
Association. - Eckblad, M., Chapman, L. J. (1986). Development
and validation of a scale for hypomanic
personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95,
214-222. - Gross, J., Sutton, S.K. Keteelar, T. (1998).
Relations between affect and personality Support
for the affect-level and affective reactivity
views. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 24(3), 279-288. - Ruggero, C., and Johnson, S. L. (In press).
Reactivity to a laboratory stressor among
individuals with bipolar I disorder In full or
partial remission. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology. - Lozano, B. L. Johnson, S. L. (2001). Can
personality traits predict increases in manic and
depressive symptoms? Journal of Affective
Disorders, 63, 103-111 - Porges, S.W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive
world Mammalian modifications of our
evolutionary heritage A Polyvagal Theory.
Psychophysiology, 32, 301-318.
SELF-REPORT
All results remain significant even after
controlling for current symptoms.
WATCH 5 FILMS
RECRUIT
FACIAL BEHAVIOR
Group Main Effect for Positive Emotion F(1,
117) 8.75, p lt .01, ?p2 .07 No significant
differences for self-reported negative emotion.
Group Main Effect for Vagal Tone F(1, 114)
4.85, p .03, ?p2 .04 No significant
differences in SCL or Heart Rate.
PHYSIOLOGY
We measured current symptoms of mania (ASRM
Altman et al., 1997) and depression (BDI Beck
Steer, 1993) to ensure any claims we made about
emotional responding were independent of current
symptoms.