Title: Choosing a Research Question
1Choosing a Research Question
2The Research Process
- Identify Research Question
Choose Method
Experiment
Quasi-experiment Ex post facto
Locate needed instrumentation
Develop Instrumentation
Develop data analytic procedures
Choose data analytic strategy
Implement Study
Communicate Findings
3Type of Research
Where does your research question fit in?
4Developing a research Question
- Questions are the engine that drive the research
enterprise - method, sample, settings, variables all follow.
- Many papers get rejected because they do not have
a discernible question. - An assessment of the questions importance also
determines publication - more than methodology or
presentation style.
5Choice of Question
- Internal Factors
- Curiosity - I wonder what would happen if?
- Compassion -
- Confirmability - easy replication
- Conformity - fad
- Predilection - skill, temperament, contacts or
training
6Choice of Question
- External Factors
- a question determined by someone other than the
researcher (e.g. sponsor) - cost - the data were cheap and easy to obtain
(e.g., AWIRS) - resource availability (opportunism)
- reward system - certain types of research are
rewarded - propinquity - what are the interests of potential
collaborators
7Significance comes from ...
- Activity Contacts with colleagues and
organisations rather than thinking in isolation - Intrinsic interest and motivation excitement and
commitment
8Lack of significance ...
- Expedience cheap, quick and easy
- Lack of interest in the question motivated by
external outcome
9Where do questions come from?
- Theory based questions (13) (highly regarded)
- tests of predictions
- comparison of theories
- Applied problem (3) (not highly regarded)
- problems defined by and limited to a single
organisation or tightly defined problem - Build on existing literature (84) (not highly
regarded) - use different subject population
- different operationalisation of 1 or more
variables - include different levels of a variable studied
previously - simultaneously study variables that have only
been examined independently - include moderators or mediators
- add an additional variable
10The form of the question
- the description of a single variable
- operationalisation of a construct and aspects of
its distribution - (not as common as it should be!)
- the interrelationships between variables
- is there an association between variables?
- is there a causal relationship
- under what conditions are they associated and in
what way (moderator, or mediator) - does variable Z add to the prediction of y after
x has been extracted?
11Choice of methodology
- Often the research question dictates the method
- is it true that 75 of employees are dishonest?
(epidemiology) - I believe that dishonesty is determined by peer
group pressure - are there high rates of
dishonesty among certain types of employees?
(still lacks explanation)
12Choice of methodology2 variables
- Are employees of low moral fibre likely to be
dishonest? - introduces 2 variables if the variables are
measured simultaneously there is little other
than correlations can be done unless one can
induce low/high moral fibre - experimental methods (even quasi) of some kind
are needed to infer causality
13Choice of methodology 3 variables
- Do peers who vary in moral fibre influence
dishonesty amongst individuals who vary in moral
fibre - this introduces a whole new set of issues that
dictates methods and analysis - does peer level of moral fibre influence
dishonesty independently of individual level of
moral fibre - does peer level of moral fibre influence
individual moral fibre which in turn influences
dishonesty (mediation) - is the strength of the moral fibre-dishonesty
relationship differentially influenced by level
of peer moral fibre (moderation) - how well can dishonesty be predicted from moral
fibre of individuals and peers (additive) - Such questions influence the decision on methods
to observe or manipulate, to measure once or
repeatedly
14The unit of analysis
- Should we look at the individual level or at the
unit level, (faculty, department, university) or
at multiple levels - why are there differences among departments?
- are all the departments within a company the
same? - Ecological fallacy (Robinson 1950)
- Simpsons paradox (Simpson, 1951)
15Two Fallacies
- Ecological Fallacy drawing conclusions about
individual people from data that refer only to
aggregates. - For instance, assume that you measured
satisfaction scores of a particular shopfloor and
found that they had the highest average score in
the factory. Later you run into one of the
employees from that group and you think to
yourself "she must be satisfied." Just because
she comes from the group with the highest average
doesn't mean that she is automatically satisfied.
She could be the most dissatisfied in a group
that otherwise consists of satisfied employees - Exception Fallacy Occurs when you reach a group
conclusion on the basis of exceptional cases.
This is the kind of fallacious reasoning that is
at the core of a lot of sexism and racism. The
stereotype is of the guy who sees a woman make a
driving error and concludes that "women are
terrible drivers."
16Critical minimal criteria
- the ultimate goals of the research will be clear
to sponsors and to consumers of the research if
the following are specified - the participants, setting and variables of
interest (extent of generalisation) - the nature of the relationship that is being
investigated and when it is expected to be found
(correlational vs causal) - the form of the relationship (mediation,
moderation) - the unit of analysis (individual, group,
organisation)
17Simpsons Paradox
- Simpson's paradox occurs when an association
between two variables is reversed upon observing
a third variable. - The following data were collected on a
university's admissions to its professional
schools. - Admit Deny
- Male 490 210
- Female 280 220
- Is there a case for discrimination here?
18Are women discriminated against?
- 490/700 males admitted 70
- 280/500 females admitted 56
- However, when we look at the individual schools,
we observe quite a different picture - Business
Law - Admit Deny Admit
Deny - Male 480 120 Male 10 90
- Female 180 20 Female 100 200
19Or is it the men?
- In the business school 80 of males were admitted
and 90 of females. - A higher proportion of females (33) make it to
the law school compared to 10 of men. - We observe the relationship between gender and
admittance is reversed when the individual
schools are considered.
20Simpsons paradox
- Simpson's paradox is a classic example of a
confounding variable. It occurs in this example
because a higher proportion of women apply to the
law school. - a confounding variables is a difference between
the treatment groups besides the treatment itself - The law school is much harder to get into.
- Even though the law school admits a slightly
higher proportion of women, the fact the law
school admits so few people results in a lower
proportion of women being admitted to the
university overall.
21Using proportions to understand
- Business Law
- Male 600 100 (600/70086)
- Female 200 300 (200/50040)
- Confounding variables present difficulty when
they have an effect on the response variable - Admit Deny
- Business 660 140 business school admits
660/80082.5 - Law 110 290 law school admits
110/40027.5 - Simpson's paradox occurs in this example because
women apply more often to the school that admits
few people.
22The sample
- Is an appropriate sample(s) readily available?
- Can they be randomly allocated?
- Is the sample size large or small (could make
tests of significance dubious)?
23The Contextual Constraints
- the design of the study is affected
- which in turn affects the statistics
- which in turn affects the strength of the
conclusions one can draw