BIAS: threats to validity and interpretation - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 42
About This Presentation
Title:

BIAS: threats to validity and interpretation

Description:

BIAS: threats to validity and interpretation Bias is the result of systematic error in the design or conduct of a study; a tendency toward erroneous results – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:97
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 43
Provided by: epibiosta
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: BIAS: threats to validity and interpretation


1
BIAS threats to validity and interpretation
  • Bias is the result of systematic error in the
    design or conduct of a study a tendency toward
    erroneous results
  • Systematic error results from flaws in either the
    (1) method of selection of study participants, or
  • (2) in the procedures for gathering relevant
    exposure and/or disease information
  • Hence - the observed study results will tend to
    be different from the true results

systematic error is different from error due to
random variability (sampling error)
2
Main points to be covered
  • What is selection bias?
  • in a case-control study
  • in a cohort study
  • in a clinical trial
  • Avoiding and detecting selection bias
  • What is information bias?
  • Examples of information bias
  • recall bias,
  • interviewer or observer bias,
  • surveillance bias,
  • Differential and non-differential
    misclassification
  • How to prevent or minimize information bias
  • Information bias in a case-control study (recall
    bias)

3
Selection bias
  • Selection Bias is present when individuals have
    different probabilities of being included in the
    study according to relevant study
    characteristics namely the exposure and the
    outcome of interest
  • Selection bias anytime there is systematic error
    in the selection of study subjects cases or
    controls in a case-control study, exposed or
    unexposed in a cohort study

4
REFERENCE POPULATION
Diseased
-
-
Exposed
STUDY SAMPLE
Figure 4.2
5
Example of bias in a case-control study Coffee
and cancer of the pancreas MacMahon et al (N
Eng J Med 1981 304630-3)
Cases patients with histologic diagnoses of
cancer of the exocrine pancreas who were in any
of 11 large hospitals in the Boston metropolitan
area and Rhode Island between October 1974 and
August 1979 Controls Other patients who were
under the care of the same physician in the same
hospital at the time of an interview with a
patient with pancreatic cancer. Patients with
diseases known to be associated with smoking or
alcohol consumption were excluded with diseases
of the pancreas or hepatobiliary tract,
cardiovascular disease,diabetes mellitus,
respiratory or bladder cancer, and peptic ulcer.
Patients with gastroenterologic conditions were
probably overepresented in relation to a general
hospital population Describe the cases - What
study base gave rise to these CASES? What would
be a good population to get controls from?
6
Coffee and cancer of the pancreas MacMahon et
al, (N Eng J Med 1981 304630-3)
Males Case Control
482 41
Coffee No coffee
216 307
OR 2.7 (1.2-6.5)
7
Selection bias case-control studies
  • In case-control studies, selection bias may arise
    if the cases and controls do not represent all
    the individuals in the population being studied,
    ie. the study base.
  • Selection bias is particularly likely when
    dealing with a secondary study base ie. when the
    cases are defined first.
  • In the previous case, the lower odds of exposure
    (coffee intake) in controls led to a spurious
    positive association between coffee intake and
    pancreatic cancer.

8
Coffee and pancreatic cancer chapter 2 Hsieh et
al, (N Eng J Med 1986 315 587-588)
All Case Control
Coffee No coffee
370 70
170 270
OR 1.5 (0.8-2.6)
9
Selection bias cohort studies
  • In cohort studies, the most common form of
    selection bias occurs with loss to follow up.
  • When those lost to follow-up are more or less
    likely to include exposed and affected
    individuals - differential loss to follow-up
    probability of the outcome is different in those
    who remain in cohort vs. those who leave (due to
    refusal, migration, jail, ...)
  • Eg In a study following adolescents at risk for
    HIV, those who sell drugs may be more likely to
    be incarcerated. If selling drugs is associated
    with a more needle sharing, a biased association
    would result for needle sharing and HIV
    incidence.

10
Selection bias cohort studies
  • Selection bias is less likely to occur in cohort
    studies (compared to case-control) since study
    participants (exposed or unexposed) are selected
    (theoretically) before the disease occurs
  • Selection bias can occur on the front-end of
    the cohort if diseased individuals are mistakenly
    entered into the cohort
  • Eg In a study of physically active men a
    positive association was found for exercise
    exposure and all-cause mortality. If study
    participants were enrolled who had undiagnosed
    cardiovascular disease were more likely to
    exercise less, what would the effect be on the
    measure of association?

11
Selection bias cohort studies
  • Results obtained from studies comparing survival
    among HIV infected IDU and homosexual men were
    not consistent. One study showed that
    progression rates in cohorts of IDU were slower
    than those in cohorts of homosexual men after
    adjusting for for potential confounders such as
    age at seroconversion. (Multicohort analysis
    Project Workshop. Part 1. AIDS 1994, 8911-921).
    Others found no such difference.
  • A subsequent study found that IDU had a high
    pre-AIDS mortality rate .. ...In contrast,
    pre-AIDS mortality was much higher in IDU than
    homosexual men... ...Withdrawal differed
    between the two risk groups. Therefore the
    inclusion or exclusion of losses to follow-up and
    pre-AIDS deaths in the denominator altered
    results. Pre-AIDs mortality may not be
    independent of progression and non-progression.
    (Prins et al, 1997, AIDS 1997, 11621-631).
  • Prins et al, further showed that pre-AIDS
    death from natural causes was found to be related
    with ongoing time since HIV seroconversion and
    immunosuppression (Prins et al, AIDS 1997,
    111747-1756).

12
Selection bias clinical trial
  • The potential for selection bias in clinical
    trials is in losses to follow-up
  • Consider - a drug that causes a symptomatic side
    effect that frequently results in discontinuation
    of the study medication or drop-out.
  • What would happen if you discontinued following
    them?
  • What will the effect be on the study findings if
    the side effect is associated with the main
    outcome?

13
Example of possible bias due to losses to
follow-up in a clinical Chestnut CH et al, A
randomized trial of nasal spray salmon calcitonin
in postmenopausal women with established
osteioporosis the PROOF study. Am J. Med 2000
109267-276. Purpose a 5-year, double-blind,
randomized, placebo-controlled study to determine
whether salmon calcitonin (SC) nasal spray
reduced the risk of vertebral fractures.
Subjects and methods 1,255 post-menopausal
women with established osteoporosis were randomly
assigned to receive the SC nasal spray. Vertebral
fracture assessed with lateral radiographs.
Primary efficacy endpoint was the risk of new
vertebral fractures in the 200 IU SC nasal spray
group compared to placebo group. Results During
5 years, 1,108 participants had at least one
follow-up radiograph. A total of 783 women
completed 2 years of treatment, and 511 completed
5 years. The 200 IU SC nasal spray dose
significantly reduced the risk of new vertebral
fractures by 33 compared with placebo 200 IU
51 of 287, placebo 70 of 270, RR0.67, 95 CI,
0.47-0.97, p0.03. In the 817 women with one to
five prevalent vertebral fractures at enrollment,
the risk was reduced by 36 (RR-0.64, 95
CI0.43-0.96, p0.03) Conclusion SC nasal spray
at a dose of 200 IU daily significantly reduces
the risk of new vertebral fractures in
postmenopausal women with osteoporosis.
14
Results 59 of participants withdrew from the
study prematurely. Rates of discontinuation were
similar in all dosage groups. Cummings
Chapurlat What PROOF proves about calcitonin and
clinical trials (Editorial). Am J Med 2000
109330-331. There was a 36 reduction in risk of
vertebral deformities in the group that received
20- IU a day, but no significant effects were
seen with a higher dose. There wa not consistent
reduction in the risk of other types of fractures
across doses. The authors state that women who
were lost from the placebo group were similar in
the most easily measurable respects to those who
were lost form the calcitonin groups. When so
many participants fail to finish a trial, ,
however, readers rightly wonder whether this
really retains the valididy of a randomized
trial. The authors used an intention to treat
analysisBut the participants who were lost were
not followed up, and the investigators have not
information about the treatments of outcomes of
more than half the women who were originally
enrolled It was not known if fractures had
occurred in these participants. Because the
overall number of fractures was small, even a few
fractures in the participants lost to follow-up
could have altered findings of the trial.
15
Avoiding and detecting selection bias
  • In case-control studies, Choose controls from the
    same study-base as cases.
  • In cohort studies, the rate of loss to follow-up
    indicates the potential for selection bias.
    Comparison of the characteristics of those lost
    to follow-up with those persons remaining under
    follow-up, may indicate the potential
    consequences of any selection bias.

16
Information Bias
  • Information Bias results from a systematic error
    in measurement thus leading to misclassification
    (in exposure or outcome category).
  • A classic example is recall bias, in which the
    ability to recall past exposure is dependent on
    case or control status. Cases may be more likely
    than controls to overstate past exposure

17
Misclassification of EXPOSURE
REFERENCE POPULATION
Diseased
The direction of the association is a function of
which cell(s) are subjected to a higher or lower
probability
-
-
Exposed
Cases Control
STUDY SAMPLE
Eg...unexposed cases in this example tend to
mistakenly report past exposure to a
greater extent than do controls
18
Misclassification of OUTCOME
REFERENCE POPULATION
Diseased
-
-
Exposed
Cases Control
STUDY SAMPLE
Egcases in this are mistakenly classified as
controls due to low sensitivity on a screening
test
19
Information Bias
  • These errors result in misclassification of
    exposure and/or outcome status
  • Terms validity, sensitivity, specificity and
    reliability refer to classification of both
    disease and exposure status (and confounders)

20
Definition of Terms Related to Classification
  • Validity the extent to which a measurement
    measures what it purports to measure.
  • Sensitivity the ability of a test to identify
    correctly those who have the disease (or
    characteristic) of interest.
  • Specificity the ability of a test to identify
    correctly those who do not have the disease (or
    characteristic) of interest
  • Reliability (repeatability) the extent to which
    the results obtained by a test are replicated in
    the test is repeated.

21
1. Exposure Identification Bias
  • Problems in the collection of exposure data
  • 2 main examples
  • Recall bias
  • Interviewer bias

22
1.1 Recall Bias
  • Most cited inaccurate recall of past exposure
    (may be due to temporality, social desirability
    or diagnosis).
  • If recall differs between cases and controls,
    misclassification is differential
  • If the error is of similar magnitude, then it is
    said to be non-differential
  • Example study of association between hair color
    and tanning ability and melanoma
  • Weinstock et al (Recall (report) bias and
    reliability in the retrospective assessment of
    melanoma risk AJE 1991 133240-245.

23
How to Prevent Recall Bias
  • Verification of exposure information from
    participants by review of pre-existing records
  • Objective markers of exposure or susceptibility
    (for example- genetic markers).
  • A disadvantage of some biologic markers is that
    they assess current, rather than past exposure.
    Eg. Cotinine as a marker of cigarette smoking.
  • Nested case-control studies allow evaluation of
    exposures prior to case status

24
1.2 Interviewer Bias
  • May occur when interviewers are not blinded to
    disease status.
  • They may probe more
  • Interviewers may be biased toward the study
    hypothesis (or have other biases).
  • They may ignore protocols
  • Prevent or assess with reliability/validity
    substudies (phantom studies)
  • eg. Doll and Hill study of lung cancer and smoking

25
2. Outcome Identification Bias
  • May occur in both case-control or cohort studies
  • Problems in the collection of outcome
    measurements.
  • Two main examples
  • Observer bias
  • Respondent bias

26
2.1 Observer Bias
  • In a Cohort study decision to classify outcome
    may be affected by knowledge of exposure status.
    Especially soft outcomes such as migraine, or
    psychiatric symptoms
  • Eg assignment of diagnosis of hypertensive
    end-stage renal disease (ESRD). Nephrologists
    sent case histories were twice as likely to
    diagnose black patients with ESRD than white
    patients.

27
Preventing Observer Bias
  • Mask observers in charge of classifying outcome
    with respect to exposure status
  • Multiple observers

28
2.2 Respondent Bias
  • Most often examples are in case-control studies
    where biases are associated with identification
    of exposure.
  • Eg Parents may be more likely to identify having
    given their children aspirin in a case-control
    study of Reyes syndrome compared to population
    controls

29
2.2 Respondent Bias
  • In a Cohort study respondents may respond with
    little consistency to un-standardized questions
    or to subjective questions.
  • Eg. Questions about depression may be very
    subjective. A solution is to use a standardized
    instrument.
  • Eg. 2 Have you witnessed an overdose?

30
3. The result of information bias
Misclassification
  • Nondifferential misclassification
  • Differential misclassification

31
3.1 Nondifferential Misclassification
  • Occurs when the degree of misclassification of
    exposure is independent of case-control status
    (vice-versa)
  • Example misclassification of HCV infection due
    to window period in a study looking at risk
    factors for HCV. Both the exposed and the
    unexposed are equally likely to be misclassified
    as unexposed.

32
Misclassification of OUTCOME
REFERENCE POPULATION
Diseased
-
-
Exposed
Cases Control
STUDY SAMPLE
Egcases in this are mistakenly classified as
controls due to low sensitivity on a screening
test
33
Nondifferential Misclassification
  • Example misclassification of exposed subjects
    as unexposed in 30 of cases and 30 of controls

No misclassification Exposure Cases Controls Yes
50 20 No 50 80 OR (50/50)/(20/80) or
(5080)/(5020) 4.0 30 Exposure
misclassification in each group Exposure Cases Co
ntrols Yes 50-1535 20-614 No 501565 80686
OR (3586)/(6514) 3.3 Effect of
non-differential misclassification with 2
exposure categories to bias the OR toward the
null value of 1.0
34
Application of sensitivity/specificity concepts
in misclassification of exposure schematic
representation of true and misclassified relative
odds. Sensitivity of exposure ascertainment
TP ? (TP FN)Specificity of exposure
ascertainment TN ?(TN FP)
Cases Controls True Exp
Unexp Exp Unexp OR
(pos) (neg) (pos) (neg) True
results A C B C
A/C? B/D Study
total study total study
MIS- Results
cases
controls classified OR Exp TP FP
TP FP a TP FP TP
FP b Unexp FN TN FN TN c
FN TN FN TN d a/c?b/d
Figure 4-4
35
Effects of nondifferential misclassification on
the odds ratio--gtSince the sensitivity and
specificity values are the same for cases and
control,the effect is nondifferential
Cases Controls
True exp
unexp exp unexp
OR true distribution (gold
standard) 50 50
20 80
50/50?20/80 4.0 Study distribution
Cases
Controls Exposed 45 10
55 18 16
34 Unexposed 5 40
45 2 64 66
55/45?34/66 2.4 sensitivity (exp) or
specificity (unexp) 0.90 0.80
0.90 0.80
Misclassified OR
Exhibit 4-3
36
Nondifferential misclassification of exposure
effects of sensitivity and specificity of
exposure identification and of exposure
prevalence in controls on a studys OR (true
OR4.0)
Table 4-5
37
3.2 Differential Misclassification
  • Occurs when the degree of misclassification of
    exposure (outcome) differs between the groups
    being outcome (exposure) groups
  • Effect is bias toward or away from the null

38
Effect of differential misclassification on the
OR, in which, for Sensitivity Cases gt Controls
and, for specificity, Cases Controls
Cases Controls
True exp
unexp exp unexp
OR true distribution (gold
standard) 50 50 20
80 50/50?20/80
4.0 Study distribution Cases
Controls Exposed 48
0 48 14 0 14
Unexposed 2 50 52
6 80 86
48/52?14/86 5.7 sensitivity (exp) or
specificity (unexp) 0.96 1.00
0.70 1.00
Misclassified OR
Exhibit 4-5
39
Effect of differential misclassification on the
OR, in which, for both Sensitivity and
Specificity, CasesgtControls
Cases Controls
True exp
unexp exp unexp
OR true distribution (gold
standard) 50 50 20
80 50/50?20/80
4.0 Study distribution Cases
Controls Exposed 48
0 48 14 16 30
Unexposed 2 50 52
6 64 70
48/52?30/70 2.1 sensitivity (exp) or
specificity (unexp) 0.96 1.00
0.70 0.80
Misclassified OR
Exhibit 4-6
40
Example Weinstock et als study of melanoma
associated with hair color and tanning
ability. Table 4-8
  • Nested case-control study allowed data collection
    at baseline and during follow-up
  • Participants were compared with regard to their
    report of hair color and tanning ability
  • Compared with pre-disease development, the odds
    for hair color use increased slightly only for
    controls when the postmelanoma interview data
    were used Result OR changed very little.
  • Differential misclassification of tanning ability
    was severe, leading to a reversal in the
    direction of the association.

41
Cases 143 without a prior history of cancer who
reported a melanoma (June 1976 to June 1984)
responded to mailed questionnaire or telephone
interview. Controls 316 age-matched controls
randomly sampled from the NHS cohort members
w/out history of cancer
42
(No Transcript)
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com