Morphology and the mental lexicon - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 40
About This Presentation
Title:

Morphology and the mental lexicon

Description:

– PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:1005
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 41
Provided by: emsah5
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Morphology and the mental lexicon


1
Morphology and the mental lexicon
  • Cogs3000
  • Week 6

2
Outline
  • Introduction an example
  • Morphological decomposition in the mental lexicon
  • Psycholinguistic studies of word structure
  • Cross-modal semantic priming
  • Priming effects of prefixes and suffixes
  • Neuroimaging studies of normal and aphasic
    morphological processing.
  • An example to start with

3
The following word is ambiguous
  • Unbuttonable
  • Capable of being unbuttoned.
  • Not able to be buttoned.
  • How do you assign these respective meanings?
  • By alternative morphological parsings of the
    word
  • (un(button))able
  • un(button(able))
  • What does this example demonstrate?

4
Example demonstrates
  • That words are composed of smaller elements of
    meaning (morphemes)
  • That word meaning assignment is compositional
  • That the way in which morphemes are combined can
    be critical for the meaning of the word
  • (un(button))able un(button(able))
  • Is the difference of meaning between these two
    words simply a function of differences in
    morphological constituency bracketing?

5
Not entirely due to constituent structure
  • There are two distinct meanings for un-
  • un1- ltnotgt e.g. unattainable
  • un2- ltreverse the action ofgt e.g. unfurl, untie,
    unbutton, undo
  • What are the conditions on the use of un2-?
    What does it combine with?
  • Verbs whose actions are reversible.

6
Morphology the syntax of words
  • The unbuttonable example demonstrates the
    compositional route to meaning, but there is also
    the direct route (non-compositional).
  • Consider the compositionality of unravel.
  • ltreverse the action ofgt ravel ??
  • It seems that unravel cannot be treated as
    strictly compositional.
  • This raises a basic question for modelling
    lexical processing.

7
Core questions for language processing at the
lexical level
  • How word meanings are represented in the mental
    lexicon.
  • How lexical meanings are assigned to words in the
    context of sentence processing.
  • The precise nature of the items which make up the
    mental lexicon, which we have thus far identified
    as words, but have not attempted to define with
    any precision.
  • Lets take the 3rd question first.

8
The question of units words and morphemes
(morphological structure).
  • The units of lexical representation, typically
    referred to as lexemes by linguists (Huddleson et
    al., 2002) and as lemmas by psycholinguists
    (Levelt, 1987),
  • Are smaller than words, the units conventionally
    separated by white space in printed text.
  • e.g., cats and cat 2 words, 1 lexeme

9
Lexical decomposition
  • The assumption that in the course of processing
    words for meaning, listeners strip inflectional
    affixes off word forms to access lexical meanings
    (Taft and Forster, 1975).
  • How far does this affix stripping extend?

10
There is a cline of decomposability
  • cats n plural clearly decomposable
  • departed v past
  • departed v adj departed guest
  • departed derived noun the departed
  • not clearly decomposable

11
Formal and semantic compositionality
  • The question of compositionality is complicated
    by this distinction.
  • Morphology is about the syntax of words.
  • Lexical semantics is concerned with the
    compositionality of word meaning.
  • We will deal first with the morphological
    structure of words before we discuss word meaning
    assignment.

12
The dual route model
  • Mapping from the sound structure of a word to its
    meaning may be achieved compostionally (by rule),
  • or by directly matching a word-form in the mental
    lexicon (the lexical route).
  • The compositional route is more likely in the
    case of a verb like departed.
  • the lexical route is the only option for a
    suppletive (completely irregular) form like
    went.
  • There has been much controversy in
    psycholinguistic circles over how morphological
    inflections (such as past tense for verbs and
    plurals of nouns) are acquired and to what extent
    regular and irregular forms are processed by the
    same or different mechanisms in language
    comprehension.

13
Disagreement between Linguists and
Psycholinguists
  • Over extent of morphological decomposition
  • Linguists favour maximal decomposition into root
    morphemes.
  • Psycholinguists usually reluctant to pursue
    lexical decomposition beyond recognizing
    inflectional morphemes and the most productive of
    the derivational morphemes.
  • Language users, they argue, are not etymologists.
  • How do we settle this issue?

14
Classical psycholinguistic investigations
  • (Berko, 1957 Derwing, 1976) used elicitation
    techniques to test the productivity of
    morphological rules.
  • This is a wug. Now there are two of them. There
    are two _____?
  • More recently, repetition priming effects have
    provided evidence that morphological
    decomposition occurs (Marslen-Wilson, et al.,
    1994).

15
Extent of morphological decomposition
  • Most agree The inflectional morphology of a word
    is compositional.
  • English cats, stronger, rained
  • Less agreement as to how far decomposition
    extends to the derivational morphology.
  • government is readily decomposable into govern
    ltrulegt ment ltnoun-makergt
  • argument, discernment, refinement, amazement,
    arrangement
  • Can the paradigm be extended to detriment or
    department?
  • How about apartment ??

16
Connectionist challenge to the dual route model.
  • Rumelhart and McClelland (1986) set the
    connectionist cat among the psycholinguistic
    pigeons.
  • Some pigeons held their ground. The connectionist
    simulations inadequately modelled the course of
    morphology acquisition. (Pinker and Prince,
    1988).
  • Connectionists responded by building more
    sophisticated simulations.
  • Pigeons again temporarily scattered.
  • Today both cats and pigeons occupy the square in
    an unstable misalliance.
  • Is there a key to solving this standoff?

17
Morphological regularity and frequency of word
usage.
  • Low frequency words which take regular
    inflections.
  • Three types of inflection may be distinguished.

What is the rationale for the regularity
frequency trade-off?
18
Rationale for the regularity frequency trade-off
  • Low token frequency forms would take too long to
    retrieve
  • Irregularly inflected forms are phonologically
    economical (shorter).
  • Partially regular forms form families of
    phonologically related members.
  • e.g. keep /kip/ - /kept/, leap, creep, sleep
  • Regularly inflected forms show no family
    resemblances in terms of phonological similarity.

19
A theory of word representation and processing
needs to
  • Account for the way in which the three types of
    morphological inflection are related to token and
    type frequencies.
  • Can a single learning mechanism and
    representational system accommodate all three
    types?
  • Or do we require a dual-route theory, perhaps
    incorporating a race model?
  • Or possibly, three different learning mechanisms
    may be needed.
  • Over to you.

20
Psycholinguistic studies of word structureThe
priming paradigm
  • Priming the facilitatory effect that
    presentation of a word, can have on the lexical
    retrieval of a subsequent word.
  • In a priming experiment, words are usually
    presented to subjects in pairs.
  • The first word, called the prime, is typically
    presented shortly before some target or probe
    word.
  • which may be related to the prime in some way
    phonologically, semantically, or morphologically.
  • Priming effects are usually assessed by measuring
    reaction time to the probe word.
  • Thus in a semantic priming experiment, lexical
    access time for the probe word author should be
    shorter, by a few tens of milliseconds when
    preceded by the related prime book, compared
    with an unrelated control probe word such as
    flower

21
Cross-modal priming task
  • Subjects heard spoken prime words and respond
    with a lexical decision task to visually
    presented probes flashed onto a computer screen
  • The lexical decision task was simply deciding
    whether or not the letter sequence flashed on the
    screen was a real word or a non-word. (In
    approximately half of the experimental trials the
    probe letter sequence was a phonotactically legal
    non-word letter sequence, e.g. glark).
  • It was the morphological relation between the
    prime and the probe word, not any phonological
    similarity between prime and probe that was found
    to matter (Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Waksler and
    Older, 1994)
  • When subjects heard the word friendly spoken at
    the same time as they made a lexical decision to
    the visual probe friend, their responses were
    faster by 40 - 60 milliseconds than to an
    unrelated probe, but when tinsel was the
    auditory prime for the probe word tin, no priming
    effect was obtained.

22
Test conditions and morphological priming effects
Only condition 4 where there is a phonological
similarity between prime and probe but no
morphological relationship failed to yield a
priming effect.
What are the effects of semantic similarities
amongst the prime-probe pairs in this experiment?
Can they be separated from effects of
morphological similarity?
23
Morphological and Semantic relatedness priming
effects
There is no evidence here for an independent
effect of priming by virtue of a morphological
relationship between the prime and probe stimuli.
Marslen-Wilson et al. (1994) do not describe
their results this way. They say that only
morphologically related pairs which are
semantically transparent, or perceived to be
semantically related, yield priming effects.
Evidence for an independent role for
morphological structure comes from the way that
semantic transparency was found to interact with
prefixed versus suffix-derived morphological
constructions.
24
Prefixes behave differently from suffixes in
morphological priming.
Confession fails to prime confessor. But
unfasten primes refasten. Why?
25
Why do suffixed words fail to prime each other?
  • The words confession and confessor are
    semantically related and this semantic similarity
    should yield a priming effect
  • Some countervailing inhibitory effect must be at
    work.
  • This can only come from competition between the
    two derivational suffixes -ion and -or.

26
The cohort model holds the key
  • The suffixes -ion and -or will always be
    competitors as perceptual targets in word
    recognition,
  • when they seek to attach to some verbal stem to
    form a derived noun.
  • The target word must be confession, or
    confessor, or conceivably some other alternative.
  • But it must be one of these alternatives.

27
Lateral inhibition between competitors
  • Lateral inhibition between competitors is
    invoked in TRACE and the Cohort model, to resolve
    competing hypotheses about the identity of an
    input string.
  • We need also to explain why prefixes do not
    mutually inhibit one another.
  • A prefix typically allows numerous stem
    attachments.

28
Conclusions from the priming study.
  • Morphological decomposition was seen to be
    justified, up to a point, on evidence from
    cross-modal semantic priming studies.
  • Morphological decomposition is justified insofar
    as the morphological components of a word are
    semantically transparent.
  • i.e., to the extent that the meaning of the whole
    word can be clearly related to the meanings of
    its component morphemes.
  • e.g., indefensible ltnotgt(ltdefendgt(ltablegt))
  • However, we did not provide an explicit account
    of semantic transparency, other than to appeal
    to language users intuitions about the meanings
    of words.
  • That is the task of a theory of lexical semantics

29
Towards a theory of lexical semantics
  • Cogs3000
  • Week 8

30
Goals of a theory of lexical semantics
  • A theory of lexical semantics needs not only to
    account for the meaning of individual words but
    how word meanings change in context with other
    words.
  • Consider the meaning of good in the phrase good
    friend (ltloyal, reliablegt)
  • Now consider the meaning of the same word in the
    phrase good lover or good meal.
  • There is a chameleon-like quality to the meaning
    of some words, which needs to be accounted for.

31
Lexical semantics and Pragmatic knowledge
  • The meanings of words are said to be stored in
    the mental dictionary,
  • along with their phonological and
    morpho-syntactic features.
  • Lexical knowledge clearly varies from speaker to
    speaker.
  • Languages also vary from one another in how they
    lexicalize our knowledge and perception of the
    world
  • Hence, lexical semantics though intimately
    connected with pragmatics (knowledge of the
    world) is distinct from it.
  • Lexical knowledge is confined to knowledge of
    word meanings and word usage.

32
The problem of polysemy
  • Dictionaries have multiple meanings listed for
    any given word and the more common the word, the
    more meanings it will typically have listed
    against it.
  • Meanings of show that are found in the pocket
    Oxford.
  • The contexts have been scrambled.

33
Semantic anomaly detection
  • Another way we might test the adequacy of a
    theory of lexical semantics.

It is the detection of anomaly in the actual
expression which is of interest to us here,
rather than how the anomalous utterance was
produced.
34
Another test
  • Ability of the system to generate acceptable
    paraphrases for individual words or phrasal
    expressions.
  • To express commonalities and differences of
    meaning between verbal expressions that are
    semantically related.
  • a bottle of wine versus a jug of whiskey

35
A fundamental unresolved problem on how to
approach the analysis of word and phrase meanings
  • Whether semantic theory should be grounded in
    logic or in psychology
  • in some formalized propositional calculus of
    truth conditions for possible worlds,
  • or in perceptual experience and its internal
    cognitive representation.
  • The answer to this long standing controversy may
    lie in the level of analysis adopted.
  • A propositional calculus is clearly more
    applicable to sentence-level semantic analysis.
  • The way we perceive objects in the world is
    clearly of critical interest for the semantic
    properties of words.

36
Semantic Networks
  • One of the earliest attempts to construct a
    computational model of lexical semantics was
    undertaken by Ross Quillian (1968)
  • A system ambitiously dubbed The teachable
    language comprehender(TLC)
  • A program designed to be capable of being taught
    to comprehend English text.
  • The TLC envisaged the lexicon as a semantic
    network encoding both world knowledge and the
    meanings of words.

37
The TLC
  • Quillians semantic network is a symbolic, not a
    connectionist network.
  • The nodes of the network represent concepts or
    words,
  • that are linked by arcs, which represent a small
    number of relational types.
  • The meaning of a word may be expressed, first by
    accessing its root node in the network and
    traversing the network elements to which this
    node is linked in a series of steps.
  • A words meaning is defined by other words, whose
    meanings are defined, in turn, by other words.

38
How the TLC represents the polysemous word plant
These three meanings of plant are disjunctive in
their usage.
The basic meaning of plant (PLANT 1) and pointers
to its alternative meanings (PLANT 2 and PLANT 3)
are expressed in a plane of word memory.
39
Semantic representations in TLC
  • Four basic types of arc linking nodes in a
    semantic network
  • In addition to the basic logical relations of
    disjunction (OR) and conjunction (AND)
  • His system recognized a subordinate-superordinate
    relation, which others have labelled the ISA
    relation and which defines a relationship of
    class membership (e.g., dog - animal).
  • A third type of linkage, which may simply be
    thought of as property attribution links one
    node directly to another, declaring that the node
    to which an arrow points is a property of the
    node from which that arrow points.

40
Testing TLC
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com