Title: Assessing Informational Text Comprehension
1Assessing Informational Text Comprehension
- Nell K. Duke, Katherine R. Hilden, Alison K.
Billman - Michigan State University
- Literacy Achievement Research Center
2Other Project Collaborators
- Juliet Halladay
- Yu Fang
- Yeow Meng Thum
- Yonghan Park
- Shenglan Zhang
- Angela Tanis
- Dipendra Subedi
- Michigan State University
- This project is supported by a grant from the
Carnegie Corporation of New York and by the
Literacy Achievement Research Center at Michigan
State University.
3Some Assumptions About Reading Comprehension
Development
- The foundations of reading comprehension
development are laid long before children can
actually read themselves. - Reading comprehension is related to, but not the
same as, listening comprehension. - Reading comprehension is not automatic even given
strong word recognition. - Many different skills, dispositions, and bodies
of knowledge are entailed in reading
comprehension. - Reading comprehension ability varies by many
factors including by child and by type of text.
4Some Existing Tools for Assessing Informational
Text Comprehension
- Think alouds or verbal protocols
- Can be used with a variety of texts
- Can provide insights into what readers are and
are not thinking about when they are reading
(e.g., level of activity, strategy use,
connections) - Suitability for young children still being
investigated - Procedures, reliability, etc. not entirely
established - Summaries or retellings
- Can provide insights into what children remember
and view as important and their structural
knowledge - A skill in itself
- Procedures, reliability, etc. not established
5Some Existing Tools for Assessing Informational
Text Comprehension
- Informal reading inventories (passages
questions) - Can estimate a childs comprehension level some
have expository text passages - Can indicate whether children have relatives
strengths versus weaknesses in literal versus
inferential questions - Procedures established but leveling often
questionable and reliability usually not
established - Self-evaluation checklists (e.g., When I read I.
. .) - Provide insights into childrens view of their
comprehension - Depends on childrens judgments
- Validity and reliability not established
6Some Existing Tools for Assessing Informational
Text Comprehension
- Norm-Referenced Tests of Comprehension
- Usually do not distinguish informational
comprehension from comprehension of other text
types - Usually cannot fully separate comprehension from
word recognition - Designed to place students on a normal curve
- Generally not very diagnostic
- Procedures and reliability established
- Others
7Some New Tools for Assessing Informational Text
Comprehension
- We have been working on developing three new
tools for assessing informational text
comprehension in the primary grades. - We want these tools to be useful to classroom
teachers, reading specialists, and researchers. - We want these tools to encourage more attention
to informational text in research and practice. - We want these tools to identify aspects of
informational text comprehension worthy of
assessment and instruction.
8About the Assessments We Are Developing
- The assessments are designed to measure five
specific dimensions of infotext comprehension - Comprehension Strategy Use (CS) (Specifically,
activating prior knowledge, predicting,
inferring, summarizing) - Knowledge of Informational Text Features (TF)
- Comprehension of Graphics in the Context of Text
(GCT) - Vocabulary Knowledge Knowledge of Tier II
vocabulary for informational text (VK) - Vocabulary Strategies Ability to ascertain Tier
III word meaning from context (VS)
9Comprehension Strategy Use
- There are differences in good and poor readers
with respect to strategy use (e.g., Duke,
Pressley, Hilden, 2004). - Degree of strategy use related to comprehension
achievement. - The relationship seems to be causal in that
instruction in comprehension strategies has been
shown to improve comprehension (e.g., Duke
Pearson, 2002). - Included inferencing although that is often
spontaneous rather than strategic in the
traditional sense.
10Knowledge of Informational Text Features
- There are differences in good and poor readers
with respect to knowledge some text features
text structure, at least (e.g., Dickson, Simmons,
Kameenui, 1995). - There is some causation at work in that text
structure instruction can improve comprehension
(e.g., Dickson, Simmons, Kameenui, 1995), as
can instruction in searching (using index,
headings, etc.) (Symons, MacLatchy-Gaudet, Stone,
Reynolds, 2001). - Research has not yet established differences in
good and poor readers, or causation, for a number
of other informational text features.
11Comprehension of Graphics in the Context of Text
- It appears that illustrations can have a
facilitative effect on comprehension for at least
some readers, although this does not seem to
divide neatly along lines of good versus poor
readers (see Gyselinck Tardieu, 1999, for a
review). - To our knowledge, it has not yet been shown
whether informational text comprehension can be
improved by instruction in building meaning
through illustrations as well as text. - For now, we are assuming that the ability to
comprehend graphics in the context of text is
important and amenable to instruction.
12Vocabulary Knowledge
- There are differences in good and poor readers
with respect to vocabulary knowledge. - Vocabulary knowledge is related to comprehension
achievement (Blachowicz Fisher, 2000). - The relationship seems to be causal in that
instruction in vocabulary has been shown to
improve comprehension (e.g., Baumann, Edwards,
Boland, Olejnick, Kameenui, 2003). - We focus on knowledge of high-utility vocabulary
(similar to what Beck, McKeown, and Kucan, 2002
call Tier II vocabulary in reference to narrative
text) words (for science text) like compare,
describe, observe, kinds, tools, cycle.
13Vocabulary Strategies
- Research is mixed as to whether good and poor
readers differ in their ability to infer word
meaning from context, or the degree to which
vocabulary strategy use is related to
comprehension achievement. - In at least some studies, instruction in
vocabulary strategies has been shown to improve
comprehension (e.g., Baumann, Edwards, Boland,
Olejnick, Kameenui, 2003). - We focus on the ability to ascertain from context
the meaning of fairly low-incidence or Tier III
words.
14Three Assessments We are Developing
- Concepts of Comprehension Assessment (COCA), for
informational text -- this is the one we will
focus on today - Strategic Cloze Assessment, for informational
text - Performance Task, for informational text
15COCA Quality
- There are three forms of the COCA Frogs,
Butterflies, and Dogs. - Based on administration to first and second grade
students at various points in time in the year - Overall reliabilities are
- Frogs .833
- Monarchs .802
- Dogs .745
- Scores generally higher in March than October and
generally higher for second grade than first. - Unsure yet of relationship between scores and
instruction.
16COCA Quality
- Frogs Reliability by Factors
- CS 0.525
- TF 0.784
- GCT 0.730
- VK 0.837
- VS 0.719
17COCA Quality
- Monarchs Reliability by Factors
- CS 0.757
- TF 0.789
- GCT 0.583
- VK 0.750
- VS 0. 754
18COCA Administration Procedures
- The COCA is individually administered.
- Assessment sessions run 15-20 minutes per child.
- Text and prompts are scripted to facilitate
consistent administration. - Student responses are recorded directly on the
score sheet to facilitate scoring.
19Administration Pointers
- It is important to be consistent across
administrations. - It is important to read as naturally as possible
with good inflection. - It is important to break between the text and the
prompt or question.
20Some Next Steps
- Examine relationship of COCA scores to
instruction - Examine use of COCA with end-of-third grade
children - Make COCA available in the public domain
(msularc.org) - Investigate predictive validity of COCA
- Continue to examine the quality of the other two
assessments we have been developing.