Title: Social Psychology
1Social Psychology
- Psychology A Concise Introduction2nd Edition
- Richard Griggs
- Chapter 9
Prepared byJ. W. Taylor V
2Social Psychology
- The scientific study of how we influence one
anothers behavior and thinking - Social psychologys focus is on how situational
forces influence our behavior and thinking
3The Journey
- How Others Influence Our Behavior
- How We Think about Our Own and Others Behavior
4How Others Influence Our Behavior
- Why We Conform
- Why We Comply
- Why We Obey
- How Groups Influence Us
5Social Influence
- Examines how other people and the social forces
they create influence an individuals behavior
6Why We Conform
- Conformity is defined as a change in behavior,
belief, or both to conform to a group norm as a
result of real or imagined group pressure - Although conformity has negative connotations
in Western cultures, some conformity is needed
for society to function - For instance, in the military, conformity is
essential because in a time of war, soldiers
cannot each do his or her own thing while in
battle
7Why We Conform
Informational Social Influence
NormativeSocialInfluence
SituationalFactors
8The Sherif Study and Informational Social
Influence
- Participants, who thought they were in a visual
perception experiment, were placed in a
completely dark room and exposed to a stationary
point of light, and their task was to estimate
the distance this light moved - The light never moved it was an illusion called
the autokinetic effect, whereby a stationary
point of light appears to move in a dark room
because there is no frame of reference and our
eyes spontaneously move
9The Sherif Study and Informational Social
Influence
- During the first session, each participant was
alone in the dark room when making their
judgments - But during the next three sessions, they were in
the room with two other participants and could
hear each others estimates of the illusory light
movement - The average individual estimates varied greatly
during the first session - During the next three sessions, though, the
individual estimates converged on a common group
norm - A year later, participants were brought back and
made estimates alone yet, these estimates
remained at the group norm
10The Sherif Study and Informational Social
Influence
- This pattern of results suggests the impact of
informational social influence, which is
influence that stems from our desire to be
correct in situations in which the correct action
of judgment is uncertain and we need information - When a task is ambiguous or difficult and we want
to be correct, we look to others for information - For instance, when visiting a foreign culture, it
is usually a good idea to watch how the people
living in that culture behave in various
situations because they provide information to
outsiders on how to behave in that culture
11The Asch Study and Normative Social Influence
- In Aschs study, the visual judgments were easy
visual discriminations involving line-length
judgments - Specifically, participants had to judge which one
of three lines was the same length as a standard
line - In this study, the correct answer/behavior was
obvious - Indeed, when making such judgments alone, almost
no one made any mistakes
12An Example of Aschs Line-Length Judgment Task
13The Asch Study and Normative Social Influence
- In Aschs study, there were other participants
who were in fact experimental confederates, part
of the experimental setting - On each trial, judgments were made orally, and
Asch structured the situation so the experimental
confederates responded before the true
participant - These experimental confederates arranged to make
mistakes on certain trials in an effort to see
how the real participant would respond when
asked to make line length judgments
14The Asch Study and Normative Social Influence
- About 75 of the participants gave an obviously
wrong answer at least once, and overall,
conformity occurred 37 of the time - This conformity occurred despite the fact the
correct answer, unlike in Sherifs study, was
obvious
15The Asch Study and Normative Social Influence
- Aschs results illustrate the power of normative
social influence, influence stemming from our
desire to gain the approval and to avoid the
disapproval of other people - In essence, we change our behavior to meet the
expectations of others and to gain the
acceptance of others - If the line-length judgments were extremely
difficult, and the correct answers were not
clear, then informational social influence would
likely lead to even higher levels of conformity
16Situational Factors that Impact Conformity
- If the group is unanimous, conformity will
increase - Asch found that the amount of conformity
decreased considerably if just one of the
experimental confederate participants gives the
correct answer, or even an incorrect answer that
is different from the incorrect answer all other
confederates gave - As one person is different somehow, it allows
other people to avoid conforming.
17Situational Factors that Impact Conformity
- The mode of responding is also critical
- Secret ballots lead to less conformity than
public, verbal reports - The status of group members intervenes
- More conformity is observed from a person that is
of lesser status than the other group members or
is attracted to the group and wants to be part of
it
18Why We Comply
- Compliance is acting in accordance to a direct
request from another person or group - Occurs in many facets of life (e.g., salespeople,
fundraisers, politicians, and anyone else who
wants to get people to say yes to their
requests)
19Compliance Techniques
Foot-in-the-door
Door-in-the-face
Low-ball
Thats-not-all
20The Foot-in-the-Door Technique
- Here, compliance to a large request is gained by
prefacing it with a very small, almost mindless
request - The tendency is for people who have complied with
the small request to comply with the next, larger
request - In Freedman and Frasers (1966) classic study,
some people were asked directly to put a large
ugly sign urging careful driving in their front
yards - Almost all such people refused the large ugly
sign - However, some other people were first asked to
sign a petition urging careful driving - Two weeks after signing this petition (that is,
agreeing to a rather small request), the majority
of these latter people agreed to allow the large
ugly sign in the front yards
21The Foot-in-the-Door Technique
- This technique seems to work because our behavior
(complying with the initial request) affects our
attitudes, leading us to be more positive about
helping and to view ourselves as generally
charitable people - In addition, once we have made a commitment
(such as signing a safe driving petition), we
feel pressure to remain consistent (by putting
up the large ugly sign) with the earlier action
22The Foot-in-the-Door Technique
- The technique was used by the Communist Chinese
in the Korean War on prisoners of war - Many prisoners returning home after the war
praised the Chinese Communists because while in
captivity, the prisoners did small things such as
writing out questions and then providing the
pro-Communist answers, which often they just
copied from a notebook - Such minor actions induced more sympathy for the
Communist cause
23The Door-in-the-Face Technique
- The opposite of the foot-in-the-door technique
- Compliance is gained by starting with a large
unreasonable request that is turned down, and
then following it with a more reasonable smaller
request - It is the smaller request that the person making
the two requests wants someone to comply with
24The Door-in-the-Face Technique
- For instance, a teenager may ask his parents if
he can have a new sports car for his 16th
birthday - His parents are likely to refuse
- Then, the teenager asks his parents to help him
pay for a used 20-year-old car, which is what he
wanted his parents to help him with all along
25The Door-in-the-Face Technique
- The success of the door-in-the-face technique is
due to our tendency toward reciprocity, that is,
making mutual concessions - The person making the requests appears to have
made a concession by moving to the much smaller
request so shouldnt we reciprocate and comply
with this smaller request?
26The Low-Ball Technique
- Compliance to a costly request is achieved by
first getting compliance to an attractive, less
costly request, but then reneging on it - This is similar to the foot-in-the-door technique
in that a second larger request is the one
desired all along - Low-balling works because many of us feel
obligated to go through with the deal after we
have agreed to the earlier request, even if the
first request has changed for the worse - We want to remain consistent in our actions
27The Thats-Not-All Technique
- People are more likely to comply to a request
after a build-up to make the request sound
better - Often in infomercials on TV, for example, the
announcer says But wait, thats not all, theres
more! and the price is lowered or more
merchandise is added to sweeten the deal, usually
before you even have a chance to respond - Similarly, a car salesperson is likely to throw
in additional options as bonuses before you can
answer yes or no to a price offered
28The Thats-Not-All Technique
- As in the door-in-the-face technique, reciprocity
is at work - The seller has done you a favor (thrown in bonus
options, lowered the price), so you should
reciprocate by accepting the offer (i.e., comply)
29Four Compliance Techniques
30Why We Obey
- Obedience is following the commands of a person
in authority - Obedience is good in some instances, such as
obeying societal laws - Obedience is bad in other instances, such as in
the My Lai massacre during the Vietnam War, when
American soldiers were ordered to shoot innocent
villagers (and they did so)
31Why We Obey
MilgramsExperiment
The Astroten Study
SituationalFactors
32Milgrams Basic Experimental Paradigm
- Stanley Milgrams obedience studies were done
primarily at Yale University in the early 1960s - Imagine that you have volunteered to be in an
experiment on learning and memory - You show up at the assigned time and place, and
there is the experimenter and another participant
there
33Milgrams Basic Experimental Paradigm
- The experimenter tells you both that the study is
examining the effects of punishment by electric
shock on learning, and specifically learning a
list of word pairs - One of the participants will be the teacher and
the other participants will be the learner - You draw slips for these roles, and you draw the
slip of the teacher, so the other participant
will be the learner
34Milgrams Basic Experimental Paradigm
- You accompany the learner to an adjoining room
where he is strapped into a chair with one arm
hooked up to the shock generator in the other
room - The shock levels in the study range from 15
volts to 450 volts - The experimenter gives you, the teacher, a
test shock of 45 volts so that you know how
intense various shock levels will be
35Milgrams Basic Experimental Paradigm
- You return to your room with the shock generator
- You notice that on the shock generator, each
switch has a label for each level of shock,
starting at 15 volts and going to 450 volts in
15-volt increments - There are also some verbal labels below the
switches, Slight Shock, Very Strong Shock,
Danger Severe Shock, and under the last two
switches XXX in red. - Each time the learner makes a mistake, he is to
receive a shock, which should increase one
15-volt level for each additional mistake
36Milgrams Basic Experimental Paradigm
- As the experiment begins, the learner makes some
mistakes, and you as the teacher throw the shock
lever as instructed by the experimenter - At 120 volts, the learner cries out that the
shocks really hurt - As the learner continues to make mistakes, he
protests and says that he has a heart condition
and that he refuses to continue with the
experiment, demanding to be let out of his chair - After a 330-volt shock, he fails to respond with
any protest - You turn to the experimenter to see what to do,
and the experimenter says to treat no response as
an incorrect response and continue with the
experiment
37Milgrams Initial Obedience Finding
- Before this experiment was run, Milgram asked
various types of people what they and other
people would do - Most people thought people would stop at
relatively low shock levels - Psychiatrists said that maybe one person in a
thousand would go to the end of the shock
generator
38Milgrams Initial Obedience Findings
- In reality, almost two out of every three
participants (65) continued to obey the
experimenter and administered the maximum
possible shock of 450 volts - This is particularly disturbing because the
learner had mentioned a heart condition before
the experiment started and during his protests - It is important to realize that the learner was a
confederate who was programmed to make mistakes
and was never really shocked - But the teacher thought that he was administering
real shocks because of real mistakes
39InterpretingMilgrams Findings
- The difference between what we say we will do and
what we actually do illustrates the power of
situational social forces on our behavior - The foot-in-the-door technique was used because
participants started off giving very mild shocks
(15 volts) and increased the voltage relatively
slowly - The learner did not protest these early shocks,
and the teacher had obeyed several times before
the learner started his protests
40InterpretingMilgrams Findings
- It should be noted that later studies with
female participants found similar obedience
rates, and other researchers have replicated
Milgrams basic finding in many different
cultures (e.g., Jordan, Spain, Italy, and
Australia)
41Situational Factors that Impact Obedience
- The physical presence of the experimenter (the
person with authority) - If the experimenter left the room and gave
commands over the telephone, maximum obedience
(administering the highest shock level) dropped
to 21 - The physical closeness of teacher and learner
- Milgram made the teacher and learner closer by
having them both in the same room instead of
different rooms, and maximum obedience declined
to 40 - It dropped to 30 when the teacher had to
directly administer the shock by forcing the
learners hand onto a plate
42Situational Factors that Impact Obedience
- Setting of the study
- Instead of conducting the research at prestigious
Yale University, Milgram did the study in a
run-down office building in Bridgeport,
Connecticut - Here, he found a 48 obedience rate thus, the
setting did not influence obedience as much as
presence of the experimenter or closeness of the
teacher and learner - Experimenter unanimity
- Milgram set up a situation with two experimenters
who at some point during the experiment disagreed - One said to stop the experimenter, while the
other said to continue - In this case, when one of the people in authority
said to stop, all of the teachers stopped
delivering the shocks
43Situational Factors that Impact Obedience
- Teacher responsibility
- In another variation, Milgram had the teacher
only push the switch on the shock generator to
indicate to another teacher (an experimental
confederate) in the room with the learner how
much shock to administer - Here, 93 of the participants obeyed the
experimenter to the maximum shock levels
44Results for Some of Milgrams Experimental
Conditions
45The Astroten Study
- Participants were real nurses on duty alone in a
real hospital ward - Each nurse received a call from a person using
the name of a staff doctor not personally known
by the nurse - The doctor ordered the nurse to give a dose
exceeding the maximum daily dosage of an
unauthorized medication, called Astroten to a
real patient in the ward
46The Astroten Study
- This situation violated many hospital rules
- Medication orders need to be given in person and
not over the phone - It was a clear overdose
- The medication was unauthorized
- Of the 22 nurses phoned, 21 did not question the
order and went to give the medication, but were
intercepted before actually giving it to the
patient
47The Astroten Study
- A separate sample of 33 nurses were asked about
this situation and what they would do if they
were placed it in - All but 2 said they would NOT obey the doctors
order, again demonstrating the difference between
what we think we will do and what we actually do
in a given situation
48The Jonestown Massacre
- In 1978, more than 900 people who were members of
Reverend Jim Joness religious cult in Jonestown,
Guyana committed mass suicide by drinking
cyanide-laced Kool Aid - These were Americans who moved to South America
from San Francisco in 1977 - Using various compliance techniques, Jones
developed unquestioned faith as the cult leader
and discouraged individualism
49The Jonestown Massacre
- Using the foot-in-the-door technique, he was able
to increase financial support required of each
member until they had turned over essentially
everything they owned - He had recruiters ask people walking by to help
the poor - When they refused, the recruiters then asked them
just to donate five minutes of time to put
letters in envelopes (door-in-the-face) - When given information about other charitable
work, having agreed to this small task, people
returned later as a function of the consistency
aspect of the foot-in-the-door technique - Informational social influence was also at work,
as being moved from San Francisco to Guyana
created an uncertain environment in which
followers would look to others to guide their own
actions
50How Groups Influence Us
SocialFacilitation
SocialLoafing
BystanderEffect
Deindivi-duation
GroupPolarization
51Social Facilitation
- The emergence of a dominant response on a task
(for which a person is individually responsible)
due to social arousal, leading to improvement on
simple or well-learned tasks and worse
performance on complex or unlearned tasks when
other people are present - This effect occurs because that the presence of
others increases physiological arousal, and under
conditions of increased arousal, people tend to
give whatever response is most dominant
52Social Facilitation
- For example, for a professional basketball
player, shooting free throws is a simple, easy
task - Thus, such a person would shoot free throws
better when other people are around and watching
than when shooting alone - However, for someone not good at shooting a
basketball, s/he will shoot even more poorly when
other people are around and watching than when
shooting alone
53Social Loafing and the Diffusion of
Responsibility
- Social loafing occurs when people are pooling
their efforts to achieve a common goal - It is the tendency for people to exert less
effort when working toward a common goal in a
group than when individually accountable
54Social Loafing and the Diffusion of
Responsibility
- A major reason why social loafing occurs is the
diffusion of responsibility, which means that the
responsibility for a task is spread across all
members of the group so individual accountability
is lessened - The larger the group, the less likely it is that
a social loafer will be detected and the more
responsibility for the task gets diffused across
group members - However, for groups in which individual
contributions are identifiable and evaluated,
social loafing decreases
55Social Loafing and the Diffusion of
Responsibility
- For instance, in a group project for a shared
grade, social loafing would decrease if each
group member is assigned and responsible for a
specific part of the project
56The Bystander Effect
- In 1964, Kitty Genovese was returning home from
work late one night when she was attacked in
front of her apartment building - She screamed for help, and many apartment
residents, at least 38 of them, heard her cries
for help and looked out their windows - The attacker fled, but no one intervened
- The attacker returned and continued his assault
for another 35 minutes before finally murdering
her - The first person in the apartment complex did not
call the police until after Kitty had been killed
57The Bystander Effect
- Many media people said this incident illustrated
big city apathy - However, experiments by social psychologists
suggested that it was more diffusion of
responsibility - The bystander effect holds that the probability
of an individual helping in an emergency is
greater when there is only one bystander than
when there are many bystanders
58The Bystander Effect
- Darley and Latané (1968) did an experiment in
which college students were ostensibly going to
participate in a round-robin discussion of
college adjustment problems, and that this
discussion would occur over an intercom system - Thus, participants could only hear each other,
not see each other - The experimenter says he will not listen to the
conversation so participants wont feel at all
inhibited
59The Bystander Effect
- After each student gets a turn to talk, the first
student gets to talk again, but he seems to be
very anxious - Suddenly, he starts having a seizure and cries
out for help - What would a participant do in this situation?
60The Bystander Effect
- Whether or not a participant helped depended on
how many other individuals the participant
thought were available to help the student having
the seizure - The researchers manipulated the number of other
people present (either 0, 1 or 4 others present) - In reality no one else was present, the supposed
other participants were merely tape recordings - When no one else was thought to be present, 85
of the participants tried to help the person,
whereas only 31 of the participants did so when
4 other people were supposedly present
61The Bystander Effect
- The probability of helping decreased as the
responsibility for helping was diffused across
more participants - In the case of Kitty Genovese, there were 38
bystanders who could see each other staring out
of their windows with some turning on their
lights - Responsibility was diffused across all of them,
with no one person assuming full responsibility
to help - Kitty might have received help and possibly
lived had there been only one person available
to give help (i.e., call the police) rather than
38!
62Deindividuation
- The loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in
a group situation that fosters arousal and
anonymity - Deindividuated people feel less restrained, so
may forget their moral values and act
spontaneously without thinking - Diffusion of responsibility also plays a role in
deindividuation because of the anonymity of the
group situation
63Group Polarization Groupthink
- Apply to more structured, task-oriented group
situations
64Group Polarization
- The strengthening of a groups prevailing opinion
about a topic following group discussion of the
topic - For instance, if students who dont like a
particular class all start talking about that
class, they will leave the discussion disliking
the class even more because each student may
provide different reasons for disliking the class - Each member learns new reasons for his or her
dislike of the class
65Group Polarization
- In addition, normative social influence is at
work - We want others to like us, so we express stronger
views on a topic to gain approval from others in
the group - For instance, students who belong to fraternities
or sororities tend to be more politically
liberal, and this difference grows during college
because group members reinforce and polarize each
others views
66Groupthink
- A mode of group thinking that impairs decision
making - The desire for group harmony overrides a
realistic appraisal of the possible decisions - Leads to an illusion of infallibility, the belief
that the group cannot make mistakes
67Groupthink
- Examples of groupthink in history include the
failure to anticipate Pearl Harbor, the Bay of
Pigs invasion of Cuba, and the Space Shuttles
Challenger and Columbia disasters - In the case of the Space Shuttle Columbia, NASA
apparently ignored safety warnings from
engineers about possible technical problems
68How We Think about Our Own and Others Behavior
- How We Make Attributions
- How Our Behavior Affects Our Attitudes
69Attribution
- The process by which we explain our own behavior
and the behavior of others - That is, what do we think are the causes of our
behavior and the behavior of others?
70How We Make Attributions
- An internal attribution means explaining behavior
in terms of a persons disposition/personal
characteristics - An external attribution means explaining behavior
in terms of a persons circumstances/situation - For example, if you are sitting in the airport
and see someone trip and fall over their own two
feet, you might think What a idiot meaning the
persons disposition lead him to trip - However, if you think He must be late for a
flight, you are making an external attribution
71Attributions for the Behavior of Others
Fundamental Attribution Error
Self-FulfillingProphecy
72Attributions for the Behavior of Others
- The fundamental attribution error is the tendency
as an observer to overestimate internal
dispositional influences and underestimate
external situational influences upon others
behavior - More simply, we tend to ignore external factors
when explaining the behaviors of other people - May have played a role in Milgrams results The
teachers figured that if the learner was stupid,
he deserved the shocks
73Attributions for the Behavior of Others
- Placing such blame on victims involves the
just-world hypothesis, the assumption that the
world is just and that people get what they
deserve - Helps justify cruelty to others
- The primacy effect is partially responsible for
the fundamental attribution error - In the primacy effect, early information is
weighted more heavily than later information in
forming an impression of another person - Be careful of the initial impression you make on
others!
74Attributions for the Behavior of Others
- In the self-fulfilling prophecy, our expectations
of a person elicit behavior from the person that
confirms our expectations - For instance, if you think a person is
uncooperative, you may act in an uncooperative
way in your interactions with the person - Given your uncooperative behavior, the person
responds by being uncooperative, confirming your
expectations
75Attributions for Our Own Behavior
Actor-Observer Bias
Self-Serving Bias
76Actor-Observer Bias
- The tendency to attribute our own behavior to
situational influences, but to attribute the
behavior of others to dispositional influences - As actors, our attention is focused on the
situation - But as observers, our attention is focused on the
individual, hence why we make the fundamental
attribution error
77Self-Serving Bias
- The tendency to make attributions so that one can
perceive oneself favorably - As actors, we tend to overestimate dispositional
influences when the outcome of our behavior is
positive and to overestimate situational
influences when the outcome of our behavior is
negative - In short, we take credit for our successes but
not for our failures
78Self-Serving Bias
- We tend to see ourselves as above average when
we compare ourselves to others on positive
dimensions such as intelligence and
attractiveness - However, such traits tend to be normally
distributed with half of us below average and
half of us above average
79Self-Serving Bias
- Self-serving bias can also influence our
estimates of the extent to which other people
think and act as we do
FalseConsensusEffect
False Uniqueness Effect
80False Consensus Effect
- The tendency to overestimate the commonality of
ones opinions and unsuccessful behaviors - For instance, if you like a certain type of food,
you overestimate how many people also like that
type of food - Or, if you failed an important exam, you tend to
overestimate the number of your classmates who
also failed the exam
81False Uniqueness Effect
- The tendency to underestimate the commonality of
ones abilities and successful behaviors - For instance, if you are a good golfer, you think
that few people are, thus allowing you to feel
good about yourself
82When Our Behavior Contradicts Our Attitudes
- Attitudes are evaluative reactions (positive or
negative) toward things, events, and other people
- Our attitudes tend to guide our behavior when the
attitudes are ones that we feel strongly about,
when we are consciously aware of our attitudes,
and when outside influences on our behavior are
not strong
83When Our Behavior Contradicts Our Attitudes
Festingers Cognitive Dissonance Theory
Bems Self-Perception Theory
Impact of Role Playing
84Festinger and Carlsmiths Study
- In the study, participants completed an
incredibly boring task, such as turning pegs on a
pegboard or organizing spools in a box, dumping
them out, and organizing them again - After the hour is over, the experimenter explains
to you that the experiment is concerned with the
effects of a persons expectations on their task
performance and that you were in the control
group - The experimenter is upset because his student
assistant has not shown up for the next
experimental session - She was supposed to pose as a student who just
participated in the experiment and tell the next
participant who is waiting outside that this
experiment was really enjoyable
85Festinger and Carlsmiths Study
- The experiment asks the participant to play the
role of the student assistant, and he will pay
you either 1 or 20 for telling the next
participant (actually a confederate of the
experimenter) how enjoyable and interesting the
experiment was
- After telling the supposed participant how great
the experiment was, another person who is
studying students reaction to experiments asks
you to complete a questionnaire about how much
you enjoyed the earlier experimental tasks
86Festinger and Carlsmiths Study
- Participants who were paid only 1 rated the
boring tasks as fairly enjoyable, whereas
participants who were paid 20 rated the boring
tasks as boring - Possible explanations of this counterintuitive
finding
87Festingers Cognitive Dissonance Theory
- Proposes that people change their attitudes to
reduce the cognitive discomfort created by
inconsistencies between their attitudes and their
behavior - For instance, people who smoke, an unhealthy
behavior known to most everyone, may feel
cognitive discomfort because of the inconsistency
between their behavior and their
attitude/knowledge that smoking is bad for their
health - According to cognitive dissonance theory, many
smokers change their attitude, so that it is no
longer inconsistent with their behavior
88Festingers Cognitive Dissonance Theory
- So why did participants paid only 1 indicate on
the survey they enjoyed the experiment more than
participants paid 20? - The people paid 1 lied and said the task was
interesting to another person - Thus, there was an inconsistency between their
actions (saying the experiment was interesting
without any significant external incentive) and
their attitudes (the experiment was in reality
quite boring) - To reduce this inconsistency, these participants
changed their attitude to be that the tasks were
fairly enjoyable - Now the inconsistency and resulting dissonance
are gone
89Festingers Cognitive Dissonance Theory
- A key aspect of cognitive dissonance is that we
dont suffer dissonance if we have sufficient
justification for our behavior (i.e., the
participants paid 20 in the study had perfectly
good reason to be inconsistent but not experience
dissonance) or our behavior was coerced - Cognitive dissonance theory also says that once
you make a tough choice, you will strengthen your
commitment to that choice in order to reduce
cognitive dissonance
90Bems Self-Perception Theory
- Proposes that when we are unsure of our attitudes
we infer them by examining our behavior and the
context in which it occurs - We have no dissonance to reduce, but are merely
engaging in the normal attribution process - For instance, in the experiment, people would
examine their behavior (e.g., lying for 1) and
infer the task must have been fairly interesting
or else they would not have lied for only that 1 - Self-perception theory contends that we dont
change our attitude because of our behavior, but
rather we use our behavior to infer our attitude
91Cognitive Dissonance vs. Self-Perception
- Cognitive dissonance theory is a better
explanation for behavior that contradicts
well-established attitudes - Such behavior creates mental discomfort, and we
change our attitudes to reduce it - Self-perception theory explains situations in
which our attitudes are not well-defined - We infer our attitudes from our behavior
92The Impact of Role-Playing
- A role is a social position that carries with it
expected behaviors from the person in it - Each role is defined by the socially expected
pattern of behavior for it, and these definitions
impact both our behavior and our attitudes
93Zimbardos Study
- In a now-classic study, Zimbardo (1970) recruited
male college students to participate in a study
held in the renovated basement of the Stanford
University psychology building, renovated to be
a mock prison - He chose the most emotionally-stable volunteers
for the study and then randomly assigned them to
play the roles of prisoner and prison guard - The guards were given uniforms and billy clubs
- The prisoners were locked in cells and had to
wear humiliating clothing (smocks with no
undergarments)
94Zimbardos Study
- The participants began to take their respective
roles too seriously - After only one day of role playing, the guards
started treating the prisoners cruelly - Some of the prisoners rebelled, and others began
to break down - Role-playing quickly became reality
- The situation deteriorated so much that Zimbardo
had to stop the study after only 6 days - Even these emotionally stable, normal young
educated men were vulnerable to the power of
the situational roles