Young childrens verbal and nonverbal responses to mislabel teases - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 1
About This Presentation
Title:

Young childrens verbal and nonverbal responses to mislabel teases

Description:

Young children's verbal and nonverbal responses to mislabel teases ... Thus some tease episodes contained more than one response. ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:158
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 2
Provided by: debu3
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Young childrens verbal and nonverbal responses to mislabel teases


1
Young childrens verbal and nonverbal responses
to mislabel teases Debra L. Burnett, Linda M.
Milosky, Stefanie Hayes and Rachel
Neuman Department of Communication Sciences and
Disorders, Syracuse University
Poster 1107 Board 198
Results
Abstract Nonliteral language comprehension in
young children can be examined through playful
teasing between parents and children. Twenty-one
children, ages 16-35 months, and their mothers
were videotaped during play. Mother-initiated
mislabeling teases were identified and examined
for childrens correct and incorrect verbal and
nonverbal responses. Analyses reveal that
children did signal teasing comprehension of
mislabel teases either verbally (e.g., protesting
and/or correcting the mothers incorrect label)
or nonverbally (e.g., pointing to the correct
picture). Children demonstrated, by challenging
the mother, they knew the information was false
and often provided the correct information.
  • Characterization of mislabel teases
  • A total of 159 mislabel tease episodes were
    demonstrated by 21 mothers.
  • Mothers averaged 7.57 mislabel tease episodes
    with a range of 1-19 episodes. Each episode
    ranged in length from 0.67 to 165.0 seconds.
  • The children averaged 2.69 responses per tease
    episode with a range of 1-12.
  • Of the sample, 55 of mislabels occurred while
    playing with objects and 40 occurred during
    bookreading.
  • Of the 159 mislabel teases, 69.9 were initiated
    in the form of a question.
  • Findings for entire corpus
  • The corpus consists of 427 responses coded from
    159 tease episodes (see Figure 1 for
    proportions). Thus some tease episodes contained
    more than one response.
  • Of the 159 tease episodes, 49.7 (n126) had at
    least one correct verbal response and 30.2
    (n48) had at least one incorrect verbal
    response.
  • At least one nonverbal response, occurring in the
    absence of a verbal response, was used correctly
    6.9 (n11) and incorrectly 2.5 (n4) during the
    159 tease episodes.

Introduction
  • Playful teasing is defined as exchanges in which
    the speaker intentionally provokes the target in
    a playful, nonthreatening way with the use of
    verbal and/or nonverbal off-record markers such
    as facial expression (Keltner, Capps, Kring,
    Young, Heerey, 2001).
  • Playful teasing is a means by which young
    children can demonstrate the ability to resolve
    ambiguities in their social interactions. Miller
    (1986) stated that teasing may be used to show
    children how to play with language and how
    language is related to the world around them.
  • The present study examines the nature of young
    childrens verbal and nonverbal responses, which
    may signal either tease comprehension or
    non-comprehension, as this has yet to be
    thoroughly explored in the literature.
  • The present study used a seminaturalistic
    methodology to answer the following questions
  • (1) What behaviors, verbal and nonverbal, do
    young children demonstrate in response to
    parent-initiated teases that indicate successful
    teasing comprehension?
  • (2) Is successful teasing comprehension related
    to language abilities as assessed by the
    MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory?
  • Nonverbal responses to mislabel teases
  • Nonverbal responses were coded as correct if they
    indicated clear comprehension such as nodding the
    head yes or no or using gestures such as pointing
    to the mislabeled or correctly labeled picture.
    Less than a third of the children used a
    nonverbal response instead of a verbal response
    to being teased (see Figure 2). Only one child
    used nonverbal responses exclusively.
  • Many of the children (n15) demonstrated a
    nonverbal response that was ambiguous or did not
    definitively indicate comprehension such as
    puzzled looks, reaching for the toy,
    smiling/laughing especially if mother was already
    smiling/laughing.
  • In addition, 10/21 children had at least one
    tease where there was no response indicating that
    they were not attending to the tease.
  • Verbal responses to mislabel teases
  • The majority of children demonstrated at least
    one verbal response to being teased (see Figure
    2).
  • 11 children demonstrated both correct and
    incorrect verbal responses while 4 children
    demonstrated only correct verbal responses and 4
    children demonstrated only incorrect verbal
    responses. Of these 8 children, 3 participated
    in only one mislabeling tease episode.

Methods
  • A subset of 21 dyads, children ages 16 through
    35 months (M24), from urban and suburban areas
    of the greater Syracuse area was used for the
    current analyses since these mothers demonstrated
    mislabel teases. The dyads were part of a larger
    study examining playful teasing between mothers
    and young children (Burnett Milosky, 2005).
  • Each mother-child dyad was seen for one two-hour
    session at home involving an examiner-generated
  • interview regarding teasing practices (Heerey,
    Capps, Keltner, Kring, 2005) and 20-45 minutes
    of
  • play in which the mother demonstrated typical
    playful teasing exchanges.
  • The MacArthur Communication Development Inventory
    (Fenson, Dale, Reznick,Thal, Bates, Hartung,
  • Pethick Reilly, 1993) and the Ages and
    Stages Questionnaires (Squires, Potter Bricker,
    1999)
  • were completed.
  • Sessions were videotaped and the middle 20
    minutes were analyzed for the presence of playful
    teases. Of the different types identified
    (interrater reliability was 80 for identifying
    tease types), object/attribute mislabel teases
    (e.g., calling a dog a kitty) were selected for
    the current analyses.
  • Mislabel teases were coded for childrens
    responses to teases initiated by the mother as
    verbal/nonverbal, correct/incorrect,
    ambiguous/unambiguous with interrater reliability
    at 87.
  • Of the 21 children, potential relationships were
    explored between performance on the MCDI and
    teasing comprehension for 20 children. One
    childs mother did not complete the MCDI.

Conclusions
  • Teasing comprehension and language ability
  • Potential relationships between language ability,
    as measured by the MCDI, and tease responses were
    explored for 20 participants using correlation
    analyses but no overall findings were
    significant.
  • Of the 159 teases, mothers ended the tease with a
    sincere question (e.g., What does a pig say?
    after mislabeling the pig) 53.5 of the time
    perhaps to make sure children knew the correct
    label and the prior teasing intent before ending
    the episode.
  • The majority of childrens responses to mislabel
    teases were verbal but nonverbal responses
    accounted for at least one of the responses for a
    third of the current sample. Further analysis of
    smiling and laughter as teasing responses will be
    of future interest.
  • Approximately half of the responses were correct
    which could indicate that children are still
    acquiring the ability to recognize and/or respond
    correctly to mislabel teasing episodes during the
    early preschool years or given some mothers high
    frequency of mislabel teases within 20 minutes
    (e.g., 19), children may not have felt obligated
    to respond to each tease.
  • While no relationship between the MCDI and tease
    responses was found during these preliminary
    findings, a measure of language comprehension
    should be employed since verbal and nonverbal
    responses tap comprehension ability.

Acknowledgements
References
Example of mislabel tease Mother says, Is that
Melvin? while coloring a picture of Scooby Doo
in a coloring book. Child responds, Scooby Doo.
Burnett, D. and Milosky, L. (2005).
Language-based playful teasing in mother-child
interactions. Poster presented at ASHA
Convention, San Diego, CA. Fenson, L., Dale,
P.S., Reznick, J.S., Thal, D., Bates, E.,
Hartung, J., Pethick, S., and Reilly, J. (1993)
MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories
Users Guide and Technical Manual. San Diego, CA
Singular Publishing Group. Heerey, E., Capps, L.,
Keltner, D. and Kring, A. (2005). Understanding
teases lessons from children with autism.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33,
55-68. Keltner, D., Capps, L., Kring, A., Young,
R., and Heerey, E. (2001). Just teasing a
conceptual analysis and empirical review.
Psychological Bulletin, 127, 2, 229-248. Miller,
P. (1986). Teasing as language socialization and
verbal play in a white working-class community.
In B.B. Schieffelin and E. Ochs (Eds.), Language
socialization across cultures (pp. 199-211).
Cambridge Cambridge University Press. Squires,
J., Potter, L., and Bricker, D. (1999). Ages and
Stages Questionnaires A Parent-Completed,
Child-Monitoring System, 2nd Edition. Baltimore,
MD Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.
The research was supported in part by an ASHA
Special Interest Division 1 Language Learning
and Education Student Research Grant to the first
author. Special thanks to Nicole Yau, Rachel
Robertson, Renail Richards, Alexine Leon, Emily
DeSalvo, and Hannah Burke for their help with
data analysis Currently affiliated with Boston
University.
For further information contact Debra Burnett at
deburnet_at_syr.edu
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com