Title: Embracing the Zone: Learning How to Blog with our Students
1Lextutor for Kids Profiling the Vocabulary of
K-2 Learners Hetty Roessingh, PhD hroessin_at_ucalga
ry.ca Tom Cobb, PhD cobb.tom_at_uqam.ca www.lextut
or.ca/vp/kids
2What is the Lextutor?
- A tool for profiling the linguistic output of
children whether narrative or written discourse. - Childrens linguistic output does not follow the
same pattern as adults. - There is a need for a tool to profile the
language of pre-literate youngsters aged 5 7
given the rapid increase of generation 1.5.
3Questions
- What are the high frequency words that children
aged 4 7 typically use that serve as the
building blocks, or the core, for their emergent
literacy development? - Beyond this core of spoken vocabulary, what kinds
of vocabulary do children have and need that
reflects their growing communicative abilities
and their cognitive development as they begin to
engage with the curriculum demands of school? - What sampling strategies are most efficacious for
assessing the vocabulary levels of preliterate
children?
4What We Know
- Children use the first 250 words to communicate
around 75 80 in their every day talk. - Children of aged 7 should know about 2500 head
words.
5What We Want to Know
- We want to know how to sample and profile a
childs output to tell us whether they are over
dependent on the first 250 words and whether the
remaining words reflect a good distribution of
the words in the increasingly higher vocabulary
bands (10 bands of 250 words each). - In other words, linguistic diversity includes
- The number of total words
- The number of different words
- The distribution of words across the word bands
- We want to develop profiles of good ESL
acquisition compared to native speakers.
6Developing the Tool
- We assembled various word lists of childrens
linguistic output. - Criteria for selection of the lists included
- List is based on primary, longitudinal research.
- List consists of words identified at ages 4, 5,
6, and 7, providing a developmental perspective
on language acquisition. - List includes words that children might recognize
and use in their initial literacy development
(beginning reading and writing efforts). - List of words was generated in various contexts
of language use at play, at home and at school. - The method for generating the language was varied
and included open ended, spontaneous childrens
talk, as well as directed / scaffolded activity
that would promote the childs best effort at
marshaling vocabulary available in the service of
naming, describing, explaining, hypothesizing
about his/her unfolding understanding of the
world. - List included a large sample size.
- Time spent in the field gathering the language
samples was sufficient for the purposes of
generating the list. - The words were collated and leveled into 10
groups of 250 words each. - Each word represented a word family.
7The Lists
Name of list Comments
Word Express, The first 2500 words of spoken English. Stemach Williams (1988, 2005) Composite list of 2500 words using Murphy (1957), Johnson (1971), Hopkins (1979), and Moe, Hopkins Rush (1982).
Murphy (1957). The spontaneous spkg vocab of chdrn in primary grades. Journal of Education, Boston U., 140(2) Used and cited by many others as a foundational study.
Hopkins (1979). Oral vocabulary of children in grade 1. Identifies the high frequency words in the spontaneous oral vocab of 125 1rst gr. children. 250 words account for 78 of the lang. produced.
Moe, Hopkins, Rush (1982). Vocabulary of first grade children. Extends the Hopkins list (above) to include 6412 words listed alphabetically and by frequency.
Reich Reich (1977). The vocabulary of primary school children. ED 147844 Canadian study. Compared about 13 different word lists (including Murphy, 1957) to help derive a new list of their own, taken from the spontaneous vocab of 457 children, gr. 1, 2, 3. 67 function words and 137 content words
Johnson (1971). Basic vocab for beginning reading. The Elementary School Journal, October, 1971, 29-34. 306 words taken from Kucera Francis (1967) and Murphy (1957). A blend of reading vocab taken from adult materials, and oral vocab of children k - 1
Fried More (1992). An initial vocab for nonspkg. preschool childn. AAC These 3 studies were driven by the need to identify a core vocab for children who cannot speak. Small scale, but useful studies conducted at home and at school with speaking children. Check their website as well! http//aac.unl.edu/VLNONAACCU2.html
Marvin, Beukelman (1994). Vocab use patterns. AAC, 10 These 3 studies were driven by the need to identify a core vocab for children who cannot speak. Small scale, but useful studies conducted at home and at school with speaking children. Check their website as well! http//aac.unl.edu/VLNONAACCU2.html
Beukelman (1989). Frequency of wd. Usage by nondisabled peers. AAC These 3 studies were driven by the need to identify a core vocab for children who cannot speak. Small scale, but useful studies conducted at home and at school with speaking children. Check their website as well! http//aac.unl.edu/VLNONAACCU2.html
Dolch, 1941. Available online. http//gemini.es.brevard.k12.fl.us/sheppard/reading/dolch.html 220 words based on childrens books. Perhaps the most well known list, even today. Still well respected and used extensively.
Edwards Gibbon (1964). Words your children use. British study that looked at samples of childrens writing at age 5, 6 7.
Harris Jacobson (1982). Basic reading vocabularies. Analysed 8 graded reading series and tabulated all the words by level/gr.
8The Tool
9Sampling Strategy
- The task design used to elicit the language
sample took into account the following
considerations - 1) developmentally appropriate and cognitively
challenging - 2) authentic
- 3) constructivist and engaging
- 4) language focus factors such as memory
capacity of youngsters reduced or eliminated - 5) culturally accessible to a diverse group of
children - 6) high interest and motivational value.
10A boy, a dog, and a frog (Mercer Mayer, 1967)
- The wordless story book A boy, a dog and a frog
(Mayer, 1967) was chosen as a prompt and a
scaffold for eliciting spontaneous talk. The
book consists of 29 black and white sketches of a
young boy with his dog seeking to capture a frog.
A series of humorous incidents leaves the boy
and his dog returning home empty handed. The
frog eventually follows their footprints home and
joins his new found friends in the bath tub.
11Pages from the book
12Sample of Output (Native Speaker)
- He tries to catch a frog. He sees a pond. Then,
he sees a frog and he wants to catch it. And
then the boy wants to catch the frog but he trips
and he ends up in the water. Plop! So does his
doggie. And the little boy says, rrrrrrr!!!!!
and the frog says, ribbit. And then the boy
tries to catch the frog. Ribbit, ribbit. And
then he sees the frog and the frog goes, ribbit,
ribbit, ribbit. Then the boy is going to catch
him again. And the dog is swimming away. Woof,
woof, woof. He doesnt see the frog. Hes going
to catch him in his net. And he ends up catching
the dog, and the frog goes over board. And the
frog quietly creeps away. Hes angry. And the
boy wants to get that frog. But he cant. He
thinks hes going to hop on the lily pad. So he
gives up. He is leaving the frog. He follows the
trail. The trail goes to the house. He finds
the boy in the bath tub with the dog at the end
of the hallway, and hes so happy to see the
frog. The end. And from now on hell take care
of him.
13Profile (Native Speaker)
14Sample of Output (ESL)
- Taking a walk. Hes going down. Hes running and
hes going to fall. He fell in. His dog fell in.
A bucket. The frog jumped. Hes going with the
dog. The frog is in the tree. I think he wants
to scare him. The frog is watching the boy. The
boy is watching to the tree. He wants to catch
the frog. Hes going to catch him. He got his
dog! Hes under. He got the dog. He feels like
hes angry. I think hes going to punch him. He
looks sad. Hes really sad. I think hes going to
his house. Well, hes angry. Hes sad.
Footprints! I think hes going to follow the
footprints. Hes going to his house. Up to the
bathroom. Theres the boy. Hes happy. I dont
know. Hes going to jump into the tub. Hes
happy. Hes going to sit on the dog.
15Profile (ESL)
16Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test
- This test efficiently measures a childs
vocabulary level by age and percentile rank. - Each childs score will be correlated to the
vocabulary profile. - For example, for the previous ESL profile (child
was 5 years, 6 months old) the EOWPVT scores are
as follows - Raw 44(ceiling item) 7 (errors) 37
- Percentile rank 5
- Age Equiv. 3-6
- Placing her two years behind her native speaking
counterparts. - We will revisit this child a year from now to
note patterns of (accelerated) vocabulary
development. - We want to know if we can establish a trajectory
for ESL learners that is distinguishable from a
native speaker profile, a learning disabled
profile, and a developmentally delayed profile
and target interventions that will bring her to
the linguistic thresholds that will permit
academic success over time. - That is, will this child be at the 30th
percentile a year from now.
17References
- Bates-MacArthur Communicative Development
Inventory - Bauman, J. http//jbauman.com/index.htm
- Beck, I., McKeowan, M., Kucan, L. (2002).
Bringing words to life Robust vocabulary
instruction. New York, NY The Guilford Press. - Beukelman, D., Jones, R., Rowan, M. (1989).
Frequency of word usage by nondisabled peers in
integrated preschool classrooms. Augmentative and
Alternative Communication, 243-248. - Biemiller, A. (1999). Language and reading
success. Cambridge, MA Brookline Books. - Biemiller, A. Slonim, N. (2001). Estimating
root word vocabulary growth in normative and
advantaged populations Evidence for a common
sequence of vocabulary acquisition. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 93(3), 498-520. Available
online. - Chall, J.S., Jacobs, V.A. Baldwin, L.E. (1990).
The reading crisis Why poor children fall
behind. Cambridge, MA Harvard University Press. - Dale, E. ORourke, J. (1981). The living word
vocabulary. Chicago World Book/Childcraft
International - Fenson et al (2000). Short-form versions of the
MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory.
Applied Psychology, 21, 95-116. - Fried-Oken, M. More, L.. (1992). An initial
vocabulary for nonspeaking preschool children
based on developmental and environmental language
sources. Augmentative and Alternative
Communication, 8, 41-56. - Greenhalgh, K. Strong, C. (2001. Literate
language features in spoken narratives of
children with typical language and children with
language impairments. Language, Speech and
Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 114-125.
Available online. - Hart, B. Risley, T. (2003). The early
catastrophe The 30 million word gap by age 3.
American Educator, Spring, 2003. Retrieved July
8, 2007. - http//www.aft.org/pubs-reports/american_educator/
spring2003/catastrophe.html - Hart, B. Risley, T. (1999) Learning to talk The
social world of children. Toronto, ON Paul H.
Brooks Publishing Co. - Harris, A. Jacobson, M. (1973/74). Some
comparisons between Basic Elementary Reading
Vocabularies and other word lists. Reading
Research Quarterly, 1, 1973/74, 87-109. - Hopkins, C. (1979). The spontaneous oral
vocabulary of children in grade 1. The Elementary
School Journal, 79 (4), 240 249. - Johnson, D. (1971). A basic vocabulary for
beginning reading. The Elementary School Journal,
October, 29 34. - Kucera, H. Francis, W. (1967). Computational
analysis of present-day American English.
Providence, Rhode Island Brown University Press.
- Marzano, R. (2004). Building background knowledge
for academic achievement. Alexandria, VA
Association for Supervision and Curriculum
Development.