Large Scale Studies of Dyslexia in Florida - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 83
About This Presentation
Title:

Large Scale Studies of Dyslexia in Florida

Description:

Large Scale Studies of Dyslexia in Florida – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:107
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 84
Provided by: david1873
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Large Scale Studies of Dyslexia in Florida


1
Large Scale Studies of Dyslexia in Florida
  • Richard K. Wagner and Yusra Ahmed
  • Florida State University and FCRR
  • NIH Multidisciplinary Learning Disabilities
    Center (P50 HD052120)

2
Alternative Approaches
  • 1. Typical research study.
  • 2. Meta-analysis.
  • 3. Large-scale study.

3
Large-Scale Study of Incidence of Specific
Reading Comprehension Disability
4
Personal Interest in Reading Comprehension
Problems
  • Comprehension errors run in my family.

5
Ellis Island
6
Question on a Form
  • If admitted to this country, would you advocate
    overthrow of the government of the United States
    by force or violence?

7
Question on a Form
  • If admitted to this country, would you advocate
    overthrow of the government of the United States
    by force or violence?
  • violence

8
Reported Incidence of Reading Comprehension
Disability
  • 10 percent of 7-11 year-olds are poor at reading
    comprehension despite being accurate and fluent
    at decoding (Nation, 2004).

9
Many Possible Causes
  • There is room for lots of things to go wrong
    when comprehension fails (Perfetti, 1994, p.
    885, cited by Nation, 2005).

10
Possible Causes (Science of Reading A Handbook)
  • 1. Decoding difficulties.
  • 2. Difficulties with meaning (vocabulary).
  • 3. Difficulty with syntax.
  • 4. Limitations in working memory.
  • 5. Poor inference making.
  • 6. Inadequate comprehension monitoring.
  • 7. Limited prior domain knowledge.
  • 8. Insensitivity to text structure.

11
Reading Comprehension Task
  • When the next slide appears, read the text as
    quickly as you can and summarize the passage in a
    brief sentence.
  • Ready?

12
(No Transcript)
13
Answer a Simple Question
  • Is necrobiosis a conceivable source of orogeny or
    of a pomiferous pompelmous?

14
Simple Question
  • Is necrobiosis conceivably related to orogeny or
    to the development of a pomiferous pompelmous?
  • Obviously the normal death of cells (necrobiosis)
    is not related to mountain building (orogeny),
    but is part of the process of the development of
    fruit bearing (pomiferous) trees (pompelmous).

15
Ordering Causes by Severity of Consequences for
Comprehension
  • Primary
  • Decoding difficulties.
  • Secondary
  • Difficulties with meaning (vocabulary).
  • Tertiary
  • Difficulty with syntax limitations in working
    memory poor inference making inadequate
    comprehension monitoring limited prior domain
    knowledge insensitivity to text structure.

16
Present Studies
  • Question Addressed
  • What is the incidence of reading comprehension
    disability not attributable to primary or
    secondary causes?
  • Design feature
  • Attempted to address issue of small samples sizes
    of typical studies by using the PMRN (progress
    monitoring and reporting network) database.

17
Study Design
  • 1. Identify individuals who are poor at reading
    comprehension.
  • Score at or below 5th percentile on Stanford
    Achievement Test (SAT-10) Reading Comprehension.

18
Study Design
  • 2. Determine how many individuals are poor at
    comprehension yet adequate at decoding.
  • SAT 10 at or below 5th percentile
  • DIBELS Nonsense Word Fluency (NWF) greater than
    or equal to 25th percentile.

19
Study Design
  • 2. Determine how many individuals are poor at
    comprehension yet adequate at decoding and
    vocabulary.
  • SAT 10 at or below 5th percentile
  • DIBELS Nonsense Word Fluency (NWF) greater than
    or equal to 25th percentile.
  • Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) greater
    than or equal to 25th percentile.

20
Out of a First Grade-Cohort 1 (N 35,314).
21
How Many Were Poor at
  • Reading comprehension (SAT 10 lt 5th)
  • 1,669 (4.73) out of 35,314

22
How Many Were Poor at
  • Reading comprehension (SAT 10 lt 5th )
  • 1,669 (4.73) out of 35,314
  • yet adequate at decoding (nonword fluency gt
    25th )?

23
How Many Were Poor at
  • Reading comprehension (SAT 10 lt 5th )
  • 1,669 (4.73) out of 35,314
  • yet adequate at decoding (nonword fluency gt
    25th )?
  • only 85 (0.24) out of 35,314!

24
How Many Were Poor at
  • Reading comprehension (SAT 10 lt 5th )
  • 1,669 (4.73) out of 35,314
  • yet adequate at decoding (nonword fluency gt
    25th )?
  • only 85 (0.24) out of 35,314!
  • and in vocabulary (PPVT gt 25th )?

25
How Many Were Poor at
  • Reading comprehension (SAT 10 lt 5th )
  • 1,669 (4.73) out of 35,314
  • yet adequate at decoding (nonword fluency gt
    25th )?
  • only 85 (0.24) out of 35,314!
  • and in vocabulary (PPVT gt 25th )?
  • only 23 (0.07) out of 35,314!

26
Surprising Result Virtually All Had Problems in
Decoding
  • Sample size was 35,314.
  • But it was a single study. Results need to be
    replicated.

27
So We Did Replicate Not One, Not Two, but
28
Three First-Grade Cohorts
29
First Grade Results
  • Of 150,000 6-year-olds, only .2 to .5 percent
    were poor at comprehension yet adequate at
    decoding.
  • May be nature of reading comprehension at age
    6decoding explains about everything.

30
Three Second-Grade Cohorts
31
Comparing Second-Grade Results to First-Grade
Results
  • Second-grade results differ a little
  • Percentage of children who are poor at reading
    comprehension yet adequate at decoding is about 2
    percent, compared to .5 percent in first grade.
  • But identical when adequate vocabulary is also
    imposed
  • Less than .2 percent for both.

32
What about Third Grade?
  • We could not do identical study because we dont
    have nonsense word fluency (NWF) as decoding
    measure for third grade.
  • But we do have Gates-McGinnite reading vocabulary
    as a combined measure of decoding and vocabulary.

33
Three Third-Grade Cohorts
34
What About Less Severe Reading Comprehension
Problems
  • Select if SAT 10 lt 20th ile.
  • Require decoding and vocabulary 1 standard
    deviation higher (56th ile) or just somewhat
    higher (40th ile).
  • Representative second-grade results for most
    lenient (40th ile) criterion.

35
Less Severe 2nd Grade Cohorts
36
What These Results Say
  • Specific reading comprehension not associated
    with presence of primary (decoding) or secondary
    (vocabulary) causes is exceedingly rare
  • Less than 0.1 of first-graders.
  • Less than 0.2 of second-graders.
  • Less than 0.3 of third-graders.

37
What These Results Dont Say
  • Results dont imply that individuals poor at
    reading comprehension dont also show deficits in
    various tertiary factors.

38
What These Results Dont Say
  • Results dont imply that individuals poor at
    reading comprehension dont also show deficits in
    various tertiary factors.
  • decoding gains from intervention rarely translate
    into equivalent gains in comprehension.

39
Conclusions
  • Individuals with tertiary causes (e.g.,
    metacognitive deficiency) in absence of primary
    (decoding) and secondary (vocabulary) causes are
    rare.

40
Conclusions
  • For screening purposes, a combination of decoding
    and vocabulary should be remarkably effective.

41
Conclusions
  • Its worse not to know the words (primary problem
    in decoding or secondary problem in decoding
    vocabulary) than to not know whether you know the
    words (tertiary problem in metacognition).

42
Gender Differences in Reading Disability
Reasons to Care
  • 1. An active and controversial issue.
  • 2. Gender bias in identification and provision
    of services may be pervasive.
  • 3. Implications for theories of etiology.
  • 4. New approaches to identification being
    considered potentially could mitigate referral
    bias if it exists.
  • Ex. Universal screening as front end of RTI.

43
Current Controversy Two Views
  • 1. Male vulnerability is a myth.
  • observed ratios of 21 or 31 in clinics and
    classrooms reflect referral bias.
  • true ratio is 11 or boys favored only minimally.

44
Key Study Shaywitz et al. (1990)
  • Obtained both school-identified ratio and
    objective ratio for same sample.
  • Statistically significant ratio of 2.21 found
    for school identified ratio.
  • Non-significant ratio of 1.41 found for
    objective criteria.
  • Sample size modest however (18 boys versus 13
    girls with RD).
  • A 21 ratio would not even be significant.

45
Current Controversy Two Views
  • 2. Male vulnerability is real.
  • males roughly twice as likely to be affected.

46
Key Supporting Studies
  • Liederman et al., 2005, review of the literature.
  • Ratios ranged from 1.21 to 6.81.
  • Concluded that true ratio was between 1.71 and
    21.

47
Key Questions
  • 1. What is the magnitude of male vulnerability
    for reading disability if it exists?
  • Answered by examining gender ratios for
    research-based operational definitions applied
    universally.

48
Key Questions
  • 2. How accurately does school-identification
    predict research-based identification?
  • Answered by classification analyses that use
    school-identification to predict research-based
    identification.

49
Key Questions
  • 3. What is the magnitude of referral bias if it
    exists? Answered by three empirical analyses
  • A. Magnitude of difference in gender ratios for
    school-identified versus research identified
    samples.
  • B. Less overlap between school- and
    research-identification for boys than for girls.
  • C. Lower mean performance for girls compared to
    boys for school-identified samples.

50
Key Questions
  • 4. Do gender ratios vary as a function of
  • A. Level of severity of reading problem?
  • Studies differ on level used.
  • B. Whether operational definition is based on
    low-achievement or IQ-achievement discrepancy?
  • Some suggestion that gender differences occur for
    IQ-discrepancy but not low-achievement
    definitions.
  • C. The kind of reading measure examined?

51
Sample
  • Five cohorts of beginning second-grade students
    in Reading First schools in Florida (03/04,
    04/05, 05/06, 06/07, 07/08).
  • N 491

52
Sample
  • Five cohorts of beginning second-grade students
    in Reading First schools in Florida (03/04,
    04/05, 05/06, 06/07, 07/08).
  • N 491 thousand (491,000)!

53
Measures
  • Phonological decoding Nonword fluency (DIBELS).
  • Reading connected test Oral reading fluency
    (DIBELS).
  • IQ proxy Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

54
Remaining Design Issues
  • Compared four levels of severity
  • 30th percentile.
  • 15th percentile.
  • 5th percentile.
  • 3rd percentile.

55
Remaining Design Issues
  • Low achievement operational definition determined
    by identifying the 4 target percentiles in the
    distributions.
  • Discrepancy operational definition determined by
    regression reading measures of PPVT and
    identifying same target percentiles in the
    distributions of residuals.

56
Key Question 1 Male
Vulnerability?
57
Gender Ratios for Research-Based Definitions
  • Ratios greater than 11 quantity extent of male
    vulnerability.

58
Gender Ratios Low-Achievement Definition,
Nonword Fluency
59
Gender Ratios Discrepancy Definition, Nonword
Fluency
60
Gender Ratios Low-Achievement Definition, Oral
Reading Fluency
61
Gender Ratios Discrepancy Definition, Oral
Reading Fluency
62
Comparing Low-Achievement and Discrepancy
Definitions Nonword Fluency
63
Comparing Low-Achievement and Discrepancy
Definitions ORF
64
Key Question 2 Accuracy of
School-Based Identification?
65
School-Identification as Learning Disabled
  • 5.1 percent of all second-grade students.
  • Carried out classification study using
    school-identification to predict membership in
    5th -ile research-based group.

66
Key Classification Statistics
  • 1. Sensitivity.
  • Proportion of research-based reading disabled
    correctly classified by school-based
    determination.
  • 2. Specificity.
  • Proportion of research-based reading non-disabled
    correctly classified by school-based
    determination.

67
Key Classification Statistics
  • 3. Positive Predictive Value.
  • Proportion of school-identified reading disabled
    who actually were according to research-based
    criteria.
  • 4. Negative Predictive Value.
  • Proportion of school-identified non-reading
    disabled who actually were according to
    research-based criteria.

68
Guidelines for Interpretation of Classification
Statistics
  • Level 3 (highest level of support)
  • Sensitivity, specificity, and positive predictive
    value gt .75.
  • Level 2 (moderate support)
  • Sensitivity, specificity, and positive predictive
    value gt .70.
  • Level 1 (lowest but still supportive)
  • Sensitivity and specificity or sensitivity and
    positive predictive value gt .70.

69
Classification Results
70
Classification Results
  • Accuracy of school-based identification poor for
    research-based identification of reading
    disability.
  • Accuracy is high for determination of non reading
    disability, but this reflects base rate of 5.

71
Key Question 3 Referral Bias?
72
Comparing Gender Ratios for School- and
Research-Identified Samples
  • Extent to which gender ratios for
    school-identified samples exceed those for
    research-identified quantifies extent of
    referral bias.

73
Comparing Gender Ratios for School- and
Research-Identified Samples
  • Gender ratio for school-identified sample 2.25
    to 1.
  • Exceeds gender ratio for nonword fluency (1.51
    or 1.61 depending on definition, for same level
    of severity).
  • Comparable to gender ratio for oral reading
    fluency (2.11 or 2.41).

74
Is Accuracy of School-Identification Higher for
Girls than Boys?
  • If boys are referred for behavior and other
    issues and girls for reading problems, accuracy
    of school-based identification should be higher
    for girls than for boys.

75
Is Accuracy of School-Identification Higher for
Girls than Boys?
76
Lower Mean Performance for School-Identified
Girls than Boys?
  • If boys are referred for behavior and other
    issues and girls for reading problems, girls
    should be poorer readers than boys in
    school-identified samples.

77
Lower Mean Performance for School-Identified
Girls than Boys?
78
Lower Mean Performance for School-Identified
Girls than Boys?
79
Conclusions
  • 1. Male vulnerability is real and quantifiable.
  • Increases with level of severity of reading
    problem.
  • Greater for broader-based oral reading fluency
    (2.41 at 3rd -ile) than for more narrowly-based
    nonword fluency (1.71 at 3rd -ile).

80
Conclusions
  • 2. Accuracy of school-identification is abysmal
    for research-based criteria.
  • Sensitivities ranged from .14 to .26 (far below
    minimally acceptable value of .70).

81
Conclusions
  • 3. Little support for substantial referral bias.
  • Gender ratios for school-identified samples
    greater than research-based for nonword fluency
    but not for oral reading fluency.
  • School identification is not more accurate for
    girls than for boys, but just the opposite.
  • School-identified girls are not poorer readers
    than boys, but just the opposite.

82
Conclusions
  • 4. Gender ratios not greater for discrepancy
    than for low achievement definitions for nonword
    fluency only marginally higher (from 0.21 to
    0.41) higher for oral reading fluency.

83
Questions?
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com