Title: Where actions meet words:
1Where actions meet words
- The paradox of early verb learning
Kathy Hirsh-Pasek Roberta Golinkoff Temple
University University of
Delaware
2With support from many students, graduate and
undergraduate, and NSF
- Mandy Maguire
- Beth Hennon
- Shannon Pruden
- Meredith Meyer
- Carolyn Fenter
- Jennifer Sootsman
- Rachel Pulverman
- Sara Salkind
- Khara Pence
- Dede Addy
- Natalie Hansell
3Beginning at the beginning
- Language- whats the big deal?
- Language can
- start wars
- ruin marriages
- allow a colloquium presentation
4Language allows us to label objects..
5Language is about relations
- The power of language is not in learning the word
cabbage and the word Jim but in learning how
to express relations between these words. - Jim ate the cabbage
- The cabbage attacked Jim
- Jim, dont sit the babies in the cabbage!
-
6(No Transcript)
7And relations are encoded in(among other things)
8Verbs form the architectural centerpiece of the
sentence.
- You just cant learn language without learning
verbs!
9In this talk
- We begin to explore the new frontiers of
- verb acquisition by studying how children
- learn their first action words.
- We will thus use the term verb loosely to
- refer to action words.
- With this caveat in mind.
10We offer a talk in 4 parts
- The ParadoxVerbs are HARD to learn
- But children have them in their earliest
vocabularies - Theories of verb learning
- Building verbs A developmental account
- Explaining the paradox A beginning
11Part 1 Verbs are hard to learn
12Act I Demonstration through a typical motion
event
13- What did you see?
- How would you describe it?
14- What nouns did you use?
- Sliding board? Child? Apartment building?
- Ground? Grass?
- What verbs did you use?
15You might have used verbs like
approach ascend bend climb descend go
grab hit leave lift pull push run
sit slide stand step straighten swing
tuck
16The verb problem
- A verb encodes only part of what is happening in
a motion event including (from Talmy, 1985) -
- Manner the way an action is carried out
- Path the trajectory of an action with respect
to some reference point -
-
17Cross-Linguistic Differences
- Languages differ in terms of the relative
frequencies of different types of verbs - Path and Motion
- e.g., Spanish, Turkish, Greek
- La mujer salió de la casa (corriendo)
- The woman exited the house (running)
- Manner and Motion
- e.g., English, Indonesian, Chinese
- The woman ran out of the house
18Sliding Event
approach ascend bend climb descend go
grab hit leave lift pull push run
sit slide stand step straighten swing
tuck
MANNER
PATH
19Gentner (1992, 2001,2003) suggests verbs are
harder to learn than nouns because
- Verbs more polysemous than nouns
- e.g., run - 53 entries! ball - 2 entries
- Label relations as compared to perceptual
- similarity or function
- Harder to individuate actions than objects and to
- to form categories of actions than objects
- ( What is the invariant in running when
performed by - Carl Lewis or your grandmother?)
- Ephemeral events not concrete
- e.g., running vs. cup
20Act II Verbs are really hard
- A demonstration from Japanese and English
21The rationale
- Some have argued that a noun bias is a product
of learning English. In Asian, verb final
languages, children have a higher proportion of
verbs in their early vocabularies relative to
nouns. Thus, verbs might be as easy to learn as
nouns in these languages. - (Tardiff 1996, Gopnik Choi)
22Standard Scene
- ??! X????
- Look ! (She is) X-ing a)
- a) X-ing is a novel verb.
23Two Test Scenes
same object, different action
same action, different object
- X??????????
- In which (movie) is (she) X-ing?
24The facts
- Participants
- 41, 3-year-olds (M36)
- 40, 5-year-olds (M5.0)
- Task Pointing to one of two scenes on video
25 Japanese Results
26A replication in English(Meyer,
Hirsh-Pasek,Golinkoff, Imai, Haryu)
- Same tapes used in Japanese
- Same ages 3 (N55) and 5 yrs. (N59)
- 3 language conditions
- - Noun (Find the blick!)
- - Bare verb (Blicking! Wheres blicking?)
- - Rich syntax Agent/Obj/Verb (Where is she
blicking? Look at her. She is blicking
it.)
27English Results
28Act III Verbs are really, really hard
- So we simplified the design.
- Asked children to learn and extend only one novel
action, no novel object present. - And they still couldnt do it by age 3 years
29THE BOTTOM LINE?
We got a headache!
PARADOX
30THE PARADOX
- Verbs are really, really, really hard to learn
- BUT
- They appear in childrens earliest vocabularies
- Choi Bowerman, 1991 Choi, 1998
- Brown Bowerman, deLeon Choi, 1995
- Fenson et al., 1994 Tardif, 1996, 1999
31Part IIAddressing the paradox
- Three theories of verb learning
32Three theories
- The Universal Concepts theories
- The Language-specific theories
- The Hybrid theories
33Universal Concepts Theories
Universal concepts
Language
- PATH
- MANNER
- CONTAINMENT
- CAUSALITY
maps onto concepts
The central problem is how do children, from an
initially equivalent base, end up controlling
often very differently structured languages.
Bowerman Levinson (2001)
34Evidence for Universal Concepts Theories?
- Languages around the world draw on the same set
of concepts - (Talmy, Langacker, etc.)
- Perceptually salient (concrete, individuated)
information will - be coded first.
- Developmental data Bowerman (1974) He falled
it. and - Clark (2001) (y for inherent properties (he is
short) and ed for - temporary (he is tired).
-
35Language-Specific Theories
Language
Concepts
- PATH
- MANNER
- CONTAINMENT
- CAUSALITY
36Evidence for language-specific theories?
- Words are invitations to form categories (Brown,
Balaban - Waxman Maguire, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff ).
- The terms, pour vs spill invite listeners to
find distinctions - between these concepts.
- Verbs learned one at a time, then generalized
via - common syntax (Tomasellos Verb Island
Hypothesis). - Choi Bowerman, 1991 Akhtar Tomasello, 1997)
Schlyter, (1990 on - bilingual development in French and German)
- GO used with separate senses (Theakston et
al.,2002)
37Hybrid theories
Universal concepts
Language input
Together determine
Verb meaning
38Hybrid Theories
- Natural partitions hypothesis (Gentner
Boroditskys, 2001 Gentner, 2003) Abstract
universal concepts that are easily individuated
across multiple instances. - Slobin (2001) Both conceptual primitives and
language input work jointly in the childs
construction of verb meaning. - Gleitman et al, 1991, Fisher et al., 2002
Naigles Syntax of language critical to zooming
in on verb meaning. - Hirsh-Pasek Golinkoff (forthcoming),
Emergentist Coalition Model children start with
universal perceptual/conceptual foundation using
syntactic and social cues to prune
language-specific verb meaning.
39The Emergent Coalition Model
Linguistic and social cues sculpt universal
concepts in ways consistent with the native
language
Linguistic
Universal perceptual/conceptual
Social
2nd and 3rd year of life
40Predictions
- Infants should be able to discriminate and
categorize universal concepts (e.g., path,
manner) - When action meets words, children should assume
that the word labels the most perceptually
salient universal relational concept (e.g., path
over manner) - Embedding the verb in rich syntax, allows
children to map the verb to the action in
language-specific ways - Attuned to speaker social intent, children should
map a verb to an action in language-specific ways
(in progress)
41To investigate this we need
- To find universally available concepts used
differently across languages - Enter PATH and MANNER
- To find methodologies that can assess verb
comprehension in young children of different ages - Enter Habituation, Preferential Looking (IPLP),
Preferential Pointing Paradigms (PPP)
42Part 3 Building verbs A developmental account
43 What does it take to learn a verb? A
3-pronged approach
44- Nonlinguistic conceptions of actions in events
(finding action processing actions in ways
relevant to language forming categories of
actions.) - 2. What happens when action meets word?
- What does it take for a baby to learn a verb?
What factors influence early verb learning? - How is childrens verb learning influenced by the
syntax of the target language and by an
understanding of speaker social intent?
45Nonlinguistic conceptions of action
46Study Can infants discriminate proposed
universal concepts in nonlinguistic events?
Method Habituation Participants 18, 7
month-olds 40, 14-17 months Vocabulary for
older children Half above mean half below (on
MacArthur) Question Can infants
dishabituate to new events that change the MANNER
and/or the PATH of an event?
(Pulverman, Sootsman, Golinkoff Hirsh-Pasek,
2002)
47Enter Starry
48The Habituation study..(Pulverman)
49Stimuli
9 computer-animated motion events
3 Manners flapping spinning bending
3 Paths over under past
NO LANGUAGE ACCOMPANIED THESE EVENTS
50Procedure and design
- Habituated to one of 9 stimulus events
- Trials ended after 2-second look away or 30
seconds, whichever came first - Within subjects design
- IV test conditions DV looking time
51Test Events, an eg.
- Control Event (Habituated to )
- Flapping Over
- Path Change Event
- Flapping Under
- Manner Change Event
- Spinning Over
- Both Change Event
- Bending Past
- (order counterbalanced across children)
52Drumroll PleaseMajor Findings
- 7 14-mo olds discriminate universal action
components (MANNER and PATH) in ongoing events.
2) HIGH VOCABULARY CHILDREN, pay more
attention to MANNER changes than PATH changes --
consistent with prominence of MANNER in English.
-Do all high vocabulary children pay attention to
MANNER? OR do children learning Spanish attend
to PATH? (Pulverman et al.).
53(No Transcript)
54What happens when actions meet words?
55Predictions
- Children should assume that the word labels the
most perceptually salient universal relational
concept (e.g., PATH over MANNER) - A common label should focus attention on
language-specific components in this case to
MANNER (Gentner) - Children who already know some relational terms
should be able to use syntactic support to learn
novel verbs (Jones Smith Slobin)
56Three experiments
- Experiment 1 Do 2-year-olds assume a novel verb
label refers to the PATH of the action? - Experiment 2 Can 2-year-olds use multiple
exemplars of events to guide verb learning? - Experiment 3 Can 2-year-olds use multiple
exemplars and syntactic cues to guide verb
learning?
57Study Maguire (2003)Will infants attach a word
to the most salient, individuated aspect of an
event?
Method Preferential Looking to one of two video
events Participants Age - 16 children at each
of 2 ages (2 2.5) High (
95) and average relational vocabulary
(prepositions, verbs, on MacArthur,) 7
high relation, 18 average Question Wi
ll children take a novel verb as a label for
the PATH or MANNER of an event? Path
most salient but English tends to label MANNER
not PATH
58Intermodal Preferential Looking Paradigm
59Design
- Introduction Introduce Starry
- Salience trials Test salience of test trials
- Training Teach a novel action label
- Test trials Does the child take the verb to mean
the PATH or the MANNER of the event?
60Design
Introduction
Meet Starry. Starry is fun!
Salience
Look Starry is blicking! Watch Starry blicking!
Training
Spin over
Look Starry is blicking! Watch Starry blicking!
61Design cont. TEST TRIALS
Test trial 1
Bend over spin past
Where is Starry blicking?
Test trial 2
Where is Starry blicking?
Test trial 3
Mutual exclusivity
Where is Starry hirshing?
Test trial 4
Recovery
Where is Starry blicking?
62Results
- No age differences
- Children with more relational words looked
significantly longer to the PATH even though
English tends to have MANNER verbs - English-speaking 5-year-olds and adults all chose
MANNER as the referent for blicking.
63Study Can we make toddlers approach verb
learning like English speaking adults?
Method Preferential Looking Participants 30
children, 2 ages (2 and 2.5 years)
Question Do multiple instances of same
MANNER (spinning) across different PATHS (around,
under) bias children to assume that a novel verb
labels MANNER?
NOTE the only difference in this study is in the
training video which now shows 4 different PATHS,
one MANNER. The training audio remains, See,
Starry blicking.
64Results Nothing!
- No age effects
- No vocabulary effects
- Significance Seeing multiple instances is not
enough to sway young language learners to a
MANNER bias for verb meaning
65Will anything cause children to interpret a novel
verb as labeling the MANNER of an event?
66Study Adding rich syntax Adding sentences to
last study
Method Preferential Looking Participants 30
children, 2 ages (2 and 2.5 years)
Question Can toddlers use syntax
multiple instances to discern that the verb is
labeling MANNER?
NOTE The only difference in this and the last
study is in the training which now adds syntactic
information, Look, Starry is blicking around
(under, past) the ball!
67Results
- No age effects
- Large vocabulary effects! The higher relational
vocabulary children now assume that the word
labels the MANNER.
68Discussion
- As predicted Children map verbs onto the most
salient universal action concepts (PATH over
MANNER, as suggested in the perception studies) - What helps English-speaking children move from a
reliance on PATH to a language-specific reliance
on MANNER? Syntax and/or multiple instances. - For children with higher relational vocabularies
sentences that block the PATH interpretation,
yield adult-like performance.
69Part 4 Given the data, how do we begin to
explain the paradox?Verbs ARE hard to learn(at
least in the lab), but children have them in
their earliest vocabularies
- Musings that need your input!
70Imagine that verb learning occurs on a
developmental continuum? (Gentner, 2001)
- Perceptually based
- Specific context
- Social intent is clear
- Rich language input
- Extension limited
Contextually impoverished Reduced language
input Social intent ambiguous Extension even
metaphorical
18 mo.
4 yrs.
71Evidence suggests that Early Verb Learning
- is context bound and used in situations where all
the cues for verb usage (perceptual,linguistic,
and social) overlap - -Behrend, Forbes Farrar studies - Young verb
learners - very conservative in their extensions
- -Tomasello verb island hypothesis One verb at
a time. - This is exemplified in Chinese early verb use.
- Tardiff Children begin with more focused narrow
verbs, - associated with particular and consistent
routines or social contexts - (e.g., hug, kiss )
72LATER Verb Learning is of two forms.
- 2.5 - 3 years - children can learn novel verbs
and extend them with syntactic (or perhaps
social) support (Maguire, 2003 Fischer, Naigles)
BUT only in limited, related contexts (e.g, , you
can substitute the agent but not the instrument) - 5-7 years - rapid extension to new situations
is observed, the meaning of the verb is lifted
from its originally learned context and become
truly relational.
73Why the paradox?
- While toddlers look like they have some verbs
early, when pressed in laboratory research, their
limitations become clear. - Only older, late preschool children can represent
the abstract relations between language and
events. These children are no longer bound to
context. E.g., Hammering can even be done
without a hammer.
74This raises the question Why cant the youngest
children learn verbs that are context free? The
learning problem is not with
- discriminating or categorizing relations in
non-linguistic events (studies by Pulverman et
al., Pruden et al.). - forming mappings between language and aspects of
the real world -- lots of work with nouns (e.g.,
Hollich et al.) - or even with recognizing which element in the
sentence is the verb (Golinkoff et al.,
sensitivity to /ing/ 16-18 months)
75Rather, the problem might be that verb learning
requires the ability to abstract relations across
multiple domains.
As Gentner (2003) argued As similarity
comparisons evolve from being perceptual and
context bound to becoming increasingly sensitive
to common relational structure, children show an
increasing capacity to reason at the level of
abstract commonalities and rules. (p. 201)
76Verb learning might not be just about verbs, it
might be about the ability to reason about
relationships.
77Indeed, there are suggestive parallels in the
mastery of verb learning and in
- Number development
- Huttenlocher
- The development of analogy
- (Ratterman Gentner, 1998, 2002 Lowenstein
Gentner) - Even in relational noun development
- Island (Keil)
- Passenger (Hall Waxman)
78We dont want to stretch this too far. Yet, we do
want to leave you with some interesting final
thoughts
- Adult-like verb learning is really, really,
really, hard for children. - And it is hard even though, children seem to have
the prerequisites that should enable them to use
verbs productively - We believe that part of the problem is that to
learn a verb, children have to coordinate
information about relations across contexts and
domains ( language, social perceptual) - And that this coordination across relations might
prove a stumbling block in language learning and
in other aspects of cognition
79Perhaps Piaget got the picture when he suggested
that young children had difficulty dealing with
relations prior to age 5.