Title: Graphical models Tom Griffiths UC Berkeley
1Graphical modelsTom GriffithsUC Berkeley
2Challenges of probabilistic models
- Specifying well-defined probabilistic models with
many variables is hard (for modelers) - Representing probability distributions over those
variables is hard (for computers/learners) - Computing quantities using those distributions is
hard (for computers/learners)
3Representing structured distributions
- Four random variables
- X1 coin toss produces heads
- X2 pencil levitates
- X3 friend has psychic powers
- X4 friend has two-headed coin
Domain 0,1 0,1 0,1 0,1
4Joint distribution
0000 0001 0010 0011 0100 0101 0110 0111 1000 1001
1010 1011 1100 1101 1110 1111
- Requires 15 numbers to specify probability of all
values x1,x2,x3,x4 - N binary variables, 2N-1 numbers
- Similar cost when computing conditional
probabilities
5How can we use fewer numbers?
- Four random variables
- X1 coin toss produces heads
- X2 coin toss produces heads
- X3 coin toss produces heads
- X4 coin toss produces heads
Domain 0,1 0,1 0,1 0,1
6Statistical independence
- Two random variables X1 and X2 are independent if
P(x1x2) P(x1) - e.g. coinflips P(x1Hx2H) P(x1H) 0.5
- Independence makes it easier to represent and
work with probability distributions - We can exploit the product rule
If x1, x2, x3, and x4 are all independent
7Expressing independence
- Statistical independence is the key to efficient
probabilistic representation and computation - This has led to the development of languages for
indicating dependencies among variables - Some of the most popular languages are based on
graphical models
8Graphical models
- Introduction to graphical models
- definitions
- efficient representation and inference
- explaining away
- Graphical models and cognitive science
- uses of graphical models
9Graphical models
- Introduction to graphical models
- definitions
- efficient representation and inference
- explaining away
- Graphical models and cognitive science
- uses of graphical models
10Graphical models
- Express the probabilistic dependency structure
among a set of variables (Pearl, 1988) - Consist of
- a set of nodes, corresponding to variables
- a set of edges, indicating dependency
- a set of functions defined on the graph that
specify a probability distribution
11Undirected graphical models
X3
X4
X1
- Consist of
- a set of nodes
- a set of edges
- a potential for each clique, multiplied together
to yield the distribution over variables - Examples
- statistical physics Ising model, spinglasses
- neural networks (e.g. Boltzmann machines)
X2
X5
12Ising models
X1
X2
- Consist of
- a set of nodes
- a set of edges
- a potential for each clique, multiplied together
to yield the distribution over variables - Distribution is specified as
X3
X4
13Ising models
14Boltzmann machines
X3
X4
X1
- Consist of
- a set of nodes
- a set of edges
- a potential for each clique, multiplied together
to yield the distribution over variables - Distribution is specified as
X2
X5
15Boltzmann machines
True image
Boltzmann
PCA
PCA
Boltzmann
(Hinton Salakhutdinov, 2006)
16Directed graphical models
X3
X4
X1
- Consist of
- a set of nodes
- a set of edges
- a conditional probability distribution for each
node, conditioned on its parents, multiplied
together to yield the distribution over variables - Constrained to directed acyclic graphs (DAGs)
- Called Bayesian networks or Bayes nets
X2
X5
17Bayesian networks and Bayes
- Two different problems
- Bayesian statistics is a method of inference
- Bayesian networks are a form of representation
- There is no necessary connection
- many users of Bayesian networks rely upon
frequentist statistical methods - many Bayesian inferences cannot be easily
represented using Bayesian networks
18Graphical models
- Introduction to graphical models
- definitions
- efficient representation and inference
- explaining away
- Graphical models and cognitive science
- uses of graphical models
19Efficient representation and inference
- Four random variables
- X1 coin toss produces heads
- X2 pencil levitates
- X3 friend has psychic powers
- X4 friend has two-headed coin
20The Markov assumption
- Every node is conditionally independent of its
non-descendants, given its parents
where Pa(Xi) is the set of parents of Xi
(via the product rule)
21Efficient representation and inference
- Four random variables
- X1 coin toss produces heads
- X2 pencil levitates
- X3 friend has psychic powers
- X4 friend has two-headed coin
1
1
4
2
total 8 (vs 15)
P(x1, x2, x3, x4) P(x1x3, x4)P(x2x3)P(x3)P(x4)
22Reading a Bayesian network
- The structure of a Bayes net can be read as the
generative process behind a distribution - Gives the joint probability distribution over
variables obtained by sampling each variable
conditioned on its parents
23Reading a Bayesian network
- Four random variables
- X1 coin toss produces heads
- X2 pencil levitates
- X3 friend has psychic powers
- X4 friend has two-headed coin
X3
X4
X1
X2
P(x1, x2, x3, x4) P(x1x3, x4)P(x2x3)P(x3)P(x4)
24Reading a Bayesian network
- The structure of a Bayes net can be read as the
generative process behind a distribution - Gives the joint probability distribution over
variables obtained by sampling each variable
conditioned on its parents - Simple rules for determining whether two
variables are dependent or independent
25Identifying independence
X1 and X3 dependent
X1 and X3 independent
X2
X2
X2
X2
X2
X2
(shaded variables are observed)
26Identifying independence
- Four random variables
- X1 coin toss produces heads
- X2 pencil levitates
- X3 friend has psychic powers
- X4 friend has two-headed coin
X4 and X2 are independent
X3
X4
X3
X4
X1
X2
X1
X2
X4 and X2 are independent
X4 and X2 are dependent
27Reading a Bayesian network
- The structure of a Bayes net can be read as the
generative process behind a distribution - Gives the joint probability distribution over
variables obtained by sampling each variable
conditioned on its parents - Simple rules for determining whether two
variables are dependent or independent - Independence makes inference more efficient
28Computing with Bayes nets
X3
X4
X1
X2
P(x1, x2, x3, x4) P(x1x3, x4)P(x2x3)P(x3)P(x4)
29Computing with Bayes nets
sum over 8 values
P(x1, x2, x3, x4) P(x1x3, x4)P(x2x3)P(x3)P(x4)
30Computing with Bayes nets
P(x1, x2, x3, x4) P(x1x3, x4)P(x2x3)P(x3)P(x4)
31Computing with Bayes nets
sum over 4 values
P(x1, x2, x3, x4) P(x1x3, x4)P(x2x3)P(x3)P(x4)
32Computing with Bayes nets
- Inference algorithms for Bayesian networks
exploit dependency structure - Message-passing algorithms
- belief propagation passes simple messages
between nodes, exact for tree-structured networks - More general inference algorithms
- exact junction-tree
- approximate Monte Carlo schemes
33Logic and probability
- Bayesian networks are equivalent to a
probabilistic propositional logic - Associate variables with atomic propositions
- Bayes net specifies a distribution over possible
worlds, probability of a proposition is a sum
over worlds - More efficient than simply enumerating worlds
- Developing similarly efficient schemes for
working with other probabilistic logics is a
major topic of current research
34Graphical models
- Introduction to graphical models
- definitions
- efficient representation and inference
- explaining away
- Graphical models and cognitive science
- uses of graphical models
35Identifying independence
X1 and X3 dependent
X1 and X3 independent
X2
X2
X2
X2
X2
X2
(shaded variables are observed)
36Explaining away
- Assume grass will be wet if and only if it
rained last night, or if the sprinklers were left
on
37Explaining away
Compute probability it rained last night, given
that the grass is wet
38Explaining away
Compute probability it rained last night, given
that the grass is wet
39Explaining away
Compute probability it rained last night, given
that the grass is wet
40Explaining away
Compute probability it rained last night, given
that the grass is wet
41Explaining away
Compute probability it rained last night, given
that the grass is wet
42Explaining away
Compute probability it rained last night, given
that the grass is wet and sprinklers were left
on
43Explaining away
Compute probability it rained last night, given
that the grass is wet and sprinklers were left
on
44Explaining away
Discounting to prior probability.
45Contrast w/ production system
Rain
Grass Wet
- Formulate IF-THEN rules
- IF Rain THEN Wet
- IF Wet THEN Rain
- Rules do not distinguish directions of inference
- Requires combinatorial explosion of rules
46Contrast w/ spreading activation
Rain
Sprinkler
Grass Wet
- Observing rain, Wet becomes more active.
- Observing grass wet, Rain and Sprinkler become
more active - Observing grass wet and sprinkler, Rain cannot
become less active. No explaining away!
- Excitatory links Rain Wet, Sprinkler
Wet
47Contrast w/ spreading activation
Rain
Sprinkler
Grass Wet
- Excitatory links Rain Wet, Sprinkler
Wet - Inhibitory link Rain Sprinkler
- Observing grass wet, Rain and Sprinkler become
more active - Observing grass wet and sprinkler, Rain becomes
less active explaining away
48Contrast w/ spreading activation
Rain
Burst pipe
Sprinkler
Grass Wet
- Each new variable requires more inhibitory
connections - Not modular
- whether a connection exists depends on what
others exist - big holism problem
- combinatorial explosion
49Contrast w/ spreading activation
(McClelland Rumelhart, 1981)
50Graphical models
- Capture dependency structure in distributions
- Provide an efficient means of representing and
reasoning with probabilities - Support kinds of inference that are problematic
for other cognitive models explaining away - hard to capture in a production system
- more natural than with spreading activation
51Graphical models
- Introduction to graphical models
- definitions
- efficient representation and inference
- explaining away
- Graphical models and cognitive science
- uses of graphical models
52Uses of graphical models
- Understanding existing cognitive models
- e.g., neural network models
53Sigmoid belief networks
y
- We can view multilayer perceptrons as Bayes nets
with specific probabilities - (e.g., Neal, 1992)
- Makes it possible to use Bayesian tools with
existing neural network models - (e.g., Mackay, 1992)
z1
z2
x1
x2
54Uses of graphical models
- Understanding existing cognitive models
- e.g., neural network models
- Representation and reasoning
- a way to address holism in induction (c.f. Fodor)
55The holism of confirmation
- If everything we know is one big probability
distribution, then discovering one small fact
requires changing all of our beliefs - Used by Fodor (2001) as an argument against the
possibility of inductive logic - Bayes nets everything can be connected to
everything, but inference can still be efficient
vs.
56Uses of graphical models
- Understanding existing cognitive models
- e.g., neural network models
- Representation and reasoning
- a way to address holism in induction (c.f. Fodor)
- Defining generative models
- mixture models, language models,
57Graphical models and coinflipping
q
d1
d2
d3
d4
d1
d2
d3
d4
d1
d2
d3
d4
Hidden Markov model si Fair coin, Trick
coin
Fair coin P(H) 0.5
P(H) q
58A hierarchical Bayesian model
physical knowledge
Coins
q Beta(FH,FT)
FH,FT
...
Coin 1
Coin 2
Coin 200
q200
q1
q2
d1 d2 d3 d4
d1 d2 d3 d4
d1 d2 d3 d4
59Uses of graphical models
- Understanding existing cognitive models
- e.g., neural network models
- Representation and reasoning
- a way to address holism in induction (c.f. Fodor)
- Defining generative models
- mixture models, language models,
- Modeling human causal reasoning
- more on Friday!
60(No Transcript)