Title: Morphology and the mental lexicon
1Morphology and the mental lexicon
2Outline
- Introduction an example
- Morphological decomposition in the mental lexicon
- Psycholinguistic studies of word structure
- Cross-modal semantic priming
- Priming effects of prefixes and suffixes
- Neuroimaging studies of normal and aphasic
morphological processing. - An example to start with
3The following word is ambiguous
- Unbuttonable
- Capable of being unbuttoned.
- Not able to be buttoned.
- How do you assign these respective meanings?
- By alternative morphological parsings of the
word - (un(button))able
- un(button(able))
- What does this example demonstrate?
4Example demonstrates
- That words are composed of smaller elements of
meaning (morphemes) - That word meaning assignment is compositional
- That the way in which morphemes are combined can
be critical for the meaning of the word - (un(button))able un(button(able))
- Is the difference of meaning between these two
words simply a function of differences in
morphological constituency bracketing?
5Not entirely due to constituent structure
- There are two distinct meanings for un-
- un1- ltnotgt e.g. unattainable
- un2- ltreverse the action ofgt e.g. unfurl, untie,
unbutton, undo - What are the conditions on the use of un2-?
What does it combine with? - Verbs whose actions are reversible.
6Morphology the syntax of words
- The unbuttonable example demonstrates the
compositional route to meaning, but there is also
the direct route (non-compositional). - Consider the compositionality of unravel.
- ltreverse the action ofgt ravel ??
- It seems that unravel cannot be treated as
strictly compositional. - This raises a basic question for modelling
lexical processing.
7Core questions for language processing at the
lexical level
- How word meanings are represented in the mental
lexicon. - How lexical meanings are assigned to words in the
context of sentence processing. - The precise nature of the items which make up the
mental lexicon, which we have thus far identified
as words, but have not attempted to define with
any precision. - Lets take the 3rd question first.
8The question of units words and morphemes
(morphological structure).
- The units of lexical representation, typically
referred to as lexemes by linguists (Huddleson et
al., 2002) and as lemmas by psycholinguists
(Levelt, 1987), - Are smaller than words, the units conventionally
separated by white space in printed text. - e.g., cats and cat 2 words, 1 lexeme
9Lexical decomposition
- The assumption that in the course of processing
words for meaning, listeners strip inflectional
affixes off word forms to access lexical meanings
(Taft and Forster, 1975). - How far does this affix stripping extend?
10There is a cline of decomposability
- cats n plural clearly decomposable
- departed v past
- departed v adj departed guest
- departed derived noun the departed
- not clearly decomposable
11Formal and semantic compositionality
- The question of compositionality is complicated
by this distinction. - Morphology is about the syntax of words.
- Lexical semantics is concerned with the
compositionality of word meaning. - We will deal first with the morphological
structure of words before we discuss word meaning
assignment.
12The dual route model
- Mapping from the sound structure of a word to its
meaning may be achieved compostionally (by rule),
- or by directly matching a word-form in the mental
lexicon (the lexical route). - The compositional route is more likely in the
case of a verb like departed. - the lexical route is the only option for a
suppletive (completely irregular) form like
went. - There has been much controversy in
psycholinguistic circles over how morphological
inflections (such as past tense for verbs and
plurals of nouns) are acquired and to what extent
regular and irregular forms are processed by the
same or different mechanisms in language
comprehension.
13Disagreement between Linguists and
Psycholinguists
- Over extent of morphological decomposition
- Linguists favour maximal decomposition into root
morphemes. - Psycholinguists usually reluctant to pursue
lexical decomposition beyond recognizing
inflectional morphemes and the most productive of
the derivational morphemes. - Language users, they argue, are not etymologists.
- How do we settle this issue?
14Classical psycholinguistic investigations
- (Berko, 1957 Derwing, 1976) used elicitation
techniques to test the productivity of
morphological rules. - This is a wug. Now there are two of them. There
are two _____? - More recently, repetition priming effects have
provided evidence that morphological
decomposition occurs (Marslen-Wilson, et al.,
1994).
15Extent of morphological decomposition
- Most agree The inflectional morphology of a word
is compositional. - English cats, stronger, rained
- Less agreement as to how far decomposition
extends to the derivational morphology. - government is readily decomposable into govern
ltrulegt ment ltnoun-makergt - argument, discernment, refinement, amazement,
arrangement - Can the paradigm be extended to detriment or
department? - How about apartment ??
16Connectionist challenge to the dual route model.
- Rumelhart and McClelland (1986) set the
connectionist cat among the psycholinguistic
pigeons. - Some pigeons held their ground. The connectionist
simulations inadequately modelled the course of
morphology acquisition. (Pinker and Prince,
1988). - Connectionists responded by building more
sophisticated simulations. - Pigeons again temporarily scattered.
- Today both cats and pigeons occupy the square in
an unstable misalliance. - Is there a key to solving this standoff?
17Morphological regularity and frequency of word
usage.
- Low frequency words which take regular
inflections. - Three types of inflection may be distinguished.
What is the rationale for the regularity
frequency trade-off?
18Rationale for the regularity frequency trade-off
- Low token frequency forms would take too long to
retrieve - Irregularly inflected forms are phonologically
economical (shorter). - Partially regular forms form families of
phonologically related members. - e.g. keep /kip/ - /kept/, leap, creep, sleep
- Regularly inflected forms show no family
resemblances in terms of phonological similarity.
19A theory of word representation and processing
needs to
- Account for the way in which the three types of
morphological inflection are related to token and
type frequencies. - Can a single learning mechanism and
representational system accommodate all three
types? - Or do we require a dual-route theory, perhaps
incorporating a race model? - Or possibly, three different learning mechanisms
may be needed. - Over to you.
20Psycholinguistic studies of word structureThe
priming paradigm
- Priming the facilitatory effect that
presentation of a word, can have on the lexical
retrieval of a subsequent word. - In a priming experiment, words are usually
presented to subjects in pairs. - The first word, called the prime, is typically
presented shortly before some target or probe
word. - which may be related to the prime in some way
phonologically, semantically, or morphologically. - Priming effects are usually assessed by measuring
reaction time to the probe word. - Thus in a semantic priming experiment, lexical
access time for the probe word author should be
shorter, by a few tens of milliseconds when
preceded by the related prime book, compared
with an unrelated control probe word such as
flower
21Cross-modal priming task
- Subjects heard spoken prime words and respond
with a lexical decision task to visually
presented probes flashed onto a computer screen - The lexical decision task was simply deciding
whether or not the letter sequence flashed on the
screen was a real word or a non-word. (In
approximately half of the experimental trials the
probe letter sequence was a phonotactically legal
non-word letter sequence, e.g. glark). - It was the morphological relation between the
prime and the probe word, not any phonological
similarity between prime and probe that was found
to matter (Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Waksler and
Older, 1994) - When subjects heard the word friendly spoken at
the same time as they made a lexical decision to
the visual probe friend, their responses were
faster by 40 - 60 milliseconds than to an
unrelated probe, but when tinsel was the
auditory prime for the probe word tin, no priming
effect was obtained.
22Test conditions and morphological priming effects
Only condition 4 where there is a phonological
similarity between prime and probe but no
morphological relationship failed to yield a
priming effect.
What are the effects of semantic similarities
amongst the prime-probe pairs in this experiment?
Can they be separated from effects of
morphological similarity?
23Morphological and Semantic relatedness priming
effects
There is no evidence here for an independent
effect of priming by virtue of a morphological
relationship between the prime and probe stimuli.
Marslen-Wilson et al. (1994) do not describe
their results this way. They say that only
morphologically related pairs which are
semantically transparent, or perceived to be
semantically related, yield priming effects.
Evidence for an independent role for
morphological structure comes from the way that
semantic transparency was found to interact with
prefixed versus suffix-derived morphological
constructions.
24Prefixes behave differently from suffixes in
morphological priming.
Confession fails to prime confessor. But
unfasten primes refasten. Why?
25Why do suffixed words fail to prime each other?
- The words confession and confessor are
semantically related and this semantic similarity
should yield a priming effect - Some countervailing inhibitory effect must be at
work. - This can only come from competition between the
two derivational suffixes -ion and -or.
26The cohort model holds the key
- The suffixes -ion and -or will always be
competitors as perceptual targets in word
recognition, - when they seek to attach to some verbal stem to
form a derived noun. - The target word must be confession, or
confessor, or conceivably some other alternative. - But it must be one of these alternatives.
27Lateral inhibition between competitors
- Lateral inhibition between competitors is
invoked in TRACE and the Cohort model, to resolve
competing hypotheses about the identity of an
input string. - We need also to explain why prefixes do not
mutually inhibit one another. - A prefix typically allows numerous stem
attachments.
28Conclusions from the priming study.
- Morphological decomposition was seen to be
justified, up to a point, on evidence from
cross-modal semantic priming studies. - Morphological decomposition is justified insofar
as the morphological components of a word are
semantically transparent. - i.e., to the extent that the meaning of the whole
word can be clearly related to the meanings of
its component morphemes. - e.g., indefensible ltnotgt(ltdefendgt(ltablegt))
- However, we did not provide an explicit account
of semantic transparency, other than to appeal
to language users intuitions about the meanings
of words. - That is the task of a theory of lexical semantics
29Towards a theory of lexical semantics
30Goals of a theory of lexical semantics
- A theory of lexical semantics needs not only to
account for the meaning of individual words but
how word meanings change in context with other
words. - Consider the meaning of good in the phrase good
friend (ltloyal, reliablegt) - Now consider the meaning of the same word in the
phrase good lover or good meal. - There is a chameleon-like quality to the meaning
of some words, which needs to be accounted for.
31Lexical semantics and Pragmatic knowledge
- The meanings of words are said to be stored in
the mental dictionary, - along with their phonological and
morpho-syntactic features. - Lexical knowledge clearly varies from speaker to
speaker. - Languages also vary from one another in how they
lexicalize our knowledge and perception of the
world - Hence, lexical semantics though intimately
connected with pragmatics (knowledge of the
world) is distinct from it. - Lexical knowledge is confined to knowledge of
word meanings and word usage.
32The problem of polysemy
- Dictionaries have multiple meanings listed for
any given word and the more common the word, the
more meanings it will typically have listed
against it. - Meanings of show that are found in the pocket
Oxford. - The contexts have been scrambled.
33Semantic anomaly detection
- Another way we might test the adequacy of a
theory of lexical semantics.
It is the detection of anomaly in the actual
expression which is of interest to us here,
rather than how the anomalous utterance was
produced.
34Another test
- Ability of the system to generate acceptable
paraphrases for individual words or phrasal
expressions. - To express commonalities and differences of
meaning between verbal expressions that are
semantically related. - a bottle of wine versus a jug of whiskey
35A fundamental unresolved problem on how to
approach the analysis of word and phrase meanings
- Whether semantic theory should be grounded in
logic or in psychology - in some formalized propositional calculus of
truth conditions for possible worlds, - or in perceptual experience and its internal
cognitive representation. - The answer to this long standing controversy may
lie in the level of analysis adopted. - A propositional calculus is clearly more
applicable to sentence-level semantic analysis. - The way we perceive objects in the world is
clearly of critical interest for the semantic
properties of words.
36Semantic Networks
- One of the earliest attempts to construct a
computational model of lexical semantics was
undertaken by Ross Quillian (1968) - A system ambitiously dubbed The teachable
language comprehender(TLC) - A program designed to be capable of being taught
to comprehend English text. - The TLC envisaged the lexicon as a semantic
network encoding both world knowledge and the
meanings of words.
37The TLC
- Quillians semantic network is a symbolic, not a
connectionist network. - The nodes of the network represent concepts or
words, - that are linked by arcs, which represent a small
number of relational types. - The meaning of a word may be expressed, first by
accessing its root node in the network and
traversing the network elements to which this
node is linked in a series of steps. - A words meaning is defined by other words, whose
meanings are defined, in turn, by other words.
38How the TLC represents the polysemous word plant
These three meanings of plant are disjunctive in
their usage.
The basic meaning of plant (PLANT 1) and pointers
to its alternative meanings (PLANT 2 and PLANT 3)
are expressed in a plane of word memory.
39Semantic representations in TLC
- Four basic types of arc linking nodes in a
semantic network - In addition to the basic logical relations of
disjunction (OR) and conjunction (AND) - His system recognized a subordinate-superordinate
relation, which others have labelled the ISA
relation and which defines a relationship of
class membership (e.g., dog - animal). - A third type of linkage, which may simply be
thought of as property attribution links one
node directly to another, declaring that the node
to which an arrow points is a property of the
node from which that arrow points.
40Testing TLC